LIBRA RY OF THE U N IVERSITY OF ILLINOIS 370 no. 6-\2 The person charging this material is re- sponsible for its return on or before the Latest Date stamped below. Theft, mutilation and underlining of books are reasons for disciplinary action and may result in dismissal from the University. UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS LIBRARY AT URBANA-CHAMPAIGN di:c :$i! 19" L161— O-1096 Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2012 with funding from University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign http://www.archive.org/details/influenceofleade09fiel UGt no .9 GROUP EFFECTIVENESS RESEARCH LABORATORY DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS URBANA. ILLINOIS THE INFLUENCE OF LEADER-KEYMAN RELATIONS ON COMBAT CREW EFFECTIVENESS Fred E. Fiedler Technical Report No. 9 Study performed under Contract N6-ori-07135 with the Office of Naval Research Project on Social Perception and Group Effectiveness June, 1954 GROUP EFFECTIVENESS RESEARCH LABORATORY DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS URBANA, ILLINOIS THE INFLUENCE OF LEADER -KEY MAN RELATIONS ON COMBAT CREW EFFECTIVENESS Fred E. Fiedler Technical Report No. 9 Study performed under Contract N6-ori-07135 with the Office of Naval Research Project on Social Perception and Group Effectiveness June, 1954 THE LIBRARY OF THE SEP 27 1954 UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS . . . I . 7o THE INFLUENCE OF LEADER -KEY MAN RELATIONS ON COMBAT CREW EFFECTIVENESS Fred j^. ii-c.-er University of Illinois Two studies dealing with the prediction of t3am effective- ness are here reported; These studies investigated the relationship between the formal leader's interpersonal perception and his team's operating efficiency. The samples under investigation consisted of B-29 Bomber crews and Army Tank crews. The former served primarily for exploratory purposes while the tank crews were used as a validation sample. The research has been based on the working assumption that relevant interpersonal relations can be inferred from standard sociometric procedures and from interpersonal perception tests measuring Assumed Similarity (aS) which will be described below. Background The research program of which the present studies are a part aims to discover some of the psychological principles underlying group productivity. We shall summarize briefly the earlier work which led to the present investigations. Measurement of Assumed Similarity . We have measured inter- personal perception with the score, " Assumed Similarity between Opposites ," or ASo. This score is obtained when we ask a subject (S) to fill out a personality questionnaire under two different sets: to predict (a) the responses of a person with whom he ran work very well. 2 and (b) the responses of the person with whom he finds it most difficult to cooperate. The difference score D (2) indicates the similarity subject (S) perceives between his most and least pre- ferred work companions. A person who perceives his most and least preferred work companions as similar has high Assumed Simi- larity (ASo); a person who perceives marked differences between them has low ASo. The split-half reliability of ASo scores ranges between .85 and .95 for a 60 item questionnaire (3). Despite numerous attempts, we have not found ASo scores to correlate consistently with traits measured by other personality inventories. Assumed Similarity appears to measure some aspects of emotional distance. A person who perceives others to be similar (has high ASo) seems to desire closer emotional relations than a person who perceives others as dissimilar. Scattered findings and interview data further suggest that the person with high ASo is essentially accep- tant. pliable, and receptive and is inclined to be uncritical of others. A person having low ASo appears to be a more critical, reserved, and analytic individual who rejects those with whom he cannot work (5). The Relations of ASo to Effectiveness of High School Basketball Teams We first utilized ASo in two studies of informal teams (4, 5). A frankly exploratory study investigated 14 high school basketball teams at the beginning of the season. These fama consisted of from 9-18 men each. ASo scores and sociometric preference ratings, by which we could identify the team's "most preferred co-worker/' were obtained from each S. Team effectiveness was defined as the proportion of games the team had won by midseason. As shown in Table 1. we found a correlation of -.63 between AS o ■ ' ■ ' ■ • ■ ■ ■ 3 of the team's most preferred co-worker and the criterion. In other words, teams v»hich chose a low ASo person tended to be more effective than those preferring a high ASo co-worker. Seven good and five poor teams, tested at the end of the season, were used as validation sample. A significant correlation between ASo and the criterion (-.58) corroborated our original find- ing (5). Relation of ASo t o Effectiveness of Student Surveying Parties The results on basketball teams were cross validated in a second study of informal teams. Members of 11 surveying parties participated in this study. These 3 to 4 man teams consisted of civil engineering students taking a required field course in surveying. We again obtained ASo scores and sociometric preference ratings from each S. The criterion consisted of instructor ratings of team accuracy. The hypothesis was supported that ASo of the team's most preferred co-worker (i.e., its informal leader) is negatively corre- lated with team effectiveness (Table 1) (5). The findings were inter- preted to indicate that effective teams chose informal lea'de'rs who perceive many differences between their co-workers^ The present report extends the earlier research to formally organized . teams. Work with Formal Teams ; B-29 Bomber Crews* Hypotheses The team effectiveness of informal teams was correlated with the ASo score of only the team's most preferred co-worker, i.e., its informal leader. For this reason we believed psychological The groups used in this study also provided data for a project sponsored by the Crew Research Laboratory of AFPTRC. We are indebted to Dr. Robert L. French, Director of Research, CRL, and Dr. Thornton B. Roby, Chief, Crew Assembly Section, for their cooperation. distance (as measured by ASo) to be an attitude related to effective leadership. We therefore hypothesized that the aSo score of formal leaders would also be negatively related to crew effectiveness criteria. Table 1 Correlations of Teams' Most prefe^redCc-workers* ASo Score with Criteria of Effectiveness Sample Correlation N P High School Basketball teams tested pre -season High School Basketball teams tested toward end of season Student Surveying Teams -.63 -.58 ** 14 .05 12 .05 -.51 22 .02 * Spearman Rho ** Point bis e rial, significance tested with student's t. (one tailed) *** Pearson r. (one tailed) However, several studies have pointed out that groups may have informal as well as formal leaders (1, 11, 12). In cases where the formal leader is not also the informal leader, his influence over the group is considerably weakened. Hence, a special case of the first hypothesis stated that the ASo score of the accepted leader (i.e., who is informal leader or most preferred crew member) would *>« correlated with effectiveness criteria. Procedure Sample. Seventy B-29 crews, from four different training classes, were available for this study. Each class contained 17 or 18 B-29 crews. Crew Structure . A B-29 crew normally consists of 11 men. Five are officers: The Aircraft Commander (AC), Pilot (P), Navigator (N), Radar (or Video) Observer (VO), and the Bombar- dier (B). Six are enlisted crew members: the Flight Engineer, Radio Operator, and four gunners. Assumed Similarity Tests . ASjo scores were obtained from all crew members who were available for testing. The test con- sisted of 80 items, such as the following: "I am often bored with people", and "It annoys me to leave a task unfinished." The test instructions asked S to predict the responses of the man in the Air Force with whom he had worked best, and the responses of the man with whom he had worked least well. The test was given during two sessions four weeks apart. Reliability for ASo in the 80 item test was obtained by the Guttman Split- half formula and is .86 for one class (N = 178). Sociometric Questionnaires . Sociometric preference ratings were derived from a routinely administered "Position Description Form", which asks Ss to indicate the persons on their crew whom they would definitely prefer, moderately, or least prefer tor five crew activities. These activities are, (a) organizing a crew party, (b) going on leave, (c) going on a dangerous mission, (d) loading special cargo, and (e) returning with the crew from be- hind enemy lines. ; ..' i : ■" • .