Class ]B F 4 7 g Hook J3 4 _ PHKSKNTED HY Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2011 with funding from The Library of Congress http://www.archive.org/details/rhythmOObolt RHYTHM. BY THADDEUS L. BOLTON, Demonstrator and Fellow in Psychology, Clark University, Worcester, Mass. Approved as a thesis for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at Clark University, Worcester, Mass. Q. Stanley Hall. (Reprinted from the American Journal of Psychology, Vol. VI. No. 2.) PRESS OT F. S. BLANCHARD AHD COMPANY, WORCESTER. 3>fc Gift The University ?2%'05 RHYTHM. By Thaddbus L. Bolton, Demonstrator and Fellow in Psychology, Clark University. Reprinted from The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. VI., No. 2. Introduction. Although experimental psychology began more than a hundred years ago through the discovery of the personal equation, it has as yet covered but a small portion of the field of mental phenomena. The nature of sensation, the time- relations of mental phenomena, memory, association, space and time concepts have been carefully studied by many eminent scientists. But the whole field of the emotions has been practically a mare clausum for psychologists. Several attempts to study and determine the nature of the simplest aesthetic forms have been reported ; theories of pleasure and pain, supported by some experimental observations, have been advanced, but no serious attempt has been made to submit the emotions to experimental investigation. Every psycholo- gist recognizes the necessity of doing so and that it is the most important field of mental phenomena, and that, until psychologists can reduce the motions to some semblance of order or more ultimate principles, experimental psychology can be said to cover only a part of the field of mental life. Every one is waiting for some one else to point out the way. There seems to be a general feeling that when once an en- trance has been effected, the greatest difficulty will have been 2 BOLTON : surmounted and the whole field will yield to experiment. In a measure this is the true view to take, and yet certain of the emotions are as distinctly separated from others as the whole field is from that of the memory. When the demand for such a study is so great, and students are being urged to make a trial, that one who does attempt it, though he fail, cannot be accounted rash. The experimental study of rhythm which is to be presented in this paper, is an attempt to push the lines of exact science a little farther forward into a field that borders more closely upon the field of aesthetics than any other that experimental psychologists have tried. The attempt is to be made to re- duce rhythm to a more fundamental activity of mind. The pleasure that individuals take in the rhythmic flow of words and sounds has been ascribed by one to the ' ' Unifying Ac- tivity of the Feelings, ' ' by another to a " Sense of Order, ' ' and by still another to ' ' The Feelings of Equality. ' 7 Such explanations as these do not meet the question at all, unless it can be shown that such activities or feelings are ultimate facts of mind. If they are ultimate facts of the mind, it will be necessary, in order to make the explanation complete and valid, to show how they underlie other activities, for it is not to be supposed that any fundamental activity will manifest itself in a single phenomenon which bears no relation to other phenomena. Such does not seem to have been done by those who have offered explanations of the rhythms in speech, and the problem remains just where it was taken up. To re- gard rhythm as the manifestation or the form of the most fundamental activities of mind, seems a clearer view and to offer less difficulties than to regard it as an ultimate fact in itself. The problem, then, is to show how and to what extent it underlies mental activity, and, as preparatory to this, what part it plays in physiology and nature. Is there not some universal principle which is adequate as an explanation of rhythm in general? Ehythm is so universal a phenomenon in nature and in physiological activity, and underlies so completely speech, that I desire to call attention to some of its manifestations in detail before presenting the experimental study. Rhythms in Nature : — Natural phenomena very generally, if not universally, take a rhythmic form. There is a periodic recurrence of a certain phenomenon, sometimes accompanied by others, going on continuously in all that pertains to nature. Motion, whether in the broader field of the universe or upon the earth, is very generally periodic. Light, heat, sound, and probably electricity, are propagated in the form of waves. A falling body does not follow a straight line, RHYTHM. 3 neither does a rifle bullet describe a simple curve which is the resultant of the combined forces of gravity and the initial velocity. Mr. Herbert Spencer has treated this subject in his " Principles of Philosophy " at considerable length, and has left but little that can be said here. Although he does not say so in so many words, he seems to hold that it is the only possible form of activity ; continuous motion is an im- possibility. 7 The cosmic rhythms, however, are the most fundamental and important of natural phenomena. They may be shown to underlie in a measure and be the cause of many other rhythms in plant and animal life. The regular alternation of light and darkness due to the rotation of the earth upon its axis is tbe most striking rhythm in the cosmos. The two periods of light and darkness constitute a unit — the day — which re- mains always the same in length. Days are grouped into months by the revolution of the moon about the earth, and into years by the revolution of the earth about the sun. These periodic changes have had a tremendous influence upon animal and plant life, and have stamped their impress upon all living organisms in the most striking manner ; some, however, upon certain organisms more than upon others. In the vegetable kingdom some plants show a daily growth and repose; their flowers bloom in the morning and close before the evening. Some turn their petals towards the sun, and make a daily revolution in order to keep them so. In certain latitudes all vegetation shows normal periods of growth and fruitage which are not necessarily cut short or lengthened by early or late frosts. It requires a certain time for development without regard to the character of the season. The lunar period is known to influence the blooming of flowers. A species of Chinese roses blooms with a monthly regularity during the season. The influence of these cosmic rhythms is not less upon the animal kingdom. The daily rhythm causes the daily periods of sleep and waking, from which no terrestrial creatures of the higher types are exempt. The periods of sleep and wak- ing are not determined by the effect of light and darkness as are the movements of many plants. The lunar period has had a far-reaching effect upon animal creatures, especially as re- gards reproduction and the nervous system. The periods of gestation and the recurrence of heat and menstrual flow in both human beings and animals bear a very close and strik- ing relation to the lunar period. The period of gestation in some lower mammalian animals is one month. In the higher forms it is a certain number of months. The time of incuba- tion is with some species of fowls a month, but it seems to 4 BOLTON : conform in general to a period of days which is a certain multiple of seven, seven being one-fourth of a lunar month. Fourteen, twenty- one and twenty- eight days are very common periods of incubation. The year exercises a still wider in- fluence upon the animal kingdom. The normal life of most species of insects terminates in a single year. The frog becomes nervous and irritable with the approach of spring, although the conditions under which it is kept may not change. The polar bear goes into hibernation, even though he has not made the proper preparation in the way of a store of fat. The migrations of birds are not necessarily prompted by the signs of approaching winter. Animals breed generally in the spring — a fact which cannot find sufficient explanation in the influence of a warmer temperature. It has been fairly established that growth is more rapid during the summer months. Although we find that these cosmic rhythms have stamped themselves upon the organism more or less permanently, they have wielded a far mightier influence upon the minds of men. Among primitive peoples that were rich in imagina- tive power, they have given rise to the most elaborate and beautiful systems of mythology and worship that the world has ever seen. It is a common speculation in childhood that, endowing animals at birth, as children do, with rational intelligence, but with a total lack of experience, the young creature must be driven to strange thoughts and speculations when the first light of day breaks in upon him, or when dark- ness approaches for the first time. What can be the thoughts of such a creature when he experiences the change of seasons or the first snow storm 1 ? No objects that are presented to the child so stimulate his thought and become such food for his fancy as the heavenly bodies and cosmic phenomena. Many of their minds are filled with myths about the stars that are as original and beautiful in conception, though lack- ing in detail, as much of the Greek mythology. The recurrence of the day of the year upon which some event has happened is commemorated as a day of joy or sorrow according to the nature of the event. All national and religious festivals recur once a year. Among primitive peoples worship takes place always at the same time of day or year, and the same might be said of most enlightened people. There seems now, and always has seemed, a peculiar appropriateness in performing certain duties at the same time of day or year, although it does not necessarily depend upon the nature of the weather or of the event. The Christian Sabbath and other religious festivals, both savage and civilized, find their origin in the nature worship of the sun and the moon. RHYTHM. 5 There are still other rhythms in the cosmos which seem to exercise an influence upon mankind. Sun spots make their appearance in great numbers once in about eleven years, and the attempt has been made to connect these with great financial disasters and religious awakenings which seem to recur in the same time. The social customs of the race show similar changes, which may prove to have some con- nection with sun spots. The coincidence warrants an in- vestigation and allows speculation. Upon the morbid side science has made discoveries of the most striking character. Even from the earliest times a periodicity has been observed in certain forms of insanity and in other mental diseases. These have been confirmed by later investigations. 1 Both crime and suicide show a periodicity which corresponds with the year, and another which corresponds to the larger period of sun spots. Physiological Rhythms : — ~Ro fact is more familiar to the physiologist than the rhythmic character of many physio- logical processes. In physiology it means the regular alter- nation of periods of activity and periods of repose or of lesser activity. The term is also applied to any alternation of activity and repose, whether it is regular or not. These periods of activity and intervals of repose may succeed one another at very small intervals of time, as in the case of a clonic contraction of the muscle, or at very much greater intervals, as in the case of sleep and waking, or better still, in the periods of growth in children. Several of the most vital and important bodily activities are distinctly rhythmical, and will serve as types of all physiological rhythms. Of these, might be mentioned the pulse, respiration, walking and speech. The first two are involuntary actions, which in the very nature of the organism must be more or less rhythmical. Such actions are controlled by the lower nerve centres, and the organs concerned in them are connected in a reflex arc with these nerve centres. Habits are in the nature of invol- untary actions. Of these, walking and speech are the most important and are true types of rhythmical activity. In each there is a series of coordinated muscles in which the contrac- tion of one is the signal for the contraction of the next in the series, the last acting as a stimulus to the first. Independent of the regular beat of the heart and forming a kind of higher grouping of these beats, the arteries undergo continuously rhythmical contractions and dilations of their 1 Dr. Koster, "Uber die Gesetze des periodischen Irreseins und verwandter Nervenzustande." Bohn, 1882. Dr. Ludwig, " Periodischen Psychosen." Stuttgart, 1878. 6 BOLTON : walls, now increasing and now decreasing the blood supply. These may be observed with a glass in the arteries of a frog's foot or a rabbit's ear, occurring about once a minute. They may be made to cease entirely by cutting off the nerves going to these organs. 1 These arteries are controlled by the vaso- motor system, and the rhythmic contractions of the arteries seem to indicate a rhythm in the activity of the nerve centres. As we shall see later, there is some ground for believing that all nervous action is rhythmical. Eegular contractions occur in the heart of some animals after they have been removed from the body, and are found to be due probably to the presence of nerve ganglia in these organs. 2 The effect of deficient arterialization upon the vaso- motor system is to cause a rise in the curve of blood pressure. This curve, then, shows certain undulations, which have been called Traube-Hering curves, from their discoverer. This result is obtained by cutting the vagi nerve and stopping respiration. The venus blood then acts as a stimulus upon the vaso-motor centres in the medulla, which causes these rhythmic movements. "This rhythmic rise 3 must be due to the rhythmic contraction of the arteries, and this is caused by a rhythmic discharge from the vaso- motor centres." " The vaso-motor nervous system is apt to fall into a condition of rhythmic activity." A similar phenomenon has been thought to be observed in regard to the spinal cord. When the spinal cord 4 of a dog, cat or rabbit was cut, rhythmical contractions of the sphincter ani and of the vagina appeared. These contractions vary in number, but are generally about twenty per minute for the sphincter ani and four per minute for the vagina. The centre for these contractions was found to be in the spinal cord, about the level of the sixth and seventh lumbar vertebrae in rabbits and of the fifth lumbar vertebra in dogs. 1 Foster's "Physiology," 6th Ed. p. 307. Dr. Ellis, working under Dr. Bowditch, has studied these contrac- tions in the web of a frog's foot with the microscope. He says that cutting the sciatic nerve does not stop them, and concludes that they are due to peripheral centres, unless he be allowed to suppose that automatic contractility is a property of smooth muscle tissue. Plethysmographic and vaso-motor experiments with frogs. Jour, of Phys. Vol. VI. No. 6, p. 437. 2 Foster's "Physiology," 6th Ed. p. 357. 3 Foster's "Physiology." 6th Ed. p. 622. 4 Isaac Ott, "Observations upon the Physiology of the Spinal Cord." Studies from Biol. Lab. at Johns Hopkins University. No. II. RHYTHM. 7 Fatigue shows itself to be a rhythmical process. Dr. Lombard 1 worked upon the flexor muscle of the second finger. After contracting the muscle several times, lifting each time a weight, he gradually lost the power of further contraction, but he continued to make the effort at regular intervals of two seconds. In a short time he regained his former power, which he maintained for several minutes, and then gradually lost it again. About five periods of alter- nating loss and recovery took place in twelve minutes. By variations in the methods of experimentation, the different factors are eliminated, and he is able to conclude that the centre of voluntary control is unaffected, but that this periodicity is dependent upon " alterations which take place in some of the mechanisms between the areas of the brain originating the will impulses and the centrifugal nerves." Dr. Hodge 2 found that when he stimulated the spinal ganglia of a cat continuously with an interrupted current, no change of the cell took place. When he applied his interrupted current for a quarter of a second and allowed the cell to rest three-quarters, a change took place in the nucleus of the cell. These experiments are inconclusive, as in the first case the animal was given curari and in the second it was not. Dr. Burgerstein 3 tested a number of school children by their ability to multiply and add figures for four successive periods of ten minutes, with five minutes' interval between the periods of work. During the third period there was a marked falling off in the amount of work accomplished and an increase again during the fourth period. He argues that the pupils became fatigued during the first two periods, and that the third was a period of recovery, since the normal amount of work was shown again in the fourth. The secondary rhythm observed in the circulation occurs also in respiration. Under ordinary circumstances respira- tion follows a rhythm of about fifteen or twenty a minute. During certain diseases and sleep a secondary rhythm — Cheyne-Stokes 4 curves — appears in respiration. The respira- tory movements decrease in depth until they disappear entirely. After an interval of a few seconds a slight movement occurs. This is followed by others, which increase in strength until they become normal and sometimes abnormally strong. Two explanations are offered : first, a waxing and waning in the nutrition of the respiratory centres, and second, a rhythmic 1 Warren P. Lombard, M. D., "Effect of Fatigue upon Muscular Contractions." Amee. Jour, op Psy. Vol. III. 2 " Microscopical Study of Changes due to Functional Activity in Nerve Cells." Jour, of Morphology, Vol. VII. 3 Die Arbeitskurve einer Stunde. Zeitschr.f. Schulges. IV. 9, 10. * Foster's " Physiology," 6th Ed. p. 605. 8 BOLTON : increase and decrease in the inhibitory impulses playing upon the centres. The latter explanation is favored. This, however, simply assigns the rhythmic action to some other centre and does not explain the phenomenon. A certain amount of secondary rhythm takes place in the breathing of hibernating animals. Bespiration appears almost to cease and then to start again, but it is generally slower during hibernation. Growth appears to take place rhythmically. Distinct periods of activity and rest occur in the embryonic develop- ment of some species that have been observed. This has been seen in the segmentation of pulmonates' eggs. 1 It is no less true of the amblystoma. In these the periods of activity last from five to fifteen minutes, and are succeeded by inter- vals of repose lasting about forty-five minutes. The activity of the protoplasm offers a resistance which must be overcome by the energy arising from the assimilation of the granular food material, which disappears as development proceeds. During the period of repose the energy is accumulating from this assimilation, which, when it becomes sufficient, overcomes the resistance, and activity sets in. This is taken to be a type of physiological and nervous activity, which will serve to explain certain phenomena of rhythm. This rhythm in growth, which is observed in the embryonic development, is characteristic of the physical and mental growth of children. For several years previous to puberty, great increase in stature is observed, puberty itself being a period of slow growth. From fifteen to eighteen is another period of growth, in which the full stature is generally reached. The mental character of children shows also periods of activity and repose. 