THE UNIVERSITY 0F ILLINOIS LIBRARY 370 106 no.i4H7 -| wtrC3,tW!l A The person charging this material is re- sponsible for its return to the library from which it was withdrawn on or before the Latest Date stamped below. Theft, mutilation, and underlining of books are reasons for disciplinary action and may result in dismissal from the University. To renew call Telephone Center, 333-8400 UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS LIBRARY AT URBANA-CHAMPAIGN OCT 18 SEP 27 A 1982 1962 L161— O-1096 BULLETIN NO. 14 BUREAU OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH COLLEGE OF EDUCATION THE USE OF DIFFERENT TYPES OF THOUGHT QUESTIONS IN SECONDARY SCHOOLS AND THEIR RELATIVE DIFFICULTY FOR STUDENTS by Walter S. Monroe, Director, Bureau of Educational Research and Ralph E. Carter Associate Professor of Educational Psychology Indiana University, Extension Division PRICE 30 CENTS PUBLISHED BY THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS, URBANA 1923 . v — -Z^ £? i /v-/y PREFACE This bulletin is a report of one phase of a larger in- vestigation relating to the study habits of school children. The types of questions asked by teachers of their students are important both because of the mental processes which occur in answering them and because an intimate relation exists between the questions asked and the detailed objectives toward which the students work. Mr. Carter was consulted in the preparation of the questionnaire. He rendered valuable assistance in deciding upon the tentative list of types of thought questions. Later, advantage was taken of his presence at the University of Illi- nois during the Summer Session of 1922 to secure assistance in the preparation of this report. He is largely responsible for its general organization. Walter S. Monroe, Director. February 1, 1923 539709 Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2011 with funding from University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign http://www.archive.org/details/useofdifferentty14monr The Use of Different Types of Thought Questions in Secondary Schools and Their Relative Difficulty for Students Recent emphasis upon questioning in instruction. The first quantitative study of the questions which teachers use in the class- room was made by Miss Romiett Stevens. 1 In this analysis of questions a distinction was made between those that required memory only and those that required thought. Miss Stevens' discussion has been widely quoted and has been very influential in causing teachers to give more attention to the types of questions asked of their students. It is altogether likely that this investigation is primarily responsible for a material increase in the percent of thought questions which are asked in our schools today. Many of the most commonly used texts on methods of teaching devote separate space to the topic of question- ing. Its importance has been especially emphasized by Charters, 2 Strayer, 3 Parker, 4 and Colvin. 5 Analysis of thought questions. An examination of typical thought questions from the standpoint of the mental processes which they require in answering reveals certain significant differences. Some questions require the student to "compare" two or more ideas; others ask that he "summarize;" still others demand that he "give reasons why." The probable mental processes occurring in the reflective thinking called for rather than the form of the question or the word- ing of the answer have been made the basis of these types of thought 1 Stevens, Romiett. "The question as a measure of efficiency in instruction," Teachers College Contributions to Education No. 48. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1912. 2 Charters, W W. Methods of Teaching. Chicago: Rowe Peterson and Company, 1912, p. 444. (Chapter XVII). •■•Strayer, G. D. A Brief Course in the Teaching Process. New York: Macmillan Company, 1912, p. 315. (Chapter XI) 4 Parker, S. C. Methods of Teaching in High Schools. Boston: Ginn and Company, 1915, p. XXV, 529. (Chapter XX) 6 Colvin, S. S. Introduction to. High School Teaching. New York: Macmillan Company, 1917, p. XXI, 451. (Chapter XV) [S] questions. Altho there is doubtless considerable overlapping in the types of mental processes which usually occur in answering such questions, there appears to be sufficient differences to justify the re- cognition of a number of types of questions. In this investigation the following tentative list of types of thought questions was used. A more refined analysis would yield probably some additional types, but, on the other hand, for practical purposes it is possible that some combinations of types might profitably be made. It is recognized that the mental processes of different students doubtless vary in answering the same question. In fact a thought question for one stu- dent may be a memory question for another student, or even for the same student on the following day. 1. Selective recall— basis given. Name the presidents of the United States who had been in military life before their election. What do New Zealand and Australia sell in Europe that may interfere with our market? 2. Evaluating recall — basis given. Which do you consider the three most important American inventions in the nineteenth century from the standpoint of expansion and growth of transportation? Name the three statesmen who have had the greatest influence on economic legislation in the United States. 