- 6 Responses to these five questions were highly intercor re- lated; a pooled preference score was therefore computed which indicated the extent to which a crew member chose various others on his crew. Because we were particularly interested in relative sociometric choices, the preference ratings were converted to ranks within the crew. Criterion Scores . Two criteria of bomber crew effectiveness were originally used. Both were developed by the Crew Research Laboratory, Randolph Air Force Base. Radar Bomb Scores - Circular Error Average (RBS) . This measure is an error score indicating how far off the target a par- ticular bomb would theoretically have fallen. The score is computed as the average for 10 missions. According to the Crew Research Laboratory the reliability of RBS is .4 to .6. Control Time Error (CTE) . This is also a theoretical score which indicates the number of minutes by which a plane would be too early or too late at a certain predetermined point of meeting. According to the Crew Research Laboratory, the Navigator (N) accounts for most of the variance of this score. The reliability of CTE is approximately .5 to .6. The correlation between RBS and CTE as computed by the Crew Re- search Laboratory is -.16 (N = 100). RBS and CTE are thus indepen- dent criterion scores. Tests of A Priori Hypotheses One hypothesis of the study stated that ASo of the Aircraft Dr. T. B. Roby, Personal communication. • 7 Commander (ASo r ) would be negatively correlated with criteria of crew effectiveness. This hypothesis was not supported. (r*-. 24) A subsidiary hypothesis stated that ASo _. would be nega- tively correlated with crew effectiveness criteria in crews in which the Aircraft Commander was most preferred crew member (AC = MPC). This hypothesis was also not supported. In fact, we found a positive correlation of .62 (N = 22, PX.01) between Control Time Error and the ASo score of accepted (most preferred) Aircraft Commanders. Relations of ASo to Radar Bombing General Considerations . Since the a priori hypotheses had yielded negative results we used the bomber crew data for further exploratory work. Hypotheses derived from these data were then tested on a sample of Army Tank crews. In our exploratory work on Air Force crews we decided to work with the radar bombing criterion since it reflects one of the most important functions of B-29 crews. Radar bombing is also considered to be more nearly a crew product in contrast with Con- trol Time Error which depends almost entirely on the Navigator. Sociometric relations . Our studies of informal groups had considered the sociometric relationship within the crew, since we correlated only the ASo score of the informal leader (the most preferred co-worker) with effectiveness criteria. Although the hypothesis that the ASo of the "accepted" Aircraft Commander would correlate with Radar Bomb Scores was not supported we explored further whether ASo of the Aircraft Commander might Dr. T. B. Roby, Personal communication 8 correlate with radar bombing under still more clearly defined sociometricr conditions. In particular we considered the accepted leader's relations with his keymen, i.e., the specialists on his crew who are most directly concerned with criterion relevant tasks in radar bombing. These keymen are the Radar Operator (VO) and the Navigator (N), both of whom have radar equipment and perform related tasks during RBS runs. Results . Tables 2, 3, and 4 present the results of this ex- ploratory work. As can be seen, high negative correlations were found in two related sociometric conditions. In the first of these the Aircraft Commander is Most Preferred Crew Member and endorses one or both of his keymen (AC = MPC --> VO and/or N), (Tables 2 and 3). Where the accepted Aircraft Commander does not choose his keyman,ASo and Radar Bomb Scores tend to be positively corre- lated. A second related method selects crews in which Aircraft Commander .and keymen mutually choose each other. Here again ASo.