2 The bright child becomes dull and the tidy slovenly. The leader in the athletic sports is now lazy and moping. Memory is dow predominant, and now reason. The child passes from one form of activity to another. The line of development goes zigzag to its goal. Other examples of involuntary action might be mentioned. These are the peristaltic contractions of the intestines, labor pains, the recurrence of heat and of the menstrual flow, and the secretions of the digestive cell. In these cells the secre- tions are kept up for about six hours, when a period of repose of about twenty-four hours follows. 3 1 W. K. Brooks, " Fresh Water Pulmonates." Studies from Biol. Lab. at Johns Hopkins Univ. Vol. II. 2 G. Siegert, ''Die Periodicitat in der Entwickelung des Kindes- natur." 3 J. M. Langley and Sewell, "Histology and Physiology of Pepsin- forming Glands." Phil. Trans. Vol. CLXXII. pp. 663-711. London, 1882. RHYTHM. 9 From this review it may be safely said that nervous action in general, and especially of the lower and vaso-motor centres, is rhythmical. This form of activity results from the resist- ance which the nervous substance offers to a stimulus. A certain amount of energy is necessary to overcome this resistance. This fact is brought out by the experiments of Helmholtz 1 and Sterling upon the summation of stimuli. Helmholtz found that when he stimulated a nerve going to a muscle by a submaximal stimulus and then added another stimulus at any time afterward within four seconds, he ob- tained a contraction. If he used a maximal stimulus in the first place and then added another stimulus during the latent period, it produced no effect upon the contraction due to the first stimulus. But if the second stimulus was added after the latent period, the effect was a greater contraction than that which followed the first stimulus alone. Submaximal stimuli 2 following one another, even as slow as one per second, will produce a contraction after a time. As the frequency of the stimulus increases, the effect is much more marked. It is much better to increase the frequency of the stimulus with- out increasing the strength than to increase the strength alone. Sterling adds further that all muscular and nervous action is due to summated stimuli — a conclusion that denies the possibility of contractions due to one instantaneous shock or at least does not explain them. Dr. Ward 3 determined that between the rates of .4 sec. and .03 sec. a contraction always followed a given number of stimuli. Above and be- low these limits the number might vary. In the same line is the work of Drs. Kronacker and Hall. It has been held by Sterling and others that when a stimu- lus is applied directly to the cortex, no matter what the rate, the brain sent out rhythmic impulses always at a constant rate. Dr. Limbeck 4 conducted a series of experiments upon the brain and spinal cord, in which he finds that the brain and spinal cord send out just as many impulses as they receive. Faster rates than thirteen shocks per second for the cortex and thirty-four for the cord, gave smooth curves. 1 Helmholtz, "Berichte der Berliner Akad." 1854, p. 358. 2 W. Sterling, " Uber die Summation electrischer Hautreize." Berlin. Berichte d. Sachsgelschaft d. Wissenschaft. December, 1874, p. 372. 3 Dr. Ward, " Uber die Auslosung von Reflexbewegungen durch einer Summe schwacker Reize." Archiv fur Anatomie und Physiol- ogic 1880, p. 72. Hugo Kronacker und G. Stanley Hall. Die willkurliche Muske- laction. Archiv fiir Anatomie und Physiologie, 1879. 4 Dr. R. U. Limbeck, " Uber den Rhythmus centralie Reize." Archiv fiir experimentale Pathologic Bd. XXV. H. 2. 10 BOLTON : The difference between the rates for cortex and cord is worthy of note in consequence of the close correspondence of the number of shocks for the cortex and the rate of the most rapid voluntary control ; while involuntary and clonic con- tractions which find their seats in the lower centres and in the cord may be much faster. In this connection the atten- tion of the reader is called to that portion of the experimental study in which the rate of clicks at which rhythmical grouping ceases is set forth. It is not far from ten a second. This is also near the lowest rate at which air vibrations give the impression of a musical tone. The theory of summated stimuli which was advanced by Wundt, and which is generally accepted, is based upon the resistance which a central cell offers to a stimulus. The in- coming stimulus is not communicated directly to the cell. The afferent nerve does not terminate in the cell, but breaks up into branches, which form a kind of envelope about the cell. The efferent nerve takes its rise in the nucleus of the cell and proceeds towards the periphery. If the stimulus is weak, it does not penetrate through the surface of the cell to the nucleus, but only part way. It sets up a kind of disturb- ance around the surface of the cell and, should another stimulus follow before the disturbance has subsided, it adds to the effect already produced. Eepeated stimuli still further in- crease the disturbance until it penetrate to the nucleus of the cell, when it causes the cell to discharge into the efferent nerve. This serves very well for summated stimuli, but other phenomena of just the opposite nature require ex- planation. There are the soothing effects of slow and gentle stroking or patting, such as hypnotizers and nurses use upon their subjects. The general fact seems to be that the stimu- lus must not rise much above the threshold, and be sufficiently slow, that there shall be no summation. As we shall see later, any repeated stimulus tends to take the form of a muscular movement accompanying it. If this stimulus becomes gradually slower, it leads finally to the concept of rest, and being accompanied by muscular movements, these movements must finally cease. Increased quietude follows the slowly decreasing movements, until before a great interval of time has elapsed the body falls into a state of rest. The stimulus must in any case be sufficient to command the attention of the subject to the exclusion of the disturbing effects of other stimuli coming from without and from the involuntary pro- cesses of the body. Let us return now to the nerve cell, to find if there are any processes going on which will throw light upon the problem. A weak stimulus is continually playing upon the cell from without, but never rises sufficiently RHYTHM. 11 in strength to penetrate beyond the periphery of the cell or in rapidity to bring about a summation. The effect of each stimulus subsides before the following one reaches the cell. The peripheral area must soon become fatigued so that it is no longer able to respond to the stimulus, and yet it is sufficiently strong to command the attention in so far as to distract it from other stimuli coming from within. This is the condition of quietude in the cell which is manifest in the muscle. Attention and Periodicity : — The most casual observer will discover that his attention is discontinuous and intermittent. It manifests itself in a wave-like form. It is a series of pulses. The mind does not rest for any length of time upon a single object. New phases and relations must continually appear, or the object is dropped, that another may be taken up. ' ' No one can possibly attend continuously to an object that does not change." 1 This process has been described as a "fly and perch." Charles Pierce says in his "Philosophy of Attention" that there is "no continuum." This periodicity in attention has been observed by Helmholtz 2 with the stereoscope and commented upon at considerable length. The phenomenon is called retinal rivalry. Mr. T. Eeed 3 re- cords some observations which he made in combining two stereoscopic views, which were ruled, the one with vertical, and the other with horizontal lines. He finds that the whole field will be occupied for a time with one view, and then this gives way for the other, which lasts an equal time. They seem to change without voluntary effort and even in spite of one's efforts to keep one view in the field. The full time for a change from one to the other and back again is from twelve to sixteen seconds for different subjects. The pulses of at- tention, however, seem to succeed one another at much shorter intervals. Two seconds seem a long time to hold any object which has no relation before the attention. James says : " There is no such thing as voluntary attention sus- tained for more than a few seconds at a time." Does it not, then, seem reasonable that during each wave or pulse of attention only one undivided state of consciousness can arise? The waxing and waning of attention seem to mark a change from one object of consciousness to another. The object of the state may be very complex, but it stands as a unit in consciousness. The problem of the relation of the parts of the object by which a great many may be allowed to stand as 1 James, " Psychology," Vol. I. p. 420. 2 " Physiologische Optik," Sec. 32. ■" Nature," August 1, 1887. 12 BOLTON : a unit in consciousness and be grasped in a single state, is of the most vital importance, but it must be deferred until later, when the normal period of a wave of attention will also be discussed. Rhythmic Speech: — The most distinguishing, and in many respects the most important, function of the human body is vocal utterance and articulate speech. Being an involuntary and habitual function in a large measure, it might be expected upon a priori grounds to be rhythmical. Speech becomes rhythmical not simply by sounds succeeded by pauses, but also by the regular recurrence of strongly accented sounds in a series. Aside from the simplest shout or exclamation of joy or pain, all vocal utterances are primarily rhythmical. Every word that contains more than one syllable consists of strong and weak syllables. These accents occur upon every other syllable in varying intensity, or at most the accented syllables are separated by two unaccented syllables. As regards vocal utterances, they can be considered from four different aspects — their regular succession, intensity, pitch and quality. The problem in a philosophical treatment of rhythmic speech is to determine the value of these properties of sound as unifying elements in a rhythmical production. It will be necessary first to inquire which is the most funda- mental, and secondly, where each enters and the part it plays in the development of literature. We must seek also other unifying principles, if such there be. Of these, we might now mention the logical meaning of words — the theme — and aesthetic forms. As we are concerned in speech in so far only as rhythmical effects are aimed at, we shall speak only of poetry. By what coordinations and subordinations of sounds with respect to their properties and meanings is the whole structure of the poem held together ? It is the same problem which Plato discussed as the one and the many. Kant put the same question by asking how the mind made a unity out of a manifold. We have to ask how the mental span becomes so enormously increased as to grasp such a poem as Words- worth's " Intimations of Immortality from the Eecollections of Childhood," or Milton's " Paradise Lost." How is the carrying power of the mind increased to such an extent? The answer is to be found in the fact that unities are formed out of the simplest elements of speech by coordinating some with others in respect to their time relations ; secondly, unities are formed of unities by subordinating them with respect to their intensities, and sometimes, their time values; thirdly, by coordinations and subordinations with respect to intensities and qualities, higher unities still are formed ; and RHYTHM. 13 fourthly, by coordinations and subordinations with respect to theme and aesthetic forms, the greatest unities are accom- plished. In the first place vocal utterances are related as regards time, that is, the same sound may recur at regular intervals, in which case the series thus formed might be termed a rhythmic series — a series which may become rhythmical. In the next place this series might be made up of louder and weaker sounds alternating with each other. The series would then be composed of groups of sounds and might be called a rhythmical series. This is a rhythm in speech. If now the louder sounds in each group were given different intensities, these smaller groups might be brought into larger groups still. In this way the mental span may be made to extend itself over a very large number of simple impressions. The principle is very clear, and one will see at a glance that if intelligible sounds were used and qualitative changes em- ployed, the mental span might be almost indefinitely extended. The carrying power of the mind, however, does not rest wholly in any case upon a single fact, if we make the excep- tion that vocal utterances must be carefully timed in a rhythmic series. Quality and pitch changes accompany changes in intensity, so that the subordination of one sound to another and their consequent unification with respect to intensity is always dependent upon pitch and quality changes as well. For this reason it is impossible to treat each properly by itself. Time-relations : — In order for vocal utterances to form a rhythmic series, they must occur at regular intervals of time which cannot exceed or fall much below certain limits. We may, however, upon the analogy of physiological rhythms, re- gard a series of sounds recurring at stated intervals as a rhyth- mical series, and also regard the recurrence of accented sounds as forming a secondary rhythm out of the primary. This is carrying the rhythmical idea farther than has been customary, and while it is more nearly correct, it would not be generally understood. The question of the time values of vocal utter- ances for rhythmical purposes cannot be answered upon an examination of poetry itself. Although the Greeks and Eomans assigned exact values to all syllables in their lan- guage, there is reason for believing that such values did not arise naturally, but were assigned when they began to specu- late upon poetry. No such relations exist among the sylla- bles of modern languages, and in English they never did. We must then dismiss the subject of time and its significance and revert to it as the subject permits. 14 BOLTON : Intensity of Sounds: — The mind accomplishes its first real unification of sounds by subordinating them with respect to their intensities. A rhythm in speech means a series of groups of sounds. Each group may contain two or more sounds, generally not more than four. Two sounds, one strong and one weak, the one succeeding the other in time, cannot give an idea of a rhythm, but two groups of two such sounds certainly can. This being the simplest possible rhythm, we should expect that it would be the earliest form in which literature appeared. Since we have not probably any extant specimens of the first literary productions, for they were not committed to writing, we must judge from those which have come down to us from later periods, and from the literature of primitive peoples and of children, what the earliest form was. In this way it has been proved that our surmise, which was made upon a priori ground simply, is correct. The oldest extant specimens of English poetry are generally composed of verses of two sections, which are separated by a pause in the middle. Each section generally contains four, sometimes six, syllables, two of which are unaccented and two accented. The first section was emphatic and corresponded to the accented syllable in the smaller division ; the second section received less stress and was less important. The two formed a kind of balance structure, in which the first section contained a rise and the second a fall. helle heafas: hearde nithas. wer leas werod: waldend sende. graes ungrande: gar secg theahte. 1 " Our Anglo-Saxon 2 poems consist of certain versicles, or, as we have hitherto termed them, sections, bound together in pairs by the laws of alliteration For the most part these sections contain two or three accents, but some are found containing four, or even five. The greater number of these sections may be divided into two parts, which generally fulfill all the conditions of an alliterative couplet " These are the rules that Guest gives according to which the elementary sections were constructed : 1. " Each couplet of adjacent accents must be separated by one or two syllables which are unaccented, but not by more than two." 2. u No section can have more than three or less than two accents. " 3. ' 'No section can begin or end with more than two unaccented syllables." " When the accents of a section are separated 1 These lines are copied just as they appear in Guest's "His- tory of English Rhythms," p. 189. 2 Guest's "History of English Rhythms," p. 158. RHYTHM. 15 by two unaccented syllables, the rhythm has been called triple measure ; and the common measure, when they are only separated by a single syllable." The greater propor- tional number of accents makes the movement slower, and adapts the measure for more solemn and graver subjects. The triple measure is more suited to lighter themes. The verse of the common measure is made more energetic by be- ing begun and closed with accented syllables. They are abrupt when too short, and become feeble when too long. There was considerable variety of rhythm as early as the fifth century, ' ' as there certainly was in the seventh century, when Csedmon wrote." 1 "It is, however, probable that the rhythms were of a simpler and of a more uniform character. ' ' .... " Most of the alliterative couplets have only four accents — very few, indeed, have so many as six." The phenomenon of accompanying the changes of intensity in a series of sounds with muscular contractions, led to the early association of dancing with musical and poetical recitation. Indeed, if we accept the current theory of the origin of language as arising during the celebrations of victory, dancing precedes even language. Just as an animal jumps and frisks about as an expression of pleasure at seeing his master, so our ancestors danced for joy over a victory, or in the worship of their deity. They emitted cer- tain vocal utterances in company with the tramping of the feet, which in time came to have definite meanings and also took on the rhythm of the dance. This rhythm was scarcely more than the simple swaying of the body or the lifting of one foot and now the other. Variations in the dance might occur either in taking several steps forward and then several backward, or to the right and to the left. These variations would produce corresponding effects in the vocal accompani- ment. The step of one foot would be stronger and a more intense sound made to correspond to it. In the same way either the forward or backward movement would become the more important and give rise to the distinction of thesis and arsis of the verse. Further groupings of the verses might take place in the same way. The two-rhythm was appar- ently the prevailing rhythm in the history of our language, if not in some others. The most common foot in our literature of all times, and a very common foot in the Greek literature, consisted of two syllables ; two feet entered into the section, and two sections formed an alliterated couplet or verse. It is the simplest possible rhythm, and corresponds to the leg- pendulum with which the language was so intimately asso- ciated in its earlier history. 1 Guest's "History of English Rhythms," p. 169. 