3. Comparison of two things — on a single designated basis. Compare Eliot and Thackeray in ability in character delineation. Compare the armies of the North and South in the Civil War as to leadership. 4. Comparison of two things — in general. Compare the early settlers of the Massachusetts Colony with those of the Virginia Colony. Contrast the life of Silas Marner in Raveloe with his life in Lantern Yard. 5. Decision — for or against. Whom do you admire more, Washington or Lincoln? In which in your opinion can you do better, oral or written examinations? 6. Causes or effects. Why has the Senate become a much more powerful body than the House of Representatives? What caused Silas Marner to change from what he was in Lantern Yard to what he was in Raveloe? 7. Explanation of the use or exact meaning of some phrase or state- ment in a passage. 8. Summary of some unit of the text or of some article read. [6] 9. Analysis. (The word itself is seldom involved in the question.) What characteristics of Silas Marner make you understand wrwfRavflgejDeople were suspicious of him? ' " R i Mention several qualities of leadership. 10. Statement of relationships. Why is a knowledge of Botany helpful in studying agriculture? Tell the relation of exercise to good health. 11. Illustrations or examples (your own) of principles in science, con- struction in language, etc. "12. Classification. (UsuallytheconverseofNo.il). What is the principle involved here? What is the construction? To what class or genus does this individual belong? 13. Application of rules or principles in new situations. 14. Discussion. Discuss the Monroe Doctrine. Discuss early American Literature. 15. Statement of aim — author's purpose in his selection or organiza- tion of material. What was the purpose of introducing this incident? Why did he discuss this before that? 16. Criticism — as to the adequacy, correctness, or relevancy of a printed statement, or a classmate's answer to a question on the lesson. 17. Outline. 18. Reorganization of facts. (A good type of review question to give training in organization.) The student is asked for reports where facts from different organizations are arranged on an entirely new basis. 19. Formulation of new questions — Problems and questions raised. What question came to your mind? What else must be known in order to understand the matter under consideration? 20. New methods of procedure. Suggest a plan for proving the truth or falsity of some hypothesis. How would you change the plot in order to produce a certain different effect? Relation to educational objectives of types of questions asked by teachers. Incidentally it may be noted that the types of questions used both for stimulating and directing the mental activity of the learner and for measuring the results of teaching reflect in a subtle way the educational objectives of a teacher. The objectives thus indicated may not agree with those stated by the teacher but they nevertheless are an index of the objectives toward which the students [7] direct their efforts. In studying, students tend to prepare to answer the kind of questions which they think the teacher will ask. It is altogether likely that the study objectives of students are influenced more by the kind of questions asked than by direct statements of aims in the course. Purpose and method of this investigation. The purpose of this study, which is Sub-project II of our investigation of the study habits of high school pupils, was announced as follows: "To deter- mine the extent of the use of different types of thought questions in actual school practise and the relative difficulty of these types for students." The method employed was to submit a questionnaire to a large number of teachers in the seventh and eighth grades and in the high school. In this questionnaire the list of the twenty types of thought questions given on pages 6-7 was reproduced. The teachers were urged to study these types carefully before answering any of the questions. It was pointed out that an attempt to classify a question according to this plan might lead to the conclusion that it is a combina- tion of two or three types. However, in such a case, it is probably true that part of the work of answering has been done by the author in the textbook, leaving to the student only that phase of the question which would definitely classify it as belonging to one of the above types. Plan of summarizing data. Usable replies were received from 199 teachers representing almost every school subject and all parts of the state. A few of these replies did not give answers to one or more of the questions but they were included in the tabulation for the other questions. By subject the number of replies were as follows: English, 41; History, 48; Science, 41; Mathematics, 31; Foreign Language, 12; Geography, 11; Commercial subjects, 6; Agriculture, 4. Taking only the replies from teachers of English, History, and Science there were 26 for grades VII and VIII combined; 34 for grades IX and X; and 45 for grades XI and XII. It was thought advisable to summarize separately the replies for English, History and Science. Those re- ceived from teachers of all other subjects were grouped together. The replies to Questions I and IV obviously do not