^, correlates negatively with the criterion. (Table 4) To judge from the magnitude of the correlations this is apparently a less powerful method but it has the advantage of utilizing a some- what greater number of crews. It is based on the assumption that Sociometric endorsement was arbitrarily defined by the ranks 1 to 2 for high choice ( --£■ ), 2.5 to 3 for medium choice ( ), and 1.9 - 10 for rejection ( -/> ). These breaking points are in part based on the desire to divide the groups into three equal sub-samples; how- eve fc, it does not seem reasonable to assume that a person tied with two others for first choice, or chosen as second most liked, is some- one toward whom the AC is indifferent. A compromise was here ma VO/N — VO/N -?» VO/N -.81 i U .13 7 .42 7 -.03 18 -.80 5 -.67 7 (.01) AC = Aircraft Commander --> High sociometric choice VO = Radar Observer -- Neutral sociometric choice MPC = Most Preferred Crew Member */> Low sociometric choice As this study explored many hypotheses, tests of significance ere not interpretable. better a leader will be/able to influence his keymen whence has a mutually good relationship with them. Relation of ASo to Control Time Error . It will be recalled that we found no correlation between the ASo score of the Aircraft Commander (AC g ) and Control Time Error (CTE) (.16, N - 51) but that the correlation of ASo _ with CTE was .62 (N = 22) in crews which accepted their leader. This relation is contrary to the results of all previous studies, and we therefore compared the opera- tions involved in CTE with those related to radar bombing. Interviews with former B-29 personnel, as well as perusal of various manuals, suggested* some basic differences in the Aircraft As we indicated above, RBS and CTE are uncorrelated scores. 10 Table 3 Correlation of the Aircraft Commander's ASo Score with Radar Bombing under Different Sociometric Conditions Sociometric K =1 Radar Operator- K=Navi gator Condition Rho N P Rho N P AC = MPC -■* K -.76 8 (.05*) -.93 9 * (.01) -- K -.05 8 — .43 10 ■— -/:>K -.05 9 — .89 6 (.05) AC i MFC ~? K .03 £ — .05 8 — --K -.13 7 — .36 10 — /*K -.56 10 * -.11 12 AC = Aircraft Commander -* Positive sociometric cnoice MPC = Most Preferred Crew Member j* Negative sociometric choice K = Keyman -- Neutral sociometric choice See Footnote to Table 2 Commander's relationship with his Navigator as against that with other crew members. The aC -Navigator relationship seems to be unique in approaching that of an advisee to an expert advisor. The Navigator is charged with collecting and integrating various naviga- tional data and he then advises the Aircraft Commander on the approp- riate course heading. While the AC ordinarily is supposed to follow his Navigator's directions during the operations relevant to CTE, Aircraft Commanders vary in their willingness or ability to do so, especially since the AC is allowed considerable latitude in using his judgment, and may even disregard the Navigator's advice if he so desires. 11 Table 4 Correlation of the Aircraft Commander's ASo with the Radar Bombing Criterion in Crews in which the AC Reciprocates his Keymen's Choices Sociometric Condition Rho N P AC «-» VO and/or N AC G) F hase subsequent to sociometric Criterion Rho N P I T/H, -.53 6 — V T/H y -.51 12 .05 Combined 2T/H -.76 1 .05 I P A -.37 6 - V P A -.71 12 .01 Combined P A -.60 7 .10 Operational Hypothesis 2a stated that ASo would be negatively correlated with Travel Time, when the accepted Tank Commander endorses the Driver. Table 8 presents correlations pertaining to the Travel Time criterion. Operational Hypothesis 2b could not be tested because of the insufficient number of avail- able erews (N = 4). We therefore consider the major hypothesis of the valida- tion study as supported, in particular since the uncorrelated Gunnery and Travel Time Criteria give consistent results. 21 Table 7 Correlation between the Gunnery Criterion with the ASo of the Tank Commander under a Specified Sociometric Condition TC *-» G Phase subsequent to sociometric Criterion Rho N P I T/Hj -.35 9 - V T/H y --32 9 - Combined ST/H -.61 9 .05 I P A -.43 9 - V P A -.72 9 .02 Combined P A -.7? 