16 BOLTON : Noire 1 believes that language took its rise in the concerted action of many persons. In this way the individual finds that what belongs to him is the common character of others. Such utterances as "hi-ho" are taken to be the first begin- nings of language, and they originate during concerted action. Any sound that is to become intelligible must first be ex- perienced in company and then by the individual alone. But, as the example shows, such utterances are rhythmical. Here it is the rhythm of heaving sails or anchor, which is seen among sailors. Variations in the number of syllables to the accent would be a necessity as a relief from the monotony of two syllables to the accent, and so, too, the number of accents to the sec- tion would be increased on account of the abruptness of the doubly accented section. Taine 2 in speaking of early Saxon poets says : ' ' His chief care is to abridge, to imprison thought in a kind of mutilated cry." "They (Saxons) do not speak, they sing or rather shout. Each little verse is an acclamation which breaks forth like a growl. Their strong breasts heave with a groan of anger or of enthusiasm. A vehement or indistinct phrase or expression rises suddenly, almost in spite of them, to their lips." After the people be- came settled down in their new homes, they lost the ruder and rougher characteristics, and such wild outpourings would be no longer suited to their milder spirits. The changes that took place in the development of our literature are due in some measure to the change in the life and habits of the people. There still remain in our poetical compositions certain evidences of some, at least, of the stages through which our poetry has passed. The choruses in many of our hymns are still made up of non- sense syllables. Irish melodies and popular songs retain this feature. Children's poetry — by that I refer to such poetry as they enjoy and recite for their own amusement — has a large element of purely unmeaning sounds in it. Savage dances are often accompanied by recitations in which no meaning has been discovered. Again, savages and children are frequently found repeating for their own amusement a series of non- sense syllables in rhythmical form. The accents are very strongly marked, and frequently enforced by alliteration. The incoherent chatter of a maniac, or the sound of a foreigner speaking his language to one who is unacquainted with the language, is distinctly rhythmical. It is more like a chant, and children frequently remark upon 1 Ludwig Noire, " Max Miiller and the Philosophy of Language." 2 Taine, " Introduction to the History of English Language." EHYTHM. 17 it. It appears, then, that vocal utterances which are kept up for a considerable time fall into a rhythmical form. Such being the natural tendency of speech, it would conform itself to any rhythm with which it might be associated, and as vocal utterances were always accompanied by the dance, it would take on the rhythm of the dance, which in its earliest forms we have seen reason to believe was the leg-pendulum. The poetry of children shows a character very similar to early English poetry. It consists often of a two- section verse which is strongly alliterated, and in which the rhythm is perfectly clear. The familiar incantation rhyme shows this characteristic very well. Sticks and stones May break my bones, But names will never hurt me. Again, Jack and Jill Went up the hill To fetch a pail of water; Jack fell down And broke his crown And Jill came tumbling after. At the beginning of each couplet there is wanting one syllable. Their poetry is usually accompanied by marching or by clapping of the hands, so that they require an accented syllable at the beginning. The verse is, then, an alternation of accented and unaccented syllables ; occasionally only two unaccented syllables occur between two accents. We have in the first couplet what was found to be a prominent characteristic of early English poetry. E. B. Taylor in his ' 'Anthropology' ' asserts that while meter, and by that he means lines regularly measured in syllables, is an evidence of civilization, one of its earliest developments is matched and balanced sounds. The Australian savage sings at the end of his verse, ' 'A bang ! A bang ! ' ' Certain of the North American Indians sing in choruses, u N yah eh wa! Nyah eh wa!" The chorus of a New Zealand song is " Ha-ah, ha-ah, ha-ah, ha!" A feature extremely common in barbaric song is a refrain of generally meaningless sylla- bles. Guest, 1 speaking of our early poetry, says, "I have hazarded the opinion that these short, abrupt and forcible rhythms were the earliest that were known to our language. They are such as would naturally be prompted by excited feeling, and well fitted for those lyrical outpourings which form the earliest poetry of all languages." The abruptness Quest's "History of English Rhythms," p. 365. 2 18 BOLTON : is felt by children, so that not more than a single couplet appears without the intervention of a different kind of verse. Shakespeare 1 adopted this measure in his descriptions of fairyland, and it is now become the fairy dialect of the English language. Qualities of Sounds : — Qualities of sounds are quite as important as unifying elements as their time and intensity relations, and were quite as early regarded. This is mani- fest from the frequent recurrence of the same sound at the beginning of Anglo-Saxon and Germanic verses. This is alliteration. The two sections of the verse, while contrasted in intensity, were coordinated by the recurrence of the same sound. The origin of alliteration is involved in some mystery, and yet the savage shouts just quoted point out a possible origin. The emotional shout of an animal for a given state is always the same ; but for the savage, who possesses greater powers of utterance, emotions find various expressions, or at least, if the expression begins with the same sound, it ends differently. Although the New Zealand savage shouts "Ha-ah" several times in succession, he closes with " Ha!" When the child torments his companion in the midst of misfortune, he says " Goody, goody gout." Other expressions of a similar character, but used with a different purpose, are "higelty, pigelty," "hee-ho," etc. In modern poetry alliteration has given place in a very large measure to final rhyme, which has become the unifying factor for the verse generally in English poetry and always in French. The qualities of sounds gave rise to melody in speech, which is common to both poetry and music, and it is as melody that the qualities of sounds play the most impor- tant part. Spencer holds, in his essay upon the origin of music, that different emotional states produce different intonations and changes in pitch, quality and loudness of vocal utterances. In the savage dances of victory, worship, and love, emotional speech grew up, and from this music arose. Originally music was recitative — a mere chant. Chinese aud Hindoo music is still so. This recitative speaking grew " naturally out of the modulations and cadences of strong feeling." The Quaker preacher who speaks only when moved by religious emotion, speaks with a recitative intonation, and church services of the present day are generally read so. This is really melody. Eecitative speaking, or emotional speech, constitutes the whole of savage poetry. Poetry and music among primitive peoples were the same. Poetry was either sung or chanted, and it was not until a 1 Guest's "History of English Rhythms," p. 179. RHYTHM. 19 later period that they became separated. With the discovery of the musical instrument, the people saw that a melody was just as well expressed by simple tones as by intelligible syllables, and music took up its own lines of development. The Emotional Effects of Rhythm upon Savages and Children ; — There is no more striking fact in the whole field of rhythm than the emotional effect which rhythms produce upon certain classes of people, savages and children. Atten- tion has already been called to the psychological phenomenon of accompanying the changes of intensity in a series of sounds by muscular movements. So strong is this impulse in all classes of people that no one is able to listen to music in which the rhythm is strong and clear without making some kind of muscular movements. With some people these movements tend to increase in force until the whole body becomes involved and moves with the rhythm. The accents in the rhythm have the effect of summated stimuli, and the excitement may increase even to a state of ecstasy and catalepsy. Although the regular recurrence of the ac- cented syllable is the most important element, the qualita- tive changes aid in bringing about the emotional states. Soothing effects result from certain rhythms, as is shown in the lulling and patting of the baby to sleep. The early hypnotizers resorted to the gentle stroking of their subjects. Savages are well aware of the exciting effects of certain rhythms, and are accustomed to use them to bring about the state of frenzy in which their priests give their prophecies and in which religious dances are danced. Mr. Ellis, J who has made a study of some tribes in Africa, says, " Music amongst the Thsi-speaking tribes is limited to airs possess- ing an obvious rhythm. Such airs seem to appeal to the primitive sense common to all people, but upon savages, that is, upon children with the possession and power of men, its influence is immense, and the state of excitement into which an assemblage of uncivilized people may be wrought by the mere rhythm of drums and the repetition of a simple melody would hardly be credited With some races this known emotional influence of music has been utilized with three objects, viz., to stimulate the religious sentiments, the mar- tial spirit, and the sexual passions." In the Yatiati 2 dance among the Indians of British Colum- bia, the tribe assembles outside of the chief's house in which the dance is to be held, and with fists and sticks they beat the time on the walls as they enter, singing the dancing 1 A. B. Ellis, "The Thsi-Speaking Peoples of the Gold Coast of West Africa," p. 325. 3 Franz Boas, Jour, of Amer. Folk-lore, Vol. I. p. 49. 20 BOLTON : song. The dancers who are on the inside are worked up into a frenzy. The gentle striking at first, gradually increas- ing in violence, and the slow approach and the assemblage of the tribe, wrought in the dancers a pitch of excitement which forced them to rush out after a time and begin the dance, jumping about in the wildest fashion. Such dances cease only with the complete exhaustion of the dancers. The Patagonian wizard x begins his performance with drum- ming and rattling, and keeps it up till the real or pretended epileptic fit comes on by a demon entering him. Among the wild Yeddas of Ceylon the devil dancers have to work them- selves into paroxysms to gain the inspiration whereby they profess to cure their patients. With the furious dancing to music and the chanting of attendants, the Bodo priests bring on a fit of maniacal possession. The excitement is allowed to continue until the prophet falls to the ground in a swoon. When the Alfurus of the Celebes invite their deity to descend among them, the priests, standing about the chief priest, upon whom the deity is to descend, chant some legends. A slight twitching of the limbs marks the beginning of the possession. The priest turns his face towards heaven, the spirit descends upon him, and with terrible gestures he springs upon a board and beats about with a bundle of leaves, and leaps and dances, chanting some legends. He falls in a swoon, and the sounds he emits are interpreted as the will of the spirit. George Catlin 2 says dancing is always accompanied by the singing of mysterious songs and chants, which are perfectly measured and sung in exact time to the beat of the drum, always with an invariable set of sounds and expressions. The religious services and singing among the Shakers are often accompanied by dancing, and more frequently by beat- ing of the time by all the members of the congregation. The excitement among them never rises to an extreme degree. A highly civilized people is not easily affected by mere rhythms. A simple tone is not so expressive as it is to the lower classes of people. The negro preacher often resorts to reci- tative speaking to produce the desired emotional state in his hearers, which is generally known as the " power." He selects some short sentence, often unimportant, such as " Moses went up into the mountain," and repeating this, at first softly, he gradually raises his voice to the highest pitch, at the same time increasing his gesticulations. The more ex- citable of his audience are thrown into a paroxysm ; the con- 1 E. B. Taylor, " Primitive Culture." 2 George Catlin, " Letters and Notes upon the Manners and Cus- toms of North American Indians." EHYTHM. 21 tagion spreads so that sometimes the whole audience is in- volved. Evangelists among all classes of people rely more or less upon the emotional effect of rhythmical speaking. Street hawkers and fakirs generally speak with a recitative intonation. Their success depends very largely upon their success in alluring and holding the attention of the crowd by the manner and intonation with which they speak. The effect of rhythm and clearly accented music is no greater upon primitive peoples than upon children. Al- though children are not allowed to go into ecstasies, the clapping of the hands to the recitation of u Peas porridge hot" is akin to the terrible leaping and gesticulations of the savage to the accompanying tom-tom and the chanting of his ancient legends. The child usually begins his recitation of " Peas porridge hot " rather slowly, and as he continues he grows in excitement and enthusiasm, his gestures become more violent and rapid, until he breaks down in the excite- ment. It is a well-known fact among school teachers that young children become excited whenever they sing rhymes with a strongly accented rhythm. Several have made this observation during the singing of a certain line in Theodore Tilton's "Baby Bye." The line in which the excitement reaches its climax is, There he goes On his toes Tickling baby's nose. This is a type of the fairy measure. The accents are strong, and every line is preceded by a pause, and at the same time all the lines are rhymed. Both the rhyme and the pause lend an intensification to the rhythm that is sufficient to call out the greatest excitement in the fairy people. In Bobert Browning's poem of the " Pied Piper of Hamlin," whose charm was rhythm, occurs this remarkably rhythmical passage, and taken with the context might easily cause some emotional excitement : Into the streets the piper stept, Smiling at first a little smile, As if he knew what magic slept In his quiet pipe the while. I have the testimony of an eminent educator that, when he read these lines, and he is an effective reader, his boy, a youngster of five or six years, would run away and hide where he could not hear the reading. He was apparently unable to bear the strain of the excitement. In later years the boy could not tell why he did so, except that it disturbed him. 22 BOLTON : The use by children of incantation rhymes for purposes of injury and torment to their companions is interesting in this connection. The habit of rhyming is almost instinctive with them. Imagine the effect of such a couplet as this upon the child to whom it is addressed : Good night, Rosie Wright. Again, any name may be put in certain adaptive rhymes which are current among children. These, however, are not so effective as the instance cited above. They admit of retort. The drawling out of a name in a sing-song measured tone is very effective, and the easy adaptation of some names makes the child who is unfortunate in having such a name an object of torment. The Place of Rhythm in Music and Poetry. Music : — We have seen how music and poetry took their rise together from the emotional utterances of savages during the dance, and how these emotional utterances gradually took the form of recitative speaking. This gave rise to the melody, though it was not disassociated from the meaning of the words. With the discovery of the musical instrument came the discovery that a melody might be sustained by simple tone intensities. Although music finds its essential basis in rhythm, its distinctive feature is the melody combined with harmony. The melody is constituted of a succession of tones which are significant of an emotional state, and when several melodies are combined and sung together, they give rise to harmony. This combination of melodies depends upon the pitch of the sounds. The melodies in harmony are all subordinated in different degrees to one dominant melody which is higher in pitch than the others. The unifying ele- ment here is pitch. This is the only distinctive use that is made of it in either music or poetry. The most important and fundamental unifying principles underlying music is the time, without which there can be no music. Musical tones must be exactly timed, if one is to get the conception of a melody from a series of tones. When they are exactly timed they may be farther unified by regular changes of intensity which group the sounds into measures. The most common measures that occur in music are 2-4, 3-4, 4-4, and 6-8 time. In what might be termed the natural system of accents, the first note in each measure receives a strong accent. This is really the only accent in 2-4 time. In 3-4 time the second note also receives an accent, but it is weaker than the first. In 4-4 time there are four grades of intensity. The first note is the strongest, the third next, the second is weaker still, RHYTHM. 