lend themselves to statistical treatment. The data yielded by the other three ques- tions are summarized in the following tables. Limitations of replies to the questionnaire. Several teachers took occasion to mention difficulties which they experienced in filling out the questionnaire blank. That some difficulties were encountered [8] is not surprising because the concept of different types of thought questions was undoubtedly new to most, if not all, of the teachers. The limitations of space prevented an extensive description of each type. Undoubtedly teachers differed in their interpretations of the various types. Furthermore, in answering Question II they were asked not only to differentiate between thought questions and mem- ory questions but also to classify the thought questions under the various types. In doing this, they had to rely upon their memory of the questions they had asked. For these reasons the replies to Ques- tion II must be thought of as giving only a very rough indication of the practise of teachers with reference to the types of questions which they asked. A few teachers reported that their replies were based on a careful analysis of lists of questions which have been used and pre- served. If these lists were reasonably complete their replies should be more accurate than those received from teachers who relied upon their memory of the questions they had asked. It may be pointed out that other methods of ascertaining the relative frequency of use of different types of thought questions would not eliminate all difficulties. For example, if a trained investigator should visit the classes of 199 teachers in various parts of the state a sufficient number of times to obtain a fair sample of the habitual practise and should try to make a .record of all types of questions asked he would encounter difficulties in definitely classifying them. It is not possible to judge accurately of the mental processes stimu- lated by a question unless one is acquainted with the previous ex- periences of the pupils in the field of this question. This has been referred to already in pointing out that what constitutes a thought question for one pupil may be merely a memory question for another. The analysis of stenographic reports of lessons would present certain difficulties for the same reason. The teacher knows better than any one else what acquaintance a student has with a topic and what the textbook states about it. For this reason he has the advantage of a casual observer. Additional types of questions mentioned. Question I was asked in order to obtain suggestions for supplementing the list of type questions for further study in this field and also to give each teacher an opportunity to report fully his own practise in the use of different types of thought questions in case he did not consider the list ade- quate. The replies to the questionnaire show that the number of teachers who did not report the use of all of the types given in the list [9] UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS BUREAU OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH COLLEGE OF EDUCATION Types of Thought Questions Name Address School Subject School grade I. Write in on the blanks at the bottom of the list below any additional types of thought questions you use to any great extent in the grade and subject you have chosen. (Include these additional types in answering all other questions.) II. What percent of all of your questions to the class are of each type listed? (See directions, p. 11). Put the percent of each type in the column on the right. Types of Thought Questions Percent of all questions 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24. Selective recall — basis given Evaluating recall — basis given Comparison of two things — on a single designated basis. Comparison of two things — in general Decision — for or against Causes or effects Explanation Summary Analysis Statement of relationships , Illustration or examples Classification Application Discussion Statement of aim Criticism Outline Reorganization of facts Formulation of new questions New methods of procedure III. In the left hand margin make an "X" opposite each of the five types for which students answers are least likely to be satisfactory. (See directions, p. 1 1) IV. Choose three of the types you marked with an "X." State for each some of the students' most common faults in procedure in answering it. Mention faults of [10] omisson as well as faults of commission. Mention only faults peculiar to this type of question. Use other side of this sheet if necessary. (See directions below.) V. If we consider all unsatisfactory answers made by students in school work, what percent of them, in your opinion, are due primarily to bad habits of procedure in answering questions? Give separate replies for memory questions % and thought questions % The following detailed directions for answering the questionnaire were given: Before answering any of the questions, decide what subject and what grade (any from VII to XII) you will have in mind in all of your replies. Answer the questions with reference to only one school subject. Fill in the blanks at the top of the first page of the questionnaire. Question I. Examine the list of types given above to see if there are not some other types of thought questions that you use to