9 .02 Table 8 Correlation of the Travel Time Criterion with Tank Commanders 1 ASo Score under a Specified Sociometric Conditio* (TC = MFC --*D) Sociometric s in Phase Criterion Rho N P I V Combined T/Tj - 2 r/T v -.74 6 st/t -.33 5 Related Findings Further analyses were confined to crews haviiag accepted leaders since the leader was not accepted in only eight of the tank crews which were available for validation. However, groups not having accepted formal leaAert do appear to differ markedly from those in which the formal leader is accepted. As we have seen above, ASo of the accepted leader who endorsed his keyman correlated negati vely with group effectiveness 22 criteria. In addition, Table 9 shows that ASo and effectiveness correlate positively in crews in which the accepted leader does not endorse his keyman. This suggests that two separate attitudes of the leader toward his co-workers interact in affecting the per- formance of his group" (a) the leader's generalized attitude toward co-workers (ASo), and (b) his specific attitude toward his keyman (measured sociometrically). Table 9 Summary of correlations obtained from bomber and tank crews in which the Leader is Most Preferred Crew Member* Leade r's Prefsrer.ce for Keyman Sample Criterion Keyman Positive Neutr al 1 Negative Rho N Rho N 1 Rho N i B-29 RBS VO/N -.81 11 -.13 7 .42 7 B-29 %SVC B -.52 7 .47* 9 .27** 7 Tanks st/h G -.60 6 .11 6 .60 5 Tanks st/t D -.33 5 .3 9 6 .43 6 Friedman's £ test (7) indicates that this table departs signifi- cantly from randomness. ♦♦ See Footnote, p. 13 Figure 1 presents the data of Table 9 in graphic form. The correlation between the leader's generalized interpersonal attitude (ASo) and his group's effectiveness is plotted along the vertical axis; the leader's positive, neutral, or negative socio- metric rating of his keyman (indicated by +, 0, -, respectively) is plotted on the horizontal axis. The interaction between ASo and sociometric preferences is readily apparent in this figure. ASo and group effectiveness correlate negatively in crews in which the leader endorses his keyman (points labelled "A") but positively in crews where the leader does not endorse his keyman (points labelled "C"). The point labelled "B" identifies ' 23 Rho's between leaders' ASo and effectiveness criteria i i- 1 i i i i H = 5 ft U H P o tj o *-i »-*» ni H V> (T> rt s ° re & § ^* P »-• (T) "o •■*» ,r p - - a re re X V 4 £ ~ rt en u re 0) ro re >C "J C V) 0) rt h»» ft 4 > 1— ' ft rt < re p re o Of - o re w» : O re _*< - 3 p> cr w) o o 3 p re a H re t" Si 5 « a- o ~ ?. p> < 2L » * t« re w S»tf w Z rt rt re a p OQ {0 M« U> rt ►-* re P) a. re 4 re re •i p O re rt» o re 3 P f ^??^ ?9?f i ?999?q 9 99 9 . O-i o-i O rt- re o II II o H !»•• rt- re H h»» O r tf o ») ii ii en < o o •i *-•• rt- re H H R o II II n h a s a h3 o O H H* rt- re M H ttO tl ii n H^g ►4 CD o -v] On O O vrv t - Vjj ro i_j o I- 1 O O O O O O O ro\jjfVn.CN-ocovo OOQO O OOO O o ^4 crews in which the leaders preference for his keyman was neutral. Discussion The studies raise a number of problems. In addition to the interaction effect presented above we will consider questions concerning the leader's status in the group and concerning the classification of tasks in terms of the hypotheses to which they give rise* Interaction between Sociometric Preference and ASo The studies of bomber and tank crews have yielded re- sults which indicate that effective work groups tend to have accepted leaders (a) who are generally distant toward their co- workers (low ASo) and have a close relationship with their key- men, or (b) who have generally close relations with co-workers but who are distant toward their particular keyman. In an earlier paper dealing with informal teams we sug- gested that effective leadership requires a certain distance between the leader and his co-workers (6). The present studies lead to the additional hypothesis that the effective leader can maintain this distance in one of two ways. If he is generally distant, i.e., has a tendency to perceive, and react to, persons in a generally cold and reserved manner, he must compensate for this feeling of distance to others by a feeling of closeness Categorization of crews into groups in which the leader's preference was positive, neutral, or negative, was based on available data. The results therefore require further validation; future work may indicate that more or fewer subdivisions provide the most efficient utilization of data for purposes of prediction. -.. , 25 toward his keyman. Multiple correlations, using the variables, leader's