23 and the fourth is the weakest of all. In 6-8 time the third, fifth and sixth are of about equal intensity, and weak. The first is strongest, the fourth is next, and the second weaker though stronger than the third. An equal amount of time is given to each measure — that is, the strong accent occurs at regular intervals — but the distribution of this time among the notes in a measure may be greatly varied ; the separate notes, however, always bearing constant and simple relations to one another. The smallest fraction that may express the relations of these notes is 1-64, and this appears only in instrumental music. In poetry, as we shall see, there is not so much freedom ; it has deviated less from the primal rhythmic stock from which both spring. For many centuries music consisted wholly of melodies, or of a single melody. The idea of combining or singing several melodies at the same time came very much later. This is harmony. It reached its highest development about Elizabeth's time, when the at- tempt was made to combine as many as forty melodies. A much smaller number was found to give better effect, and the number now used is generally only four. Symphony was a still later development, but the general feeling among mu- sicians now is that it culminated in Beethoven, and its further development in music is impossible. Although the term has had several significations in the history of music, in Beethoven it was the combination of several themes in such a way as to bring about a succession and combination of strong emotional states. The musician who desires now to produce new effects, turns to the Volks-Lieder for a theme. He aims at variations of the rhythmical effects and introduces new harmonies. Mendelssohn is said to have remarked, when he heard some of the negro melodies of our slaves, that here was a field for a great musical talent. Wagner, taking the suggestion, has made such an adaptation of the Hunga- rian melodies, and with what success the musical world is well aware. Wagner has made a real advance, and for some time musical composition will follow his lead. Although there is a feeling among musicians that rhythm is distasteful, it is more apparent than real. It is the regular monotonous recurrence of the same rhythm without sufficient variations that is displeasing and not the rhythmic flow itself. Rhythms in Poetry: — We have already seen that when language appeared as literature, it took the form of the simplest possible rhythm. Even then it was the vocal accompaniment of a dance, and there are many analogies to the simple swaying of the body or the tramping of the feet in the march. There were no fixed rules in regard to the number of syllables to the measure. The verse, so far as we can 24 BOLTON : speak of a verse, consisted of an alternation of accented and unaccented syllables. Very generally it began and ended with an accented syllable, so that a pause occurred between each verse. The line of development along which poetry followed was an increase in the number of unaccented sylla- bles as compared with the accented, and also an increase in the number of accents to the verse ; the verse preserving for some time the same balance of structure that it had in the beginning. The number of accents then might be four, six or eight; the latter number never became popular, for the reason, it would seem, that it exceeded the normal mental span. This even and balanced structure could not hold out forever ; a demand for variety and the influence of foreign rhythms con- tributed to overthrow it, so that Chaucer wrote altogether in a verse of five accents, but he still retained the middle pause. This came after the second accent or just before the third, though sometimes after the third also. There were many verses in which the first section more generally contained three accents. Guest takes no account of the measures or feet in English verse. He divides lines into three general classes : Those that begin with an accented syllable, those that begin with one unaccented syllable, and those that begin with two unac- cented syllables. The varieties in each of these classes depend upon the position where the variation occurs from the form in which the verse sets out. Should the verse begin with an accented syllable and continue with an alternation of accented and unaccented syllables, it would constitute one variety. If, however, two unaccented syllables occur between any two accents, it would constitute a different variety accord- ing as the two unaccented syllables occur between the first and second accents, the second and third, and so on through the verse. Early poetry was sung to the accompaniment of the harp and hence was sung in exact time. On this account Guest says that up to the fourth century, English rhythms were temporal and then became accentual. Previous to that time the syllable had a time value. This, however, is not to be taken in any absolute sense. Poetry was chanted in a. kind of trance state, and the reciter aimed to produce such a state in his audience. For this purpose the thought was of minor importance. Great dependence was placed upon the rhythmical flow, and doubtless a very exact time was given to the syllables that the movement might be clearer. A rhythm which depends wholly upon either the time element or the accent, is certainly less forcible than one which combines both factors. It must be conceded that though some regard was paid to the time of syllables, no such exact time was main- RHYTHM. 25 tained as modern musicians keep in their music. Perfect time is the result of the application of scientific methods to music. Poetry has never lost the time element entirely, for accents that occur at irregular intervals could not have been but very displeasing, and they are now. It is reported of some of our modern poets, and especially of Tennyson, that they read their poems with the strictest observance, not only of the ac- cents, but of the time, showing that they regarded the time ele- ment of great importance. Many readers and teachers of English poetry pay little heed to the regular recurrence of the accent. For them the thought is the chief element in poetry, and in attempting to bring that out, they disregard the rhythmi- cal flow. But when the proper observance of the thought does violence to the rhythm, the poet must be adjudged lacking poetic inspiration, and to that extent his poetry is not true poetry. It is to the great renown of Chaucer, Milton and Shakespeare that there is such a perfect adaptation of the rhythm to the theme in hand, and any lack of observance of the accents by the reader betrays his want of understanding of that which he reads. The strict observance of time in music and the unity of origin of poetry and music, which argues that time was once an essential element of poetry, show that the time element is still there, unless it can be shown when and why it has dropped out. Poetry has admitted fewer variations and allows a greater prominence to the rhythmical flow than music. It must be admitted, however, that the thought has taken the place of the melody to a great extent as the unifying element, but it cannot be allowed to take the place of other factors. Whenever it does, just so soon the composition fails of being in any sense poetry. Alliteration, which was very prominent in Anglo-Saxon, was gradually lost. The influence of the church and of Latin scholarship aided somewhat in this movement, but as the Anglo-Saxon element prevailed against all foreign influences in the political and social affairs, it won the day in the struggle against the Norman and Latin languages. Our lan- guage remains essentially Anglo-Saxon, and alliteration, though less common, is still a prominent feature of our poetry. Originally, alliterated syllables marked the beginning of the section and constituted the unifying factor of it, but there was no strict observance of such a principle, except that the alliterated syllables were accented. They might come anywhere within the section. The use of alliteration by later English poets was to place the alliterated syllables away from the beginning of the section and to put them in the same verse. The purpose of alliteration is not to coordinate two sections •or two lines, but, by intensifying certain accents in the verse, 26 BOLTON : to make a more perfect subordination of them, or to make a more perfect unity of the line. Final rhyme succeeded allit- eration. The chief reason seems to have been for a more emphatic or distinguishing mark of the rhythm than could be obtained through accents alone; especially when run-on lines came to be used and the thought was about to usurp everything. When two successive sentences or words begin with the same sound, it interferes with the understanding of them. Both the reader and hearer are more likely to con- found them. For this reason alliteration must give way r except for purposes of emphasis, when the thought becomes of the first importance. Simple intensities are not sufficient as unifying factors ; they cannot be properly subordinated to give unity to the line. It is interesting to note how the change from alliteration to rhythm has come about. In the early poet- ry, the alliterated syllables came at the beginning of the verse, but in modern poetry the rhymed syllables, which are their successors, come at the end. We shall see later how the begin- ning and end of rhythmical groups run into one another and become indistinguishable. The same is to be observed with reference to the feet. The accents in the feet become transposed. Although it seems probable that the foot in early poetry and the measure in all music began with the accented sound, the accented syllable in English poetry is more generally the last, and in Latin and Greek poetry it was quite as frequently the last as the first. The series of accented syllables in the verse and of articulate sounds in the foot seem to appear as a series of stimuli which are to be summated. The two sections of the verse in old English were made to rhyme with their last syllables, and were then written as two verses. Two such couplets together form the most common stanza in English poetry. Instead of writing the members of each couplet next to each other, they are made more fre- quently and quite generally to alternate. JEsthetic Forms : — That which binds the four verses into a stanza is not wholly the interelation and balance of the two rhyming couplets. The members of the two couplets are fre- quently made to begin, the one with an accented syllable and the other with an unaccented syllable. Sometimes this, and sometimes a less number of accented syllables, make the lengths of the alternate lines less — a fact that gives artistic form to the verse when it is properly printed. It becomes then an appeal to the eye as an aesthetically beautiful form. This principle was seized upon by our poets during the six- teenth century, and carried to an extreme as regards form alone, which could not be sustained by the thought. The RHYTHM. 27 poem had nothing but form. The principle of form becoming a unifying factor for a poem is perfectly true, and effective use is made of it in modern poetry. Among the older poets, George Herbert introduced many novelties into the forms of stanza. He relied upon both rhymes and artistic forms. Some of his stanzas take the form of a vase, an hour-glass, a pyramid and an inverted cone. Although they read smoothly, one cannot help but feel that his attempt at aesthetic forms has destroyed the beauty of the poems. The sonnet 1 is probably the most organic of all poems. While the theme is very essential in binding the whole together, the lines are coordinated in the most intricate way by rhymes. A rhyme- scheme runs through the whole, which, when represented by letters, or dots of different sizes, or lines of different lengths, forms an artistic group, obeying the laws of principality, subordination, etc. The number of accents to the line is varied in some cases in such a way that it lends a kind of subordination of some lines to others, or of all to one or two. Theme : — Little or no regard is paid to the thought in a poetical recitation by children or by primitive peoples. They delight in the emotional effect of sounds properly measured and balanced. With the growth of literature the thought has gradually become more important until it is about to usurp everything. The unity of the stanza and of the verse very generally depends upon it. Higher unities of the stanza — poems — depend entirely upon the theme. The attempts to coordinate stanzas by rhyming their last lines have not proved a great success. The strength of the connection is often lost. If the thought in the verse or stanza is allowed to become the prevailing element, the poetry becomes measured prose. Poetry arose in a kind of trance or highly emotional state, and for centuries it was used to produce such states in others. The whole structure is calculated to produce emotion, and for that reason it cannot easily become the medium of expression for the intellect. There must be a mutual dependence between the thought and the form, or they result in mutual destruction. Under the influence of the church and Latin scholarship, English poetry became, or at least the attempt was made to conform it to certain rules of Latin prosody. English critics, misunderstanding probably both English and Latin poetry, tried to make the former conform to the rules of the latter. And there are many persons now who cannot see 1 The reader is referred to Prof. Corson's " Primer of English Verse" for a treatment of the stanza and sonnet. 28 BOLTON : why the rules of Latin prosody are not universal. A verse beginning with an accented syllable and consisting of an alternation of accented and unaccented syllables, was trochaic measure, and the accented syllable was double the length of the unaccented. If two unaccented syllables were used be- tween two accents, it was the substitution of a dactyl for a trochee. In this case, if the syllables preserved their proper time values according to the Latin prosody, four time- units — the dactyl — would appear in the place of three — the trochee. This, however, did not strike the critics as forming a defect in the rhythm, and the error has gone on. It is the current view among respectable English authorities to-day. In order not to keep the reader in suspense about so disputed and important a point, let me say what seems to be the true view. As the simplest time-unit of Greek poetry was a short syllable, and whatever value in time was given to it in a verse, that value must be maintained throughout, so the simplest unit of English poetry is the time between two accents — the foot is the simplest unit in the verse — and this must be constant. The time is apportioned among the syllables that are present between the accents, whatever the number. From the very nature of the accent the syllable receiving it will be longer relatively, though it does not bear a constant and simple relation to the length of the unaccented syllables. "Besides the increase 1 of loudness and the sharper tone which distinguishes the accented syllable, there is also a tendency to dwell upon it, or, in other words, to lengthen the quantity. We cannot increase the loudness or the sharpness of the tone without a certain degree of muscu- lar action ; and to put muscular action into motion requires time." Another fact which has been greatly overlooked in the study of English rhythms, and which has led to much con- fusion and erroneous speculation and criticism of some poets, is the sectional pause, which allows two accented syllables to stand together in the verse. It was very common in Anglo- Saxon poetry, and disappeared almost entirely under the influences spoken of above. Shakespeare made free use of it, and for a lack of this knowledge, critics assert that he made use of false accents. Csedmon placed it before words upon which he desired to have a strong emphasis. It occurs be- fore names of the deity. Guest says it owes its existence to the "emphatic stop," and is really the greatest departure from the rules of accent, which were observed with much care by the Saxon poets. It has been revived by more 1 Guest's " History of English Khythm," p. 75. RHYTHM. 29 recent poets, and effective use is made of it. The value of this pause is the great emphasis it lends to the word follow- ing, and my purpose in dwelling upon it now is that it will come up again prominently in the experimental study. Another question which connects itself very closely with this point of accent and pauses, is the foot or measure division of the line. Guest does not recognize such a division as the foot. The line is an alternation of accented and unaccented syllables, and he does not mention the fact of these forming groups which in Greek prosody were called feet and in music are termed measures. There seems to be no question that readers do make such groups by placing a slight pause either after or before the accented syllables. The Greeks associated these groups with a complete step in the march, and since in matters of aesthetics it is a rash thing to dispute or deny the accuracy of their judgments, we must regard the foot as a real division of their verse and inquire whether the lack of quantity in English syllables has any- thing to do with the absence of the foot division. The English verse is made up of a series of syllables in which every other one is uttered with greater intensity than the rest. The accented syllable requires more time, and the unaccented syllable unites or fuses with it into an organic group. These groups are then apparently separated by pauses. In French poetry there are no accented syllables, and the foot division is not recognized at all. This gives English poetry a kind of intermediate position between Greek and French poetry. The question of a foot division cannot be finally answered from an examination of our poetry, except as has already been said, such divisions are invariably made. The question will find its final answer in the experimental investigation. Another problem which follows closely upon this is, what is the inherent nature of a group in a rhythmical series, or, what is the relation of the different syllables to one another in the poetical foot, and what determines the length of it? The length of sentence 1 in prose is found not to deviate long from an average. Long sentences may prevail in an author for a few pages, but they are sure to be followed by short ones in sufficient number to balance the long ones. There appears a kind of rhythm in which long and short sentences succeed one another. This rhythm is constant for the same author ; his earlier and later writings show no difference in the length of sentences. The writers of the more ancient prose show a greater average length of sentence than our more recent writers. 1 L. A. Schurmann. University Studies, Nebraska University, Vol. I. 30 BOLTON : There have been several attempts in late years to con- struct philosophies of English verse. Several of these will be taken up and their more salient features presented. The purpose is not to give a complete review of the books, but to call attention to a few facts which will supplement the work that has gone before. The Science of English Verse by Sidney Lanier: — A simple auditory impression recurring at regular intervals of time furnishes the essential conditions of a rhythm. Of the four properties of sounds — duration, intensity, pitch, and tone- color — the mind can and does form exact coordinations of duration, pitch, and tone-color ; intensities cannot be com- pared with exactness. The regular recurrence of sounds and silences constitutes primary rhythm, and a grouping of these sounds by means of intensity, pitch, or tone-color, consti- tutes secondary rhythm — the bar in music and the foot in poetry. For purposes of verse, syllables correspond to sounds and bear relations to one another in point of time, which are expressed by the simple numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, etc. The regularly recurring syllables of a sentence, whether prose or poetry, constitute a primary rhythm, " which the rhythmic sense of man tends to mould into a more definite, more strongly marked and more complex form, that may be called secondary rhythm. " ' < The tendency to arrange any primary units of rhythm into groups, or secondary units of rhythm, is so strong in ordinary persons that the imagination will even affect such a grouping when the sounds themselves do not present means for doing it." Accent simply arranges the ' ' materials already rhythmical through some temporal recurrence." As the comprehension of a series of sounds is rendered more easy by grouping, so the compre- hension of a series of these groups is rendered more easy by again grouping these groups into tertiary rhythms. Allitera- tion, the recurrence of emphatic words and punctuation marks signify the tertiary group. The fourth order of rhyth- mical grouping is the line which, except in the case of run- on lines, completes a logical division of the sentence. Lines are again grouped into couplets by tone-color coordinations. The fifth order of rhythmical grouping is the stanza, and a complete poem is spoken of as the sixth order. The effort of the author, in his treatment of the foot, is to make the rhythmical accent and grouping correspond to the logical accent and meaning. For this purpose he treats at length the iambic foot, it being the most common in English poetry. Making use of musical terms, this foot is equivalent to three eighth notes, and its typic form is one eighth note RHYTHM. 