a considerable extent. Altho the given list may seem long and inclusive at first, it is not complete. Question II. Before you put down your estimates for Question II, you should decide what is your proportion of thought questions and of pure memory questions. This question calls for a further analysis of your thought questions. It may be advis- able to make rough estimates for all before you fill in the data on the questionnaire. After you have the differences between the types in mind, make the best estimate you can, even tho you may not be very sure in many cases. You are merely declaring your best judgment, not guaranteeing that it is infallible. We want your opinion of what your practise is, not oj what you think it ought to be. Question HI. In Question III, we are concerned with the process of answering so we must assume that the student has the information necessary for a satisfactory answer if he will only use it as the type of the question requires. The common starting point for an informal diagnosis of a student's study faults is in his unsatisfactory answers. An unsatisfactory answer may indicate lack of information or certain faulty habits in answering questions. There are many cases in which teachers find out by other means that the student has enough information to give a satisfactory answer but has failed because he did not take the necessary steps and precautions in replying to the question. When some students correct examination papers returned to them, they are heard to say, "I knew that, but I didn't think of it," or "I knew that but I didn't know that was what the question meant." Some of the faults in procedure are common to all kinds of questions; others are peculiar to particular types. In Question III, you are asked to decide on the difficulty of the types for students. In other words, in which type do they have the worst habits of procedure? Question IV. It will be helpful in answering Question IV to think of some par- ticular questions of the type under consideration and then express the students' faults in fairly general terms. Question V. As long as the unsatisfactory answer is a resultant of poor methods of preparation and poor habits of answering questions, we must correct the latter or take it into consideration in inferring what methods of study are needed. It is important to get an estimate from a large number of teachers of the percent of failures (in answers) that are due primarily to bad habits of procedure in answering questions. [11] is very much greater than the number mentioning additional types Only thirty-nine teachers (19.6 percent) indicated that they used all twenty types of questions. Thirteen teachers supplemented the list, each writing in from one to three additional types. These teachers were distributed among the different subjects as follows: seven in History, three in English, two in Science, and one in Mathematics. Only four of those suggesting additional types of questions indicated that they used all twenty of the types given in the questionnaire list. Some of the additional types of questions suggested are clearly included in the list given in the questionnaire. The following are typical: (A) "Connections between historical events," Type 4 or 6; (B) "Cause and results," included in Type 6; (C) "Application to the present time," included in Type 13; (D) "Determining the significant word in a statement," included in Type 19. Other suggested types are not so clearly included but several of them under a fairly loose inter- pretation may be placed with the types listed. Illustrations of these are: (A) "Imagining the results if conditions were different," a special case of Type 6; (B) "Questions on appreciation such as, which do you like best?" a fairly common form of Type 2; (C) "Identify a ' known character in a scene in the story when his name is not mention- ed by the author," a special case of Type 12; (D) "What should you judge from these facts?" This last is broad enough to cover many types. Frequently, it would fall under Type 6. There were a few suggestive questions which are not so easily classified under the 20 types given. Some of these are: (A) "Trace the development," mentioned by two teachers; (B) "Estimate the importance of"; . and (C) "Why is this statement true?" a very common question in geometry. By a very liberal interpretation these might be put under Types 9, 2, and 6 respectively but it is likely that many teachers would consider them sufficiently different to justify naming them as additional types. The answers to the first question indicate that the list of types is reasonably complete. For practical purposes it is probably better to give a rather loose interpretation in classifying special cases than to try to extend the list. Largely for this reason the authors decided to limit the summary of the replies to the other parts of the ques- tionnaire to the original twenty types. Frequency of use of different types of questions. Question II of the questionnaire was asked in order to secure answers for the following: [12] *^o 1. Which types of questions are most commonly used by teachers in classroom instruction? 2. Which types of questions are most characteristic of the in- struction in different subjects? 