ASo, sociometric preference (SP) and their product ASo x SP, yielded R's as shown in Table 10. Table 10 Prediction of three effectiveness criteria by the Assumed Similarity and Sociometric Preference variables, singly, jointly, and in combination with the Assumed Similarity x Sociometric Preference Score Criterion N ASo SP R^ 3 ^1*2 3 4* (1) (2) (3) Radar Bomb- 22 -.21 -.04 .21 .45 ing Time per Hit 17 -.30 -.15 .32 .45 Travel Time 17 .12 .15 .20 .3 * Variable 4 is ASo x SP; these scores were obtained by multi- plying the standard scores of ASo and SP Si Table 10, neither the multiple correlations nor the incre- ments of R^.2 3 4 ar e statistically significant when considered in- dividually. The beta weights for the product term ASo x SP were -.56, -.33, and -.24 for RBS, T/H, and T/T, respectively. The first two of these weights are considerably greater than the weights for ASo and SP in the RBS and T/H correlations. The third beta weight for ASo x SP is equal to the weight for ASo in the T/T correlation and these two weights are again considerably greater than the weight for SP. This finding is related to the interpretation that we are deal- ing with a single dimension of emotional distance which is a composite of ASo and sociometric preference. The negative signs show that an increase of the cross product is In terms of the work by Halpin (8), Hemphill (9) and their associates, these may well be the leaders who have high "considera- tion" as well as initiation-of -structure scores. The writer is indebted to Dr. C. F. Wrigley for suggesting the use of the cross product term as a variable and for his assistance in interpretation of the results. It may be noted that this use of the product term is arithmetically identical to that of Saunders' (15 ) "moderator variable" model. 26 detrimental to effectiveness, thus again indicating that effective group leaders tend to be persons with high ASo and low Socio- metric preference for their keyman, or vice versa. On the whole, these multiple regressions lend further support to the interpretation that the effective leader's gener- alized and specific attitudes toward co-workers complexly inter- act, resulting in an optimum leader-keyman distance. This hypothesized distance may be visualized as the sum of the dis- tance engendered by a generalized attitude (ASo) and of the distance resulting from a particular interpersonal attitude (expressed by sociometric choice). An attempt has been made to present this relationship in Figure 2. ASo is visualized as an index of the leader's generalized relations to his co-workers. The less similarity the leader assumes, i.e., the lower his ASo, the less approachable will he be in his relations with others. In this figure, this distance is shown as the radius of the circle. The sociometric preference is also visualized as a distance. The higher the leader' 3 pref- erence for his keyman, the closer his psychological distance to him. The less his sociometric preference, the greater the dis- tance between himself and his keyman. This distance is optimal in diagrams b and c. Overly distant or overly close relations are detrimental to group effectiveness. The latter are diagrammed in a and d, respectively. Most large organizations implicitly or explicitly recognize the importance of social and psychological distance in work rela- tions, and have established elaborate rules for maintaining this distance between leader and follower. Thus the armed forces 27 Optimal distance between leader and keyman High ASo and high socio- metric preference for keyman - Poor Crew (L = MPC-JK K High ASo and low socio- metric preference for keyman - Good Crew (L = MPC ~fT) Low ASo and high socio metric preference for keyman - Good Crew (L = MPC — » K) ■>K Low ASo and low sociometric preference for keyman - Poor Crew (L = MPC/->K) Figure 2. Schematic Representation of the Interaction between ASo and Sociometric Preference in Good and Poor Crews. Z8 separate the senior officer from the junior officer, and the junior officer from his enlisted men. We may point to the restricted officers' and NCO messes and washrooms, the of- ficers' country aboard ship, the Admiral's deck, the periodic job rotations, and many other rules which operate to restrict informal contacts between leader and follower. Large industrial