31 iollowed by a quarter note. Instead of the eighth note, the foot may contain two sixteenth notes, and instead of a quarter note, there may be two eighth notes, or a dotted eighth and a sixteenth. The foot may also contain three syllables, each being equivalent to one eighth note, or four syllables, but the four must be read in the time of three eighth notes. In the place of any note, may be substituted a rest of equal length. An anapaest or dactyl cannot take the place of ar iambic or trochaic foot, since the former are equivalent to f oui time-units and the latter to three. He says there are two kinds of rhythm only — 3-rhythm and 4-rhythm. All other kinds resolve themselves into these two ; 2-rhythm is really 4-rhythm, and 5-rhythm is equivalent to a 3-rhythm and a 2-rhythm combined. A Primer of English Verse by Hiram Corson ; — The object of verse to him is "the expression of impassioned and spiritualized thought. ' ' It originates in ' ' the unify- ing activity of feeling and emotion." Upon whatever objects " feeling 1 or emotion is projected, or with what it is incorporated — it is unifying. " u The insulated intellect, in its action, tends in an opposite direction — that is, in an analytic direction. When feeling is embodied in speech, that speech is worked up ... . into unities of various kinds." The primal unity is the foot, which is combined u in a still higher unity which is called the verse, and this in turn is combined into a still higher unity, which is called the stanza." " Ehythm is a succession and involution of unities, that is, unities within unities." It applies to a succession of either feet, verses or stanzas. Each class of unities has its combining principles ; that of the foot is accent. Melody is the combining principle for the syllables. Alliteration is a common and effective form of consonantal melody. The combining agencies of the stanza are harmony and rhyme. Individual verses may be melodious, but when several are taken together they lack harmony. Ehyme is also an enforcing agency of the individ- ual verse, and the emphasis resulting is neutralized in pro- portion as the verses are separated. Blank verse depends u upon the melodious movement of the individual verses, pause melody, and the general harmony or toning." Varia- tions of the theme-meter produce important effects. "The feelings of the reader of English poetry get to be set, so to speak, to the pentameter measure, as in that measure the 1 This is the author's great mistake. No such distinction can be drawn between feeling as unifying and intellect as analytic. Both analysis and synthesis are equally properties of the intellect, and it is difficult to conceive how the feelings can accomplish a synthe- sis or unify anything. 32 BOLTON : largest portion of English poetry is written." The intro- duction of any other than the theme-meter gives an emphasis to the thought. The substitution of a different foot gives a variety ' ' which is essential to harmony. ' ' The shifting of the regular accent gives a special enforcement, either logical or aesthetic. " There should never be a non- significant departure from a pure monotony. ' ' Rationale of English Verse by E. A. Poe: — Verse originates with the human enjoyment of equality. Unpracticed ears ap- preciate simple equalities. Practiced ears appreciate equalities among equalities ; they are able to compare two sets of equals. The rudiments of all verse may possibly be found in the spon- dee. In this, the mind finds its first pleasure in the equality of two accented syllables. A collection of two spondees — two words of two equal syllables — forms the second step in the development of the verse. A third step would be found in the juxtaposition of three words. This, however, gives the idea of a monotone, a relief from which is found in words of different accents — iambics. A dactyl might be employed as a further relief from the monotone. A sequence of words of any sort would form a monotone, if they were not curtailed or defined within certain limits. This gave rise to the lines, the terminations of which are again determined by equalities in length, and marked by equalities — likenesses in sound. Every foot in the same verse requires equal time. A three syllable word may appear as iambic or trochee, providing that two syllables can be read in the time of one. Blending is an unwarranted liberty. He states this general principle : The substitution of a foot, the sum of whose syllabic times is equal to the sum of the syllabic times of the foot substituted, is allowed with this restriction only, that the regular foot shall continue long enough or be sufficiently prominent to leave no doubt of the kind of verse. He says " that rhythm is erroneous, which any ordinary reader can, without design, read improperly. ' ' The real test of the perfection of a verse is the pleasurable feeling it yields. Classical Poetry: — Classical Greek poetry was either chanted or sung, and for that reason was exactly timed. There was really no difference between a poetical recitation and a song. The simplest elements in the measure, accord- ing to which poetry was sung, was a time-unit equivalent to one eighth note. By combining these time-units into groups, they formed the measure or foot. A group of several feet constituted the section, and two sections entered into the line, a certain number of which were united into strophes or stanzas. A time value was given to all syllables and words RHYTHM. 33 in the language ; they were either long or short. A short syllable was equivalent to one time-unit, and a long to two. Various measures were employed. They might be equal to two, three, four, five or six time-units. The most common measures contain three or four time-units. The three time- unit foot most generally contains two syllables, one long and one short, or one short and one long. The four time-unit foot contains two or three syllables, generally two long, or one long and two short, or two short and one long. "When only one long syllable occurred in the foot, it received an accent ; when there were two in the foot, the first received the accent. The accented portion stood as the thesis, and the unaccented as the arsis. In the same way the two sec- tions of the verse stood as thesis and arsis. The thesis came first. The middle pause did not usually divide the verse into two equal divisions. The first was the shorter, the pause coming within the third foot. Except as showing a perfect subordination to a chief accent, and a slight anacrusis at the close, the verses had no distinguishing marks ; they were not rhymed, and very rarely alliterated. The number of feet in a verse varied with different kinds of poetry, two being the smallest and six the greatest. The kind of foot with which the measure set out was not always maintained. Any other foot agreeing with the theme-foot in position of accent, and in the number of time- units, might be substituted. As such agreements in the kind of feet were few, there could be very little variety in the verse. Greek poetry was not allowed to develop long untram- meled by rules. A rigid philosophical system was imposed upon it, and all future poetry was made to conform to this system. But it would be difficult to say that Greek poetry suffered from the restriction. It prevented novelty for novelty's sake, but allowed great freedom where freedom was most needed. There are several facts in the history of rhythm that are interesting, both for the subject in hand and for psychology in general. Soon after the idea of varying the number of syllables in a foot had become known, and its effects appre- ciated, there arose a kind of mania for verses which contained a variety of feet. They were characterized as "tumbling verses ' 7 from the peculiar effect they gave rise to. This was a discordant and unpleasurable feeling. There was really no rhythm to them, and they never became popular. The same took place in regard to the length of line. Various novelties were introduced, when a longer line than that of the earliest poetry was found more pleasing and less abrupt. Verses of 3 34 BOLTON : six and seven accents were tried , and verses containing two sections, each of which was an alliterated couplet, having four or six accents, appear in some authors. No new com- bining agency was employed, and probably for that reason the verses exceeded the mental span. Had the older poets grasped the principles of unifying their lines by rhyme, or by proper subordination of the sections, they might have made such long verses a success. In the same line were the attempts at aesthetic forms, which have already been spoken of. Experimental Investigation. HI This work was undertaken with several objects in view. The first and most important object was to determine what the mind did with a series of simple auditory impressions in which there was absolutely no change of intensity, pitch, quality or time-interval. Each separate impression was to be indistinguishable from any or all the others. Eegular variations with respect to the intensity or time-interval of the sounds in this series, which will be called a rhythmic series, were then to be tried separately and together, with the purpose of determining what values these properties of sound have in forming a rhythmical series — that is, a series of groups of impressions — out of a rhythmic series. It was seen at the outset that it would be practically impossible with the apparatus at our disposal to employ pitch variations, and for that reason no attempts were made with variations in pitch. Variations in quality or tone-color were contemplated, but the experiment was not carried out, first on account of a lack of time, and secondly of proper apparatus. The results of the first experiment anticipated much that was to be tried in the later experiments. As the work progressed, new problems were suggested for investigation until the narrow limits within which the work was begun were greatly over- stepped. These problems will be taken up in what seems to be their proper order, and the results presented. Apparatus : — The click of an electric telephone when con- nected in an induction circuit is constant in intensity, pitch and quality, when breaks occur in the primary circuit, pro- viding the primary circuit is constant. The click is not the same in intensity when the primary circuit is made as it is when the primary circuit is broken. For this reason, the sound at the break only could be utilized. It is perfectly constant and stronger in intensity than the click at the make. It varies directly in intensity with variations in the strength RHYTHM. 35 of the current and changes slightly in pitch and quality with variations of intensity, but the pitch and quality are always the same with the same intensity of current. A break at regular intervals in the primary circuit, when the secondary circuit is closed, the secondary circuit being open when the primary was closed, was all that was necessary to furnish the required series of auditory impressions with which the investigation might begin. A chronograph after the pattern devised by Wundt 1 and FlGUBE I. built by C. Krille, furnished a constant power. Figure I« gives a general view of the whole apparatus as it was used in this experimental investigation. PThe drum- shaft was slipped off the drum and five arms, two and one-half inches long were put upon it by passing the shaft through a hole near one end. Each arm was provided with a set screw, that the arm might be held in position and Js^This apparatus will be found fully described in the second volume of Wundt's Physiologische Psychologic, p. 279. 3d ed. 36 BOLTON its position changed at will. They were set at equal distances apart along the shaft, and their points separated by 72 degrees, so that the space about the shaft was divided equally into five divisions. (See Figure II.) Corresponding to Figure II. H Figure II. shows the operations of the keys. A. Drum- shaft. B. The wooden arm on the drum-shaft. C. The dotted line represents the following point of the arm. D. The continuous line represents the leading point. E. The dotted line indicates the position of the arm when the key is pressed down. F. The key bearing the platinum points, which project below and are connected by the wire indicated by K. The dotted line below shows the position of the key when the platinum points dip in the mercury. H. The rubber elastic which caused the key to react. I. The rod upon which the key turned. L. The mercury cup. M. The wire connections. each arm were two keys placed in such a position on the top of the chronograph that as the shaft revolved the ends of the arms came in contact with the ends of the keys and pressed them downward about half an inch to allow the arm to pass by in its revolution. The keys, which were ten in number, two to each arm, were made of strips of wood, six inches long and a half inch wide, and hinged horizontally upon a steel rod two inches from one end in such a way that the ends might move up and down. To the short ends were attached elastics, which caused the long ends with which the arms came in contact to rise up after they had been released by the arms on the drum- shaft. They were prevented from rising up too far by a piece of wood placed above them. Each arm RHYTHM. 37 bore two points, the one about an inch to one side and ten degrees in advance of the other. The leading point came in contact with one key and pressed it down in advance of the other. As each point was broad, covering about twenty degrees of the circle described by the end of the arm, the first key would remain down until after the other had been pressed down. As both points upon each arm were of the same width, the key first pressed down would be released before the other. Near the long end, each key carried two platinum points which projected downward below the key, and which were connected at the upper ends by a wire. When the keys came down, the platinum points dipped into cups of mercury, which rested upon the top of the chronograph. ( See Figure II. ) These mercury cups were made by boring holes into the side Figure III. Figure III. shows the electrical connections. A. The strip of hard rubber. B. The battery. C. The mercury cups. E. 1, 2, 3, 4, 5. The primary coils. The double rings about 1 represent the induction coil. K. The key-board. The primary circuit is represented by light lines indicated by Y, and the secondary circuit by a heavy line, X. T. The telephone. 38 BOLTON : of a strip of hard rubber, five- eighths of an inch thick. Holes were drilled into the edge of the rubber opposite the mercury cups and copper wires inserted, which were connected with the battery and induction coils in the manner which is schematized in Figure III. Beginning at the left hand end (marked "L") of the hard rubber strip, the first pair of opposite cups and each alternate pair along the strip were connected with a coil of wire on one side, and with the battery on the other. For purposes to be described later, were five coils of wire which might be connected with these mercury cups. The coil and the battery were connected, thus completing the primary circuit. The other pairs of opposite cups which alternated with these were all connected together on the one side with an induction coil, and on the other with the tele- phone. The induction coil and the telephone were joined, thus completing the secondary circuit. The ten keys corre- sponded to the ten pairs of mercury cups. When the first key at the left hand, and each alternate key thereafter, was pressed down by the arms on the drum- shaft so that the platinum points dipped into the mercury, it would close the primary circuit, for these keys joined the opposite mercury cups which were connected with the battery. When the second key at the left hand, and each alternate key there- after was pressed down, it would close the induction cir- cuit. Key 1 at the left hand end of the strip of hard rubber matched the first pair of opposite cups of mercury and was paired with key 2, which matched the second pair of the opposite cups of mercury. These first two keys were operated by the first arm at the left hand end of the drum- shaft. The other four pairs of keys were operated by the other four arms on the drum- shaft. Let us consider now only the first pair of keys and the first arm at the left. As the shaft revolves, the point of the arm which was in advance of the other was made to come in contact with the long end of key 1, and pressed it down. After coming in contact with key 1, the point of the arm could move through an arc of ten degrees, keeping the platinum points in the mercury, and thus closing the primary circuit, before the second point of the same arm would come in contact with key 2. When the keys were pressed down sufficiently to make the cir- cuit, the points of the arm were made to slide by the ends of the keys in such a way that the key was not released until the arm had moved through an arc of twenty degrees. A further revolution of ten degrees by the arm would press key 2 down sufficiently to close the secondary circuit. If, now, the arm continues to revolve, key 1 would be released and rise up, breaking the primary circuit, but key 2 would RHYTHM. 39 remain down while the arm moved through an arc of ten degrees, keeping the secondary circuit closed for a time after the primary circuit was broken. This would give a sound in the telephone. The same process would be repeated with each of the five pairs of keys and their corresponding arms. If, now, the arms were set at an equal number of degrees apart and the drum- shaft were made to revolve at a uniform rate, the clicks in the telephone would be separated by equal inter- vals of time, and not varying in intensity, pitch or quality, these clicks would form the required series of auditory impressions. If a change in intensity is desired, as it was, the five wires connecting the different pairs of mercury cups might each be connected with the five different coils which were referred to above. These were set at different distances from the induction coil (see Figure III.). As the different primary coils were of the same size, the strength of the induced current, and therefore the intensity of the sound, would depend upon the distance at which the primary coils were placed from the induction coil. They were placed at just sufficient distance apart to make the sounds easily dis- tinguishable from one another in a graded series of intensities. By means of the key-board (marked "K") it was possible to connect all the five wires in any way that was desired with the five primary coils. The clicks might all be of the same intensity, all different, or of two, three or four different inten- sities. Whatever the variation, according to this arrange- ment it would recur every fifth click. When variations every fourth or third were desired, three or four arms were set upon the drum-shaft and only three or four pairs of keys operated. If the arms were separated by an equal number of degrees, the series of clicks would still be regular. Two kinds of arms were employed, those with a single end and those with a double end. (Figure II. represents the double ended arm.) By using both single and double ended arms on the shaft, and operating the five pairs of keys, it was possible to get an arrangement by which variations in intensity might occur every sixth or eighth click. Taking all the possible arrangements together, the operator might introduce a more intense click every two, three, four, five, six or eight clicks. Again, he might make a series of clicks which were composed of two, three, four or five different intensities of sound. By making the number of degrees between the arms on the drum- shaft different, a difference in time- interval between the clicks was produced. In the same way as with the dif- ferent intensities, a longer interval of time might be made to recur every two, three, four, five, six or eight clicks. 