3. Do teachers in the more advanced grades tend to ask different types of questions from those in the lower grades? Table I gives a detailed summary of the replies by forty-eight teachers of History. This table shows that four of these teachers did not report any use of questions requiring selective recall (Type 1); one teacher reported that this type of question formed 1 percent of all his questions; five teachers indicated that this type made up 2 percent of all the questions which they asked; eleven named 5 percent as an index of its frequency; eight teachers considered that 15 per- cent of their questions were of this type; and another eight teachers indicated that more than 15 percent of their questions called for selective recall. The median practise is 7.5 percent. The outstanding characteristic of the table is the variability of practise which it indicates. In the case of each type of question there were one or more teachers who gave no indication of its use. For several of the types the number of teachers not indicating any use is surprisingly large. Furthermore, there is no type of question which did not receive an indication of at least 5 percent by one teacher. Most of the types received an indication of 10 percent or more. Thus, if we may assume that the replies to this portion of the questionnaire are a reasonably true indication of practise, it is clear that teachers of History vary widely in the types of thought questions which they ask of their pupils and hence necessarily vary widely in the detailed objectives which their pupils strive to attain. Similar tabulations were made for English, Science and other subjects combined. The median frequencies for the four groups of subjects are given in Table II. If we consider only English, History and Science, students are most commonly required to answer the following types of thought questions: cause and effect, Type 6; selective recall, Type 1; discussion, Type 14; and evaluating recall, Type 2. The types of questions which are asked least frequently are: formulation of new questions, Type 19; new methods of procedure, Type 20; and reorganization of facts, Type 18. Different subjects require different mental processes. An ex- amination of Table II reveals that the frequency of use of some of the [13] OS DC o cr> OS to U < w H oo m Q w en < O M H en W 8 H DC (J D O DC H to O C/3 W CL, >H H e r3 tncOCO w- >sO''fvO lJ "> l '">©©OV©co©'OfNCOOO© T3 r-r^-iou->eN©eN»ococO>-i h o « M «i M — s I* «^n OO CO O CO — ON — O w» 00*0-— i— i tF cN — i i-i VO <-i i-h """' ■* ~- ri *"* CO O] "■" ' CN co — — -i C o CO 3 ^ H c 4-t O \0"ivi^MPinONNH CNCN Ot ?i * « h X 60 3 O J= H 00 « cs * -h n ^^^^Tf — co ~h oi^^h >-*- o c r^- CNCO - N ^h-^ ^hc^^h j-> — • r~'^Oco© K >O v O vy "> — '•>+'\0 v >0eNN0C4O^-m CO r-^ ^^ ^— i 1 . 1 *— i 1 ■* CO ■* CN CO CO ■— iCN'-o , ^i-* , CNCO-HTtiTjo to oo t^ ui rH o *N , o*0'^lou^^to , 'l»0'*'t , <^ | c , ^^o^ , '-^' H ^ rt M Ol N CN — ' m « (N to C o • CO to to .2 a, 2 = CO U £ «s * : aj o CO recall l — sin i— gei CO o £ r C U °C8 O tion o n of n ds... U tive re uating parisoi parisoi « o ° «« - to C « y m £ •J > tionshi tration sificati ication ussion. ;ment ganiza lulatio methc coWUUQuW7) « H Q < Q D H co O co W CL, tH H fa O u Q Z 2 co £* a! O U >" l 3 .- to Q co I £ + + Jm .3 2? d < I W !> j « ._; >-3 >< X CO oo < ^+ 4- + > 60 Ml 60 ti c c c .2 UJ UJ UJ I w '0 u co O < + *J . 60 £ .60 *^ .j* u CO ■* •— i CN CO CO o •z. w CJ co .£•-* -a c CO •* •* CO CO CN *3 _k: 1 s CN CS Tf CO CN ■*■ t- C/X a: o M ffi £••* 3 rt CN -h cN co if co co* « ^i | s CN CO CO — i r-> Tf s a* c/3 O 2: w -£■ ■* ^3 C 3 P3 — CN CO CN ■*■ «*• CO — o z •Si 1) co f^ — i t^- Cs OS 3 C o a ^ - ^_ a; o co — — •> [25] high school juniors and seniors. It would seem from the data that some teachers may not be using the types of questions that would stimulate and test students' achievements in some of the types of study they are expected to use. Conclusions. Probably the most significant conclusions to be drawn from this investigation are: (1) teachers are not sufficiently conscious of the types of questions which they are accustomed to ask and of the significance of these types, and (2) in general teachers do not analyze unsatisfactory answers to questions in order to ascertain whether such answers are due to a faulty technique on the part of the student. A number of other conclusions are worthy of mention. (1) Teachers of the same subject vary widely in the extent of their use of different types of thought questions. (2) The frequency of the use of a given type does not seem to depend very much on (a) the school grade, (b) the subject, or (c) the supposed difficulty of the type. It is very likely that some teachers, who expect their students to use cer- tain types of study, do not use the types of questions that are best suited to test their students' achievements due to those particular types of study. (3) IfTeachers individually think that certain types of questions are more difficult for students than others. However, there is no very great agreement among teachers as to the relative diffi- culty of the various types.jj [26] BULLETIN NO. 15 BUREAU OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH COLLEGE OF EDUCATION THE CONSTANT AND VARIABLE ERRORS OF EDUCATIONAL MEASUREMENTS by Walter S. Monroe Director, Bureau of Educational Research PRICE 25 CENTS PUBLISHED BY THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS, URBANA 1923