organizations have similar, although perhaps less explicit, rules in industry. We also find the executive dining room and the em- ployees' cafeteria, informal sanctions against vice presidents and bookkeepers playing golf together, and many others. These customs and institutions, whether by design or not, operate to maintain a distance between leader and followers. In light of our studies, the maintenance of such a distance by means of various barriers appears justified when the leader tends to have generally close relations with others. These same barriers may be detrimental in cases where the accepted leader naturally tends to have distant relations with others: here w^ would expect poorer group perform- ance since the extreme distance between the generally reserved leader who has negative feelings toward his keyman may perhaps make successful communication too difficult. On the other hand, the approachable outgoing leader may well become emotionally too involved with the keyman whom he likes, and he may therefore have difficulty in making sound decisions involving his subordinate, and in applying sanctions in the case of poor keyman performance. Thus, Katz, et al. report a greater role differentiation between supervisor and supervisees in high productivity than in low productivity groups (13). -'■ 29 The leader's status in the group The relations which we have found in our studies are limited to groups in which the formal leader is also the group's informal leader. It is probable that the informal leader status provides the formal leader with considerable influence and power over his crew members. Because the group supports his actions, the leader* s demands can be reinforced not only by official sanc- tions but also by the much more potent pressures which the group itself might bring to bear on the non-conforming individual. Where the leader does not have informal leader status another person may perhaps take over some of his leadership functions as has been sug- gested by other investigators (1, 12). It is also possible that effective teams, which do not accept their leaders, utilize the keyman's skill and the leadership ability of other crew members to a relatively greater extent. While our data provide some leads, further research is required to develop theory and methods for predicting the effect- iveness of these groups. Categorization of group tasks . Our studies have been mainly concerned with situations which seem to require positive, ie., direction-giving leadership behavior. However, we found in the bomber crew study that the correlation between Control Time Error and the Aircraft Com- mander's ASo is in the opposite direction from that obtained in other studies. While the finding may be due to chance, we are inclined to believe that Control Time Error requires a receptive attitude on the part of the Aircraft Commander. If this is the case, 30 a categorization of group tasks in terms of optimum leader atti- tudes would be of considerable practical and theoretical importance. Such studies may well lead to new conceptions of training and place- ment of potential leaders, a conclusion which is also suggested by research on Naval Leadership by Shartle, Stogdill, and others (16, 17). Research on this aspect of the group effectiveness problem is now under way. Summary and Conclusions Two studies are here reported which investigate the re- lation between the leader's interpersonal attitudes and the effect- iveness of small military combat crews. Two types of leader attitudes were considered; a general- ized attitude toward co-workers, and the attitude toward a specific crew member. The former is obtained when we ask S to predict the personality test responses of the persons whom he considers to be his most and least preferred co-workers. The statistical comparison of these two predictions yields the interpersonal per- ception score, Assumed Similarity between Opposites (ASo). The second attitude measure is concerned with sociometric pref- erences of the leader for a particular co-worker, namely, the k«yman, or the specialist on the crew who is most directly con- cerned with the criterion relevant operations. An exploratory study using 53 B-29 bomber crews led to the hypothesis that the ASo of the leader would be negatively correlated with crew effectiveness