40 BOLTON : The rate at which the drum- shaft revolved determined the rate of the clicks in the telephone. This was controlled by the fan regulator upon the chronograph. Faster or slower rates were obtained by using smaller or larger fans. The rate was determined by counting the clicks in the telephone by a stop-watch. Eates between one click in two seconds and ten in one second were possible. As the rate was a very important factor, it will be given in all cases in the presenta- tion of results. The " time" will indicate the interval be- tween two clicks. The battery used consisted of 36 cells of the "Watson's patent. A further method of testing the accuracy of the setting of the arms upon the drum- shaft, which was done with a pro- tractor, was to connect a time-marker in the primary circuit and take the record upon a drum along with a tunning-fork. It was found that setting might be accurate, but the drum- shaft might vary between one and two hundredths seconds in six seconds. There is one particular in which an improvement might have been made in the apparatus. It was this : When the primary circuit was made, though the secondary circuit was open, a faint sound was heard in the telephone with close attention. The induction coil acts as an electric condenser, and the telephone being extremely sensitive, betrayed the presence of a weak current. This might have been avoided by making a break in both wires leading to the telephone, in such a way that the telephone would be wholly disconnected from the induction coil, when the primary circuit was made. During the entire experiment, only a single subject detected the presence of this sound, and for that reason it may be disre- garded. The telephone was placed in a different room from the chronograph, where there was as little disturbance from other noises as possible, especially from any noises that were in the least suggestive of a rhythm. When the experiment first began, the apparatus was set so that about three or four clicks to the second were heard in the telephone. The subjects were not informed in any par- ticular in regard to the experiment. They were invited to be seated and listen to the telephone. This they did, taking very generally a rather critical attitude. They were then invited to say anything that suggested itself to them, what- ever the character. These statements were all carefully recorded, and will be given in substance. The sounds sug- gested most generally and immediately the clock. Other suggestions were: slowly dripping water, galloping horse, pile-driver, etc. After the subjects had been seated for a time, during which it was apparent they were making a critical RHYTHM. 41 study of the nature of the sounds, the statement most gener- ally given, and voluntarily, was that the sounds were all alike, and seemed to be separated by the same interval of time. After this statement the subject paused, as if most that could be said had been said. In some cases they asked for particulars in regard to what they should look for. Some- times, however, they went on to say that there was an apparent change of intensity in the sounds ; the clicks seem to group themselves by twos or fours, as the case might be ; generally, however, it required some kind of a suggestion to direct the attention of the subject to the grouping of the sounds. An indirect method was preferred to a direct one. In cases where the subject had spoken of the clicks seeming like the clock ticks, they were asked if there was the same difference of intensity or quality in the sounds as was apparent in the clock ticks. This suggestion was sufficient in many cases. The subject directed his attention then to the matter, and if there was any tendency to make groups of the clicks, it was apparent in a few moments. Sometimes it was remarked that they had noticed such a grouping, but had regarded it as a freak of their imagination, and did not think it worth mentioning. Another method of directing the attention of the subject to the grouping was to make a refer- ence to the fact that they had said the sounds were all alike, and then to ask why they had said sounds and not sound ; did they suppose there was more than one sound ? In this case also, they replied frequently that they imagined that there was more than one sound, but did not think it worth while to mention the fact. In some cases it was sufficient to ask the subjects to count the clicks as they heard them, and then to ask how they counted. The reply was that they counted four or two, as the case might be, and then began again. Again it was noticed that the subject was uncon- sciously keeping time, with the foot tapping to every fourth or every second click. Such a subject was asked why he tapped every fourth or second click, and so his attention was directed to a grouping that was going on unconsciously. Such indirect methods were usually successful, but there were several cases in which indirect suggestions of this sort failed of their purpose. Direct methods of tapping a rhythm with the fingers or counting did not suggest anything beyond the clock tick to two subjects. These persons possessed no appreciation of music at all ; they could not " carry a tune," and yet were able to recognize some of the common airs when they were sung or whistled. The general statement of the remarks and answers of each subject will be given as fully as it seems necessary. They will be abridged as far as possible, 42 BOLTON : but the special features in the answers of each subject will be mentioned. The treatment of special phases of these results will follow, and then will be taken up the result of special investigations that were suggested during the first part of the experiment. Subject 1. Some musical talent and training. Time, .23 sec. The first suggestion was a 4-group. Subject could suggest groups of two, three, five and six, but when he made no suggestion either by tapping or counting, he returned to a 4-group. The third in each 4-group was accented, 1 but it was pos- sible in the later experiments to accent any member of the group. In general the first in all forms of grouping was accented. The 3-group was unpleasant and the 5-group was very difficult to main- tain. Time, 1.14 sec. The most natural form of grouping was by two. It was possible to get a 4-group, but when the subject made no suggestion of any other group, he returned to the 2-group. Time, .167 sec. The 6-group was most easily suggested. It had the appearance of being composed of two 3-groups. The subject showed a tendency with this rate to group the 3 and 4-groups into higher groups. Eight-groups of threes and 4-groups of fours succeeded very well. It was not so easy or natural to make higher groups of fours. Time, 1. sec. This rate produced a drowsy feeling. The subject was inclined to make each click stand as the accented click in a 3-group, supplying the unaccented sounds between the accented in imagination. When the subject was tired he noticed a tendency to change the grouping frequently from two to three, and vice versa. The subject showed a strong tendencjr towards 4-grouping in pref- erence to all other forms of grouping, and yet during one experi- ment, when the time was -208 sec, he found a 3-group more pleas- ant than either a 2-group or a 4-group. The rate was too fast for easy grouping by two. When he counted objects he counted them by fours. Time, .323 sec. The subject was disposed to make a 4-group, and, even when every third sound was made more intense than the others, he persisted in saying that he grouped them by fours, but that there was probably a longer interval in the series which disturbed the smoothness of his 4-groups. When his atten- tion was called later to the accented clicks, he made no further mistakes of longer intervals for accented sounds. Subject 2. Some musical talent and training. Accustomed to introspective work. Time, .323 sec. The subject grouped by twos, visualizing the pendulum. 2 He could suggest groups of three and four easily, the four being more difficult than the three. Time, .263 sec. He grouped the clicks by four, but the 4-groups were divided into two 2-groups. Time, .208 sec. This rate yielded easily and naturally to a double 3-grouping. When he first listened to the telephone after 'This accent consisted of an apparent increase in intensity with a change in pitch and quality. 2 Almost every subject either visualized the pendulum or spoke of the pendulum-swing movement sometime during the experiments. In either case it was a form of grouping. When the rates were slow, the subject visualized the clock pendulum and made one click come near the completion of each half swing. The clicks were then grouped by two and were called the clock tick. In my own case and in some others there was a strong tendency to sway the body with the pendulum. This was called the pendulum- swing movement by the different subjects. It was quite visible at times. By this pen- dulum-swing movement groups of two, three, four, six or eight were frequently grouped into 2-groups. The first group, then, in the 2-group was accented or more emphatic than the other, and a distinct pause seemed to follow the second group. RHYTHM. 43 either a change of rate or at the beginning of a new experiment, the clicks did not group themselves, but in a short time the tendency to group increased until it required the greatest efforts to hear the clicks as a uniform series. The subject was able to hear the clicks as a uniform series, only by imagining some one pound- ing in the distance. It required a mental picture of some object- ive thing that was perfectly uniform. When he gave himself up and listened to the series as a whole, he fell into some kind of grouping, which might or might not continue for any length of time. He had a strong tendency to shift from one grouping to another. He com- pared it to the optical illusion of the "stairs." The double 2-group is confounded sometimes with the double 3-group. Time, .187 sec. The subject said he got a compound 2 and 3-group, which by actual count of the accents to which he tapped with the fingers, showed he was making a double 4- group. 1 This subject was strongly dis- posed to double groups of all sorts. Time, .323 sec. At this rate the 2-group was most naturally accompanied by the mental image of the clock. Time, .263 sec. This yielded most easily to a 4-group, which took the form of two groups of twos. Time, .167 sec. This rate yielded at first to a 6-grouping, which was divided into two groups of threes, but it did not persist there ; he returned to a double 2-group. The pulse seemed at times to impose a grouping in which the clicks coming nearest in time to the heart-beat was accented. When the subject gave his attention to breathing, it more generally conformed itself to some grouping that was already going on. Inhalation lasted during a 4-group and exhalation during a 3-group. Subject 3. Considerable musical talent and training. Time, .5 sec. The subject's first suggestion was of a 2-group, but he immediately decided that a 4-group was more natural. He was able to count almost any rhythm at this rate as far as twelve, and the clicks seem to group themselves with the count. At first the groups were apparently separated by a longer interval, which the subject believed in the first place to be real. He was disposed to regard the 4-group as the most satisfactory. Any grouping was plainer when he counted. Diaphragmatic movements also accompany the grouping. With indifferent attention there was no grouping. The 4-group usually contained two accented clicks, either the first and the third or the second and the fourth. The former were pre- ferred. This rate was found to be most pleasing. It was animating. The 5-group was difficult to get. A slight pause occurred between the groups in every form of grouping. In the presence of the chronograph, which gave a 6-rhythm which was composed of two 3-groups, the subject still grouped by four for a time, but this tendency was finally overcome and the series yielded to the sugges- tion of the chronograph. When every fourth was accented, the subject being unaware of this accent, said that the 4-group only was possible, for there appeared to be a longer interval between every four clicks which made any other grouping impossible. When the accent was strengthened, he said the interval had been lengthened. This long interval might come anywhere within the group of four, but it more generally came between the groups. When two stronger clicks followed by two weaker ones formed the series, the subject said the 'It is not unfrequent for a subject to mistake the actual grouping which he is making. Sometimes a subject is so disposed to a particular number that he persists in saying that he gets groups of that number, when it is perfectly evident a greater or less number of clicks according to the circumstances is grouped with the accented clicks to which he taps. 44 BOLTON : rate was slower. He grouped the series by fours, but it appeared as though two long sounds followed by two short ones formed the group. When three strong sounds and one weak one formed the series, he still grouped by four. The first two in each group seemed to be of the same length, the third was longer and the fourth very short. During all the experiments the subject confounded stronger clicks with long intervals, and was never able to tell the difference between a strong sound and a long interval. He was surprised when told afterwards that the longer interval had been caused by accenting one sound. Time, 2.304 sec. The subject visualized the pendulum, but said the pendulum seemed to reach its full swing before the click corresponding to the swing was heard. The clicks seemed to delay too long. Time, .323 sec. Every third sound was accented. The subject had a strong mental habit for grouping by fours and was greatly puzzled by this accent on every third, which he said was a longer interval and broke up his tendency to form groups of fours. Time, .208 sec. Every third was accented. The subject forms 6-groups, which were accented upon the first and fourth and a long interval appeared between the groups of six. Time, .137 sec. When the series was composed of clicks of three different intensities repeating themselves in the same order, the 3-groups were again grouped by four generally, though the subject could suggest groups of three 3-groups. Subject 4. Some musical talent. Time, .288 sec. The clicks suggested the clock-tick. The subject could group them by twos, but he found it more natural to groap by fours. It has long been a mental habit with him to make groups of four of any objects or impressions that would admit of any kind of grouping. He counts by four and groups the puffs of a locomotive by four. Four objects or impressions of any sort standing together have always arrested his attention. He found it possible to group these clicks by two, three or five when he made a suggestion either by counting or tapping with the fingers, but when the suggestion was stopped he returned to a 4-group. In every kind of grouping the first sound was always accented. Time, ,115 sec. The subject said the grouping was by four and was requested to tap the accented click in every group. In six trials for five seconds each, he tapped just five times during each trial, showing he made a group of four in one second. The actual number of clicks to the second being 8.6, it was apparent that he was making a much larger group than four, probably an 8-group. When asked to make a 3-group and tap the accented click in each group, the results were nineteen taps in fifteen seconds, showing that his groups were not far from six instead of three clicks to the group. 1 The 3-group was really a 4-group in many cases. Between each group of three occurred one click, of which no account was taken. It seemed to him something like this when he counted : 1, 2, 3, 1 — 1,2,3,1-1,2,3,1. When the subject gave attention to the pulse, the number of clicks coming between the beats of the heart formed a group. The click which came nearest in time to the heart-beat seemed always to correspond to it. The breathing adjusts itself to the 4-rhythm. Inhalation lasts during one group of four, and exhalation during another. In this way the 4-groups were grouped by two. Time, .156 sec. By forming a mental image of the pendulum, or of some object moving up and down, he was able to make double 4-groups, 'It is probable that the primary grouping was two, and these groups of two were then united into larger groups of three and four. RHYTHM. 45 which corresponded to the full swing of the pendulum. These three rates were given in rapid succession. Time, .536 sec. The 4-group was very clear. Time, .268 sec. The 4-group was un- pleasant at first, but he gradually became accustomed to it. At first the rates seemed too fast. Time, .536 sec. With this latter rate the 4-group seemed to divide into two groups of two during the second trial. Time 1.072 sec. The 2-group seemed most natural and the subject felt a strong tendency to form higher groups of twos. After the subject became accustomed to this rate, he was more inclined to form 4-groups than 2-groups, but still the third and fourth clicks seemed at times more like a 2-group than a part of a 4-group. The general effect of this last rate was soothing. Time, 1.66 sec. This rate was soporific ; it was possible to form a 2-group, but he did not do so spontaneously. Time, .323 sec. The subject grouped the clicks by four and found difficulty in suggest ing a 3-group. When every third was accented, he persisted in grouping by four. Again he was asked to suggest a 3 -grouping, and he succeeded in doing so. The subject was unaware of the accent, and expressed surprise that he could group by three, and found it easier than grouping by four. The following rates were given in rapid succession. Time, .268 sec. The 4-group was very clear and pleasant. Time, .17 sec. The subject grouped by fours, but felt a confused irritating feeling. There was something added onto each group of four. Time, .134 sec. This rate recalled the sound of a locomotive. He visualized a revolving wheel, during each revolution of which he counted four. Time, .116 sec. He still groups by four, but the 4-groups are grouped by two, a strong and weak group together. When every eighth was accented, he grouped by eight. There was a distinct pause along with the accented sound. Time, .134 sec. Every eighth was accented. The 8-group divided into two 4-groups, with, a pause after the second group. The 4-groups were grouped by two with the pendulum swing. Time, .268 sec. Every eighth was accented. The subject grouped by four and associated the pendulum swing with the groups of four. Subject 5. Some musical talent. Time, .288 sec. The subject was most naturally disposed to a 4-group, and found it difficult to get any other. Time, .78 sec. The 2-group was most natural with this rate. The 3-group was pleasant and easy when it was suggested. Time, 1.44 sec. It was easy to form 2-groups, but other groups were impossible. These three rates were given in rapid succession. Time, .353 sec. The 4-group was most natural. Time, .183 sec. The 8-group was most agree- able with this rate. Time, .156 sec. The subject found the 8-group most natural. Time, .3 sec. When every fifth was accented, the subject made groups of four, accenting the four, and said that there was a rest between each group. He found it quite as easy also to accent the first. When his attention was called to this pause between the groups, he decided that the groups contained five clicks, in which the fourth was accented. When the first and third clicks were made more intense, the subject was greatly puzzled for a time, but decided that the series was compounded of a 2-group and a 3-group. Time, .969 sec. He was able to form 3 and 4-groups. The series was associated strongly with the clock, and for that reason the 4-group tended strongly to divide into two 2-groups. Time, .323 sec. He found the 4-group most natural and pleasant, and when 46 BOLTON : he attempted to form groups of three, they would immediately run into fours. Time, .208 sec. The subject formed a long group of the clicks which he thought was an 8 -group. The rate was too fast for easy grouping by four, and his attention seemed to waver between a 4-group and a longer one which he thought was an eight. Subject 6. Some musical talent and training. Time, .3 sec. "When the subject first listened to the telephone he found no tendency to form groups of these clicks. Even after repeated suggestions the subject did not comprehend what was desired or to be looked for. He was asked to tap an accompani- ment to the sounds with four fingers. After a time he found himself accenting the third, and grouping the sounds by four. When he tried the suggestion with three fingers it did not succeed very well. When two were tried, the subject decided that the 4-group was a combination of two 2-groups. In the same way he was able to group the sounds by six, but the groups divided easily into two 3-groups. On the whole the 2-group was the most natural with this rate. Time, .156 sec. The 4-group prevailed and easily combined into 8-groups. Time, .78 sec. The 2-group was most natural, but the subject was able to suggest the 3-group easily. Time, 1.44 sec. Even a 2-group was difficult to get. The time seemed to be too long. Time, .353 sec. Groups of two, three and four were all possible. The first click in all groups was accented, and the third also in the 4- groups. When he suggested a 6-group, it divided easily into two 3-groups or three 2-groups. The 8-group di- vided readily into two 4- groups. It was difficult to get a 5- group. The 5-group tended to run into a 6-group, which then divided easily into two 3-groups. Time, .288 sec. The 4-group was most natural, and readily combined into double 4-groups. Time, .156 sec. The 6-group was most natural. When the telephone was disconnected and the chronograph continued to run, the grouping always began with the first click in a new group. Time, .78 sec. The 2-group was the most natural. Time, 1.66 sec. There was no spontaneous grouping with this rate. It was too slow. The subject found it convenient to regard the click as an accented click in a 4-group, supplying the three intermediate sounds in imagination. Time, .323 sec. At this rate, the subject showed strong tendency to muscular movements, either to tap with the finger or toe upon the accented click, some- times to nod the head or sway the body. He found this rate very favorable for voluntary changes of the grouping, which he did either by counting or tapping with the fingers. The general emotional effect was depressing. The pendulum-swing movement or 2-rhythm was an important factor in all his groups. The 6-group was usually composed of two 3-groups, and the 8-group of two 4-groups. Time, .167 sec. The 4-group was very clear and pleasant, and the subject tended to group them by the motions of the pendulum. When every third click was strengthened, the subject grouped by threes, and made the 3-groups follow the motions of the pendulum. In this way the series produced an exciting effect. Time, .134 sec. The 4-group was plain and distinctly grouped by twos by the pendulum-swing. This rate was also exciting and animating. When every sixth click was accented, the grouping lost its exciting effect. When the series was composed of clicks of three intensities, the strongest first, the clicks were grouped primarily by threes and these 3-groups were again grouped into 4-groups. When the time was changed to .167 sec, and three grades of intensity retained, the higher grouping of 3-groups by four ceased. Time, .137 sec. When RHYTHM. 47 every sixth click was accented, the series was grouped by six, and the six groups were again grouped by the motions of the pendulum. Time, .167 sec. Every sixth was accented. The grouping was still by six, but the 6 -groups did not group by two. With the slower rate the 6-groups did not seem so compact as with the faster rate. Time, .208 sec. Every sixth was accented. The 6-group was diffi- cult to grasp. The time was too long and the group tended to divide into two 3-groups. Time, .263 sec. Every sixth was accented. There was greater difficulty still in grasping the 6-group, on account of the tendency to divide the group into two 3-groups. Time, .323 sec. Every sixth was accented. The subject now grouped the series by four in spite of the accent upon every sixth. When the subject heard the sound of the chronograph, which was rhythmical, he grouped the clicks according to this rhythm, which in this case was a double 3-rhythmical. Time, .3 sec. Every fifth sound was accented. The subject grouped by fours, but the accent came in a different place in each group. It shifted one place further to the right. When a 6-group was suggested to the subject, the accent changed its position in the opposite way. It shifted its position one place to the left in each group. Subject 7. Some musical talent and training. Time, .3 sec. Almost immediately the series divided into groups of four, and soon after the 4-groups were grouped by two. With the suggestion of tapping, the subject was able to group by two, three or five. Time, .78 sec. The 2-group could be suggested only with difficulty. The 3 and 4-groups were not at all pleasant. Time, .156 sec. There was no distinct grouping. The series seemed to rise and fall in intensity at regular intervals. At times he had a "dreadful" feeling that the chronograph was slowing up and about to stop. The subject had observed this grouping of sounds in the puffing of a locomotive. He had not noticed a definite number in the group. The sounds simply rise and fall in intensity. Speaking of the 8-group, the subject said he had a feeling of not being able to " round up " until he came to eight. It seemed natural to stop at eight, and start over again. This group was accompanied by a feeling of completeness. During inhalation, the clicks seemed to come faster, and slower during exhalation. In all forms of group- ing the subject felt a muscular sensation in the stomach and intes- tines. He also felt a strong tendency to beat time with the thumb. He had been taught to do so when quite young. Four- grouping is a kind of mental habit with him. When the series was accented upon every fifth, he still grouped by four, making a pause between each group in which he pronounced the word "and:" 1, 2, 3, 4, and 1, 2, 3, 4, and 1, 2, 3, 4. In the same way the 3-group appeared as 1, 2, 3, 1, — 1, 2, 3, 1, — 1, 2, 3, 1. In the 4-group the first and third were always accented. In other groups the first was accented. When the rate was .72 sec. or .156 sec. the grouping did not come without suggestion by muscular movement or counting ; with intermediate rates the grouping by four was wholly involuntary. The subject could group 4-groups by two very easily, but it required an effort to group them by four, or 8 -groups by two. The 8-group was generally composed of a more and a less emphatic group of four. Time, .268 sec. A double 4-group was the most natural and easy. One group was more emphatic than the other. Time, .263 sec. Though the time was only slightly changed, the subject thought the previous double 4-group changed into a more perfect 8-group. 48 BOLTON : There was not such a strong division into two 4-groups. Time, .208 sec. This rate gave a " better 8-group " than the previous one. Time, .167 sec. The groups did not separate distinctly. There was a kind of confused feeling about the clicks. Time, .137 sec. The confused feeling with the previous rate was more apparent still. When every sixth was accented, he grouped by six and the 6-groups were grouped by two with the pendulum- swing movement. Subject 8. Time, .3 sec. The subject took a critical attitude. He had no preference for any grouping. He could count any number as far as ten, and the series seemed to group itself according to the count. In the longer groups, groups of two were frequent. Time, .156 sec. The 8-group was the most suitable. It was composed of two 4-groups, and each4-group of two 2-groups. Time, 1.44 sec. It was not possible to form any grouping. The rate was too slow. The subject has noticed rhythms in the sounds of a mill-wheel, locomo- tives and fans. He was not aware of any definite grouping of the sounds. Subject 9. Considerable musical talent. Long and careful training in music. Accustomed to introspective study. Time, .3 sec. The subject adopted a critical attitude and gave his attention to the nature of the sounds. At first he was inclined to believe that they were all alike in intensity, but then he thought every third was stronger than the rest. For a time the interval between the clicks seemed to be irregular, but he soon discovered that this irregular interval might occur anywhere he chose to put it. In a short time his tendency to find groupings of the clicks grew so strong that it required an effort to hear the series uniform. Such an effort was akin to the feeling of ''looking long into the future." The grouping tendency had to be restrained. Time, .3 sec. The 4- group was so plain that he did not discover the fact that it was imaginary and was completely surprised that the illusion was so complete. It was then more than ever an effort to hear a uniform series of single impressions. He said, ''I find no rhythm as long as I hold my breath and stick to it." "I get hold of one click to compare it with the succeeding clicks, but I can't hold onto more than eight or nine." The simple suggestion of any grouping was sufficient to produce that grouping. Groups of two, three, five, six and eight follow immediately the suggestion of any of them. A group of seven was more difficult. Groups of all numbers were generally accented upon the first, but the accent could be volun- tarily changed. In the 8-group the subject had a tendency to accent every other one. The grouping was generally accompanied by visible motions of the head and lips. A slight feeling of muscle tension in the ear and back of the scalp marked one group from another. There was a feeling of innervation of the muscles con- nected with attention. When the attention was directed to respiration the grouping was not affected. Respiration was more inclined to follow the group- ing. The heart-beat coming in about the same time as the accent in a 4-group, tended to coincide with it. Time, .2 sec. The tendency to group was still present in a small degree. The sound was quieting. It suggested slowly dripping water. Time, 1.5 sec. The suggestion was of a big clock. After listening to a fast rate for a time and then to the rate of .687 sec, he decided that he felt no tendency to group the sounds of the latter. Time, .116 sec. The most natural group was eight, with a slight tendency to divide into RHYTHM. 49 two 4-groups. Time, .134 sec. The subject found a 4-group more natural than an 8 with this rate ? but felt some tendency to make a double 4-group instead of a simple 4-group. Time, .116 sec. An 8-group composed of two 4-groups was most pleasant. Time, .268 sec. His most pleasant group was two, but these groups tended to combine to form double 2-groups. When two strong clicks followed by two weak ones formed the series, he grouped by fours, but accented the second and fourth. He described the phenomonon as a summation in the second : "The after-image of the first was left to the second to increase its strength." When the subject heard the rhythm of the chronograph, he grouped the sounds accordingly. Subject 10. Some musical talent and training. Time, .3 sec. The 4-group appeared immediately. The subject could suggest other groups of three, five, six and eight. The 3- group was accented upon the first, and the 4-group upon the first and third. During one experiment the subject said the accents in the 4-group were not distinguishable, but the groups were separated by a slight interval. The 5-group was accented upon the first and fourth, the 6-group upon every other one, and the 8-group was a repetition of two 4-groups. Higher groups of threes as far as four were easily obtained. The first and third groups of threes were accented. Higher groups of fours were not easy or distinct. During all the experiments unconscious movements in the tongue were present. A slight muscular contraction took place with the accented click. Other movements of the head, trunk, feet and hands were visible, and the subject found it difficult to restrain them. Time, .134 sec. These sounds were grouped by eight and the grouping was pleasant and animating. Time, .156 sec. The first suggestion was of a 6-group. The 8-group was difficult. Time, .268 sec. This yielded to a 4-group, which the subject said required about the same time as the previous 8-group. Time, .78 sec. The 2- group was the only one practicable. The suspense for others was too great. Time, .116 sec. During this experiment the rate yielded most easily to a double 4-group, and when the time was changed to .45 sec. he had a similar feeling with the group of two, but one click stood in the place of the 4-group with the previous rate. Time, .116 sec. Every eighth was accented. The subject got a very pleasant and "harmonious" 8-group. Time, .134 sec. Every eighth was accented. The subject was less animated. He said, "The group was more staid and steady. It had lost its tones." Time, .17 sec. Every eighth was accented. It now required an effort of attention to get the 8-group. It grew more pleasant as the subject became accustomed to it. Time, .268 sec. Every eighth was accented. The grouping was by four. Sometimes the subject accented every other one and felt. disposed to count thus : one and two and three and four, and repeating this between the accented clicks. Subject ll. 1 Some musical talent and training. Time, .5 sec. The 4-group suggested itself immediately. The first and third clicks were accented, the first stronger than the third. Sometimes the third might be stronger than the first. It was possible to accent the second and fourth. When the subject gave close and critical attention to the sound, there was no tendency to grouping. The grouping seemed most clear with an indifferent state of mind; He showed a decided preference for 2 and 4-groups. Time, .25 sec. The 4-group was most natural. Time, .115 sec. This •The subject knew beforehand that this was to be an experiment ia the rhythmical grouping oi: sounds. 50 BOLTON : rate yielded to an 8-grouping, each group being composed of four strong and four weak sounds. At other times with this rate the sounds seemed to rise and fall at regular intervals, which the subject described as a waxing and waning of the attention. Time, .167 sec. The subject grouped by four, but felt a straining for a larger group. Time, 1.67 sec. He grouped by two and visualized the pendulum. One click came during each half swing. When the subject gave attention to his breathing, he made an inspiration last during the time of one click, and expiration during the time of another. The first click was louder than the last. Time, .115 sec. When the subject gave attention to his pulse the groups corre- sponded to the time of the heart-beats. The click which came near the beat was louder and became the first in the group. The pulse seemed to reinforce the sensation of the sound. When the attention was directed to respiration, the clicks increased in intensity during inspiration and were grouped by two and decreased in intensity during expiration. He visualized a curved line which rose during inspiration and fell during expiration. Smaller undulations in the larger curve corresponded to the 2-group. A melody always appears to him as a zigzag line, in which the rises correspond with every two notes. Time, .156 sec. He grouped the clicks by eight and visualized an ellipse with four points upon either side. The clicks seemed to locate themselves on these points. The subject showed a strong tendency to muscular movements. He felt an impulse to dance, clap the hands and tap the toes and fingers upon the accented click. When the rate was .286 sec, this tendency to muscular movements was stronger than with the other rates. There was something animating about this rate. Time, .3 sec. Every fifth was accented. The clicks were grouped by five. The accented click always appeared as the fourth in the 5- group and longer than the others. When this click was further increased in intensity, it seemed very much longer than the rest and appeared as an extraneous sound which did not enter into the group. The other four sounds then formed a group by themselves. When every sixth was accented, the accented sound again appeared as an extraneous sound. It simply disturbed his mental habits of forming some other groups. When two clicks in every five were made stronger with one weak click between the two strong ones, the grouping was still by five but it was a combination of a 2-group and a 3- group. When three strong and two weak clicks formed a group, it was composed then of a 3-group and a 2-group. The 3-group contained two strong sounds and one weak, and the 2-group one strong and one weak. A short pause came after the fourth sound, which made it impossible to make the 5 -group appear as composed of a2-groupand a3-group. Time, .268 sec. Every third sound was accented. This accent simply broke up the tendency of the subject to group by four and did not compel him to group by three. When every sixth was accented, he grouped by six, and accented the first and fifth, but there was a strain towards a 4-group. Time, .167 sec. Every sixth was accented. With this rate the 6 -group was pleasant and did not tend so strongly towards a 4-group. Time, .137 sec. Every sixth was accented. The 6-group was pleasant, and it tended to unite into higher groups of two with the pendulum- swing move- ment. Time, .208 sec. When the subject listened to the sound of the chronograph, which made a distinct and strong 8-rhythm, he was unable to form any other group than eight. The 8-group was com- posed of two 4-groups, the first of which was much stronger than EHYTHM. 