criteria in groups in which the accepted leader sociometrically endorsed his keyman, 31 or in which leader and keyman mutually chose each other. In addition, the presence of an interaction effect between the leader's status, his attitude toward generalized co-workers (.aSo) and his attitude toward his keyman (his sociometric rating of his keyman) was found, suggesting that the psychological distance between leader and keyman is related to the effectiveness of the team. a validation study was conducted on 25 Army tank crews which participated in a carefully controlled weapons analysis experiment. Two uncorrelated criteria were available for the study. The hypothesis was supported that leaders* aSo scores correlate negatively with criteria in crews of specified socio- metric structure. The study also provided corroborating evidence that the psychological distance between accepted leaders and key- men, here defined by ASo and sociometric preference, is related to effective team work. 32 References 1. Cattell, R. B. New concepts for measuring leadership in terms of group syntality. Hum . Relat., 1951, 4, 161-184. 2. Cronbach, L. J., and Gleser, Goldine C. Similarity between persons and related problems of profile analysis. Cham- paign-Urbana, Illinois: 1952. (Mimeographed, Technical Report No. 2, Contract N6ori-07135 between the University of Illinois and the Office of Naval Research.) 3. Cronbach, L. J., Hartmann, W., and Ehart, Mary E. Investi- gation of the character and properties of assumed similarity measures. Champaign-Urbana. 1953. (Mimeographed, Technical Report No. 7, Contract N6ori-07135 between the University of Illinois and the Office of Naval Research.) 4. Fiedler, F^ E., Hartmann, W., and Rudin, S. A. Interpersonal perception in the prediction of effectiveness of basketball teams. Champaign-Urbana, 1952. (Mineographed, Technical Report No. 3, Contract N6ori-07135 between the University of Illinois and the Office of Naval Research.) 5. Fiedler, F. E. Assumed similarity measures as predictors of team effectiveness in surveying. Champaign-Urbana, 1953. (Mimeographed, Technical Report No. 6, Contract N6ori- 07135 between the University of Illinois and the Office of Naval Research.) 6. Fiedler, F. E. The psychological distance dimension in inter- personal relations. J. Pers ., 1953, 22, 142-150. 7» Friedman, M. The use of ranks to avoid the assumption of normality. £. Am . Stat. Assn ., 1937, 3_2, 675-701. 8. Halpin, A. W. The leadership behavior and combat perform- ance of airplane commanders. J. abnorm . soc . Psychol ., 1954,49, 19-22. 9« Hemphill, J. K. and Coons, A. E. Leader Behavior Descrip- tion . Columbus, Ohio, Pers. Res. Bd. Ohio State U. 1950 10. Hill, F. I., Scott, G. W., Eckles, A. J., Hardison, D. C, McAuley, J. A. An assembly of Project STALK data . Ballistics Research Laboratory Memorandum Report No. ?45, Vol I (Classified). 11. Jennings, Helen. Leadership and isolation. ( 2nd Ed.) New York: Longmans* Green, and Co, 1950. ■ 33 jl2. Kahn, R. L. and Katz, D. Leadership practices in relation to productivity and morale, in Cartwright, D. and Zander, A., Group Dynamics . Evanston, 111.: Row Peterson, 1953. 13. Katz, D., Maccoby, N., Gurin, G., Floor, Lucretia G. Productivity supervision and morale among railroad workers. Ann Arbor, Mich.: Univ. of Michigan, 1951 14. Knoell, Dorothy and Forgays, D. G. Interrelationships of combat crew performance in the B-29 « San Antonio, Tex.: Human Resources Research Center, Lackland AFB, December 1952. (Research Note CCT 52-1) 15. Saunders, D. R. Moderator variables in prediction. Res. Bulletin , RB-53-23. Educ. Testing Service, Princeton.N. J. 16. Shartle, C. L. and Stogdill, R. M. Studies in naval leadership : methods, results, and applications. Final technical report. Columbus, Ohio, Pers. Res. Bd. Ohio State U. 1953. Litho, 17. Stogdill, R. M., Wherry, R. J., and Jaynes, W. E. Patterns of leadership behavior: A factorial study of navy officer performance . Columbus, Ohio, Pers. Res. Bd. Ohio State U. Mimeo* 18. Wherry, R. J. and Fryer, D. H. Buddy ratings: Popularity contests or leadership criteria. Pers. Psychol ., 1942, 2, 147-149. 19. Whyte, W. F. Street Corner Society . Chicago: Univ. Press, 1943. I ■■*.*, . ,. <«*.,*• / I •I « \ t i J til i W tj if If 1 lif tiff 1 iiiiidil • l I ! t It 111