51 the second. When he listened to the chronograph, which gave a 6-rhythm, which was composed of two 3-rhythms, he was unable for a time to get anything but a 6-group, but this faded out with continued effort and gave place to his previous 4-groups. The 4-groups were then grouped by two with the swing of the pendulum. Time, .134 sec. Every eighth was accented. The grouping was by eight, and the 8-groups were then grouped by two. Time, .116 sec. Every eighth was accented, and the grouping was by eight, and pleasant. Time, .134 sec. Every eighth was accented. The subject took no spontaneous interest in the 8-group at this rate. The period seemed to be too long. "It breaks off with a dead end," he said. Time, .17 sec. Every eighth was accented, but the grouping was by fours. The accented click was simply a disturbing element. The series did not group easily by either four or eight. Time, .208 sec. Every eighth was accented. The grouping at this rate was distinctly by four. The accented click acted somewhat as a disturbing element. When every fourth was accented at this rate, the 4-grouping became pleasant, and the accented sound was the first in each group. The 4-groups were grouped by two with the swing of the pendulum. Time, .17 sec. Every fourth was accented, but the time seemed to be too fast for a pleasant 4- group. When every sixth was accented, and the time .323 sec, the grouping was by three, but the tendency to a 4-grouping was so strong that it was possible to get a 4-group in which every sixth sound was accented, the accented sound shifting its position in the group. The accented click seemed longer, and a longer interval followed it. When a very weak sound was followed by a very intense one, the sound of the loud click spread itself over the weaker one. Subject 12. Considerable musical talent and great interest in music. Accustomed to introspective study. Time, .3 sec. The subject began immediately to count the clicks, accenting every third. He unconsciously rocked himself in the chair to keep time. He thought the rate slowed up at times and then quickened again. The grouping was changed from three to four by simply thinking of the number. He believed there was some unconscious muscular movement about the change from one rate to another. He could suggest a change by simply tapping with his fingers. When he changed from a 3-group to a 4-group, the 4-group seemed too long at first, though he became accustomed to it. In a short time the grouping seemed to change of itself into three and then again into four. The 4-group was inclined to fall into two 2-groups, the subject unconsciously nodding his head to every other sound. He was able to suggest a 5-group, in which the first and third were accented, the third more strongly. He could accent any click in the group, but the first and the third seemed easiest. Time, .156 sec. The 6-group appeared imme- diately and spontaneously, and then broke up into two 3- groups. He suggested a double 4-group, which gave rise to a feeling of a slower pace. It was not so distinct as the double 3-group. This had a kind of impelling force. The subject attempted to step in time with the double 3-group, and then with the 4-group. The double 3-group required a sprightly step. It was exciting. The 4-group at this rate did not appeal to him; it didn't take hold. This rate was more stimulating than the previous one. Time, .115 sec. The subject dropped into a 4-group, but the three was found more stimulating. It was difficult, however, for him to put aside the 52 BOLTON : previous rate, and adapt himself to the new one. One click in each group, however, seemed distinctly louder than the rest. "When he grouped by four, it easily passed into an 8-group, but the 8-group was not so clear as the 6-group. He imagined a wheel going around, making six clicks to a revolution. When he changed the telephone from one ear to the other, the grouping changed from six to a double 4-group, and persisted for a time. The 5-group came only with difficulty. Time, .76 sec. The grouping was by twos. The subject imagined the clock at home. The 3-group was suggested by an image of a musical conductor beating time. Time, 1.44 sec. The subject gets the rhythm of the pendulum swing with- out suggestion. He suggests also a 3-group, which recalls the time of church music. Time, 1.66 sec. He finds it easy to imagine inter- mediate sounds between the actual clicks, and these he groups by three, the real click being the accented click in the 3-group. Time, .286 sec. In order to obtain a notion of a rhythmic series — one of uniform intensity — the subject turned his attention backward, and saw a series of images to which he was adding one all the time. He throws his attention upon what comes, and studies the nature of the noise to see if the timbre is the same. It is a comparative effort. But in spite of all efforts the series groups into a 2-group at times. When a relay sounder was connected in the circuit of a vibrator, which made 20 vibratious to the second, the subject was still able to effect a grouping of the sounds into either 3 or 4-groups by tapping with the fingers upon the table. When he dispensed with the suggestion, the clicks of the relay signal were perfectly uniform, except perhaps a slight waxing and waning in intensity, due probably to the waxing and waning of the attention towards the sound. There was no real grouping. When a longer interval was introduced every fourth, the clicks came in a group of four, but there was nothing satisfactory about the group. The clicks did not form an organic group. Each group of four stood rather as a single compound impression. There was no organic relation between the separate clicks in the group. When the rate was rapid, the groups of four were grouped into higher groups, the groups of four standing as single impressions. When the rate was slow the long interval might come between the groups or anywhere within it. There was something wanting, something to be looked for in the interval. As the nature of the group, the subject described his feeling as a tendency to go back when he had heard three or four clicks, as the case might be. He says he has a "mouthful" — a unity — and when he has one, he seeks to get another. The same process continues to repeat itself. When he directed his attention to the timbre of the click, he got no grouping, but when he looked at the series as a whole, the grouping was clear and spontaneous. There was not, however, necessarily an accent in the group. Subject 13. Considerable musical talent and training. A lover of 2-4 music. Time, .285 sec. It suggests the gallop of a horse — a short gallop — and the clock. There is a breathless feeling about it. It is the sound of car wheels — the whole train. It has a double vibration. The clicks are grouped by two or by four. The group seems to close with a rising inflection ; the last is apparently accented lightly, as the first is strongly. The 2-group prevailed over the four. Parts of "Erl King" are suggested by this grouping. An objective suggestion was displeasing to the subject. The subject preferred a mental suggestion in order to change the grouping from two to anything that was desired. By such a suggestion the sub- RHYTHM. 53 ject was able to get most any group up to eight. The eight group was not clear ; the accents were not sufficiently prominent. The shorter measures are more strongly accented. Time, .115 sec. This rate had a bad effect ; it was tormenting. The grouping was by four of a particular pitch, followed by four of a lower pitch. The subject might group the clicks by two in the same way, but with less clearness. Time, .352 sec. This rate suggested something going around, and every other sound was accented. When the 3-group was suggested, the first click was accented, and the group closed with a rising inflection. Higher groups of 3- groups could be obtained as far as four. The groups seemed to rise and fall in inten- sity. At this rate also the short groups were more strongly accented than the long. When the subject suggested a 4-group, the first and the third were accented, the first probably stronger. The 4- groups may be grouped again by four. Twenty was the greatest number of clicks that the subject could grasp easily in this way. The grouping becomes lost and disconnected with larger numbers. The first groups in the larger groups were of greater intensity, and the last of a lesser intensity. The intensity of each succeeding group seemed to be less. This rate was said to have the most " aesthetic effect." Time, .268 sec. The 2-group was most easy ; a double 2-group was pleasant. The general effect of this rate was a hurried feeling. The previous rate had been restful. Time, .156 sec. The 4-group was most natural, and was accented upon the first and the third. The 6-group appeared without volun- tary effort. There may have been a mental suggestion of the six. Time, .78 sec. There was no real grouping. It seemed painfully slow. Time, 1.44 sec. The subject supplied two intermediate sounds between the clicks, and grouped by three. The actual click was the accented sound in each group, and came first. Time, 1.66 sec. The subject supplied three intermediate sounds between the clicks, and grouped by fours. The real sound came first. Time, .134 sec. The double 4-group was most natural, and the subject breathed with it. When every eighth was accented, the subject did not become aware of the accent. The grouping was spoken of as being so strong that it could not be gotten rid of. The groups of eight were grouped by two with the swing of the pendulum. The clicks in the 8- group seem to decrease in intensity from the beginning to the end. Time, .116 sec. Every eighth was accented. The movement was the same as with the previous rate, or perhaps in place of the pendulum movement the subject visualized an object moving up and down, the upward motion lasting during the time of an 8-group, and the downward motion during another 8-group. There was apparently a longer pause after the second group. The subject felt a strong tendency to nod the head, and keep the time by tapping the toe. Time, .17 sec. Every eighth was accented. The 8-group lacked completeness. It was not so smooth as the 8-group before ; it was distinctly divided into two 4-groups. The accented sounds were generally unpleasant. The subject " has not the restful impression of evenness" which had characterized the uniform series. Time, .323 sec. When the clicks were all of the same intensity, the slightest suggestion of any sort was sufficient to cause the clicks to fall into the group suggested. Even when the attention of the subject was not called to a suggestion, and the subject apparently did not attend, it would change the grouping to that suggested. At times the subject had a feeling which was described as " awful," that the chronograph was slowing up and about to stop. When stronger clicks were introduced, the effect was unpleasant. 54 BOLTON : The following rates were given in rapid succession: Time, .268 sec. The clicks were grouped by two, and the 2-groups seemed to rise and fall in intensity at regular intervals. Subject could suggest other groupings, but it drifted back to this, unless the subject kept up the suggestion of some other. Time, .208 sec. The grouping was by four. The rate was unpleasantly fast for a time. Time, .134 sec. The grouping was by four ; the 4- groups seemed to rise and fall in intensity, every other one being more intense. The subject unconsciously breathed with this secondary grouping. Every eighth was made more intense. The subject did not detect the accent, but said the grouping by eight was so clear that it could not be avoided. The 8-groups tended to group into 2-groups. Time, .116 sec. Every eighth was accented. The clicks were grouped by eight, and the 8-groups were grouped by a wave-like motion. There appeared to be a longer interval between every two groups. Time, .17 sec. Every eighth was accented. The grouping was primarily by two, and the 2-groups were grouped by four. The intensity of the clicks seemed to decrease from the beginning to the end. The grouping was rough in comparison with that for the previous rate. This form of grouping gave place finally to a double 4-grouping, and the subject was strongly inclined to keep the time by nodding or tapping with the toe. Especially strong was this impulse when strength of the accent was increased. Time, .208 sec. Every eighth was accented. The 8-group was now more distinctly divided into two 4-groups. This grouping had more " dignity and force, but was not so tripping as the fast rate was." The 8-group was not so complete as it was with the faster rates. Subject 14. Some musical talent and training. The first suggestion of a grouping was by eight, and the 8-group was divided into two 4-groups. When a 2-group was suggested the subject agreed that he could get it, but the 2-groups were again grouped by two into 4-groups, and the 4-groups by two into 8-groups. A 6-group was suggested by counting six, but there seemed to be a division corresponding to 4-groups. The subject was under the impression for a time that there was a longer interval or four different intensities of sounds which made this 4-grouping. The 4-group was accented upon the first and the third. The 3-group did not succeed very well. The subject seemed to have a habit of forming groups of two, and the strongest kind of a sugges- tion was not sufficient to put it aside for a 3-group. Time, .156 sec. The 8-group, which was divided into two 4-groups, was the most natural, and seemed to prevail over all others. Time, .78 sec. The 2-group was most easily obtained, but it was possible to sug- gest either a 3-group or a 4-group. The subject was not sure whether he preferred a 2-group to a 4-group. He also found the 3-group quite pleasant. Time, 1.44 sec. The 2-group was most natural, and the subject could still suggest either a 3 or a 4-group, but when he dispensed with suggestion, he returned to the 2-group. The subject has noticed rhythms in the sound of mill wheels. When he gave his attention to these sounds he visualized a series of points on a fine which he counted by four or two. When he was asked to count a series of dots, he said they were divided off into twos by a bracket above them. It has always been a habit with him to count objects by two. When every fifth was accented, he grouped by five; the accented click came fourth in the group, and it seemed longer than the rest. When the accented click was made more intense still, its time seemed longer than the rest. When one of the five was made RHYTHM. 55 weaker than the rest, they formed a somewhat irregular group that was unpleasant. The weak sound caused a disturbance in the group which was not present when a louder sound was introduced. When all the clicks were made more intense, the rate seemed to be slower than at other times. Subject 15. Some musical talent and training. Time, .3 sec. The sound suggested the clock. It was more easy and natural to regard every other one stronger. Groups of three, four and five were suggested. The 4-group was the most natural; the first and third clicks were accented. At times the 4-group seemed to divide into two 2-groups. When the subject attempted to compare the 3-group with the 4-group in point of their agreeable- ness, the three group appeared as three, with one sound coming between the groups, thus : 1, 2, 3, 1, — 1, 2, 3, 1, — 1, 2, 3, 1. This extra sound seemed to occupy a blank space between the three groups. During other experiments afterwards, the 3-group appeared in this form. The 4-groups were easily grouped by two. They would combine into no higher groups as simple 4-groups. The subject was able with great effort to combine two double 4-groups. When the subject counted objects, he usually grouped them by twos. The objects seemed to be joined together by bars. Time, .57 sec. This rate was very quieting. The 4-group was most natural. The first and third or the second and fourth might be accented. A longer interval appeared between the separate groups. A long interval generally follows the accented click whether it is imaginary or real. The subject regards real accents as extraneous intruders. They introduce a long interval, and for that reason the series seems irregular. By irregularity he understood a difference in time interval of the clicks. The accented click seemed nearer to the preceding click than the others. When two real accents o f unequal intensity were put into a group of eight, the interval fol- lowing the more intense click was the longer, and gave to a series a very irregular appearance. When the accented clicks were dropped out, the series became regular again. Time, .268 sec. This rate was very favorable for voluntary changes of grouping. He could suggest any grouping that he might desire within limits. During every experiment the subject mani- fested a strong tendency to some kind of muscular movements. Any kind of muscular contractions would suffice as a suggestion of a grouping. He said he either counted the clicks or made the proper muscular adjustments for counting. There was mental counting always at the start. He made unconscious movements with the eyelids. Motions of the head were clearly visible the whole time. When the subject was asked to restrain all movements of which he was conscious, he said there was great difficulty in keeping the grouping. The telephone was disconnected, and the subject was requested to restrain his muscular movements or attempts to count. When the telephone was connected again, he said that the grouping had kept up during the interval. Although he had restrained all visible motions, slight muscular contractions were observed in the eyelids at the proper intervals of the accented clicks. He said it was possible to keep the grouping by imagining a series of colors passing before the eyes. He spoke of a feeling in the eyes as " muscular color sensation." He seems to have felt an adjustment of the muscles ordinarily used in visual attention. At no time was he conscious of the muscular contractions of the mus- cles in the eyelids. Time, .57 sec. Every fifth was accented. The series was grouped by five and the accented click came anywhere in the group. It was 56 BOLTON : more generally and naturally near the first place. Time, .268 sec. The 3-group could be suggested, and was more naturally accented upon the first, sometimes upon the third. The 6-group was strongly accented upon the third and slightly upon the first and fifth. With a uniform series, the 5-group required a distinct effort and was then accented upon the last. In general the long and complicated groups were less differentiated; they ran together. The 6-group broke up into two 3-groups and the 8 group into two 4-groups. A 7-group was very difficult to get. It would run readily into an 8-group. Time, .134 sec. Every eighth was accented. The 8-group was pleasant at this rate. When the time was .116 sec. and every eighth accented, there was a tendency to group the 8-groups by two. During a subsequent experiment when the time was .116 sec, the series seemed to rise and fall in intensity with no definite grouping. Whenever an accent was put in, it made the series irregular and unpleasant. The series became pleasant in proportion as it was uniform, and with this rate the subject perceived only a rhythmic rise and fall in intensity. Subject 16. Considerable musical talent and training. Time, .3 sec. His first suggestion was that every other one was stronger in intensity, the stronger one coming first in the group of two. For a time, the subject did not discover that the sounds were uniform. He could suggest a 4-group, in which the first and third were accented, the first stronger than the third. It was difficult to get a 5-group, but when the subject did, the accents were upon the first and third. The 2-groups might be grouped by fives, in which case the first and third 2-groups were more intense than the others; 4-groups of twos were accented upon the first and third 2-groups; 2 and 3-groups of twos were accented upon the first. Higher groups of 3-groups as far as five were possible. The accents were the same as for higher groups of twos. Three-groups of three were the most pleasing. Higher groups of four were more difficult. The accents could not be kept clear. From early childhood, the subject has observed and taken pleasure in the rhythms in the sounds of the fanning mill, feed cutter and other machinery. The 4-rhythm was the prevailing rhythm with him. The puffs of the locomotive are grouped by fours, the first and the third being accented, the first stronger than the third. He associates the pendulum with the 2-group. With the 4-group, he associates the locomotive or a wheel turning around, making four sounds to each revolution. The 3-group generally requires attention to keep it and a suggestion to begin. The 5-group breaks up into a 2-group and a 3-group. The 6-group generally divides into two 3-groups. Time, .176 sec. This rate seemed most favorable for a 6-group. It was composed of two 3-groups, the subject visualizing the pendulum which grouped the 3-groups by two. In general, the subject preferred short groups to long ones. The shorter groups were simpler. He preferred also his own accents to real accents. When he listened to the sound of the chronograph, which was distinctly rhythmical, he grouped the sounds accordingly. When he was dull and tired, faster rates were generally more satisfactory. The following rates were given in rapid succession: Time, .323 sec. The clicks were grouped most easily by the pendulum-swing movement. The subject could visualize a revolving wheel which made four strokes during each revolution and thus group by four. Time, .263 sec. The 4-grouping was decidedly pleasant and com- pelling. It required an effort of attention to group by three. He visualized the locomotive wheel with the 4-group. He could group the clicks by two with a pendulum- swing movement, but "it was RHYTHM. 57 too fast to be real natural." Time, .137 sec. This was " a train at full speed." The rate was more pleasant and enlivening than any previous rate. It required very little effort of attention to get either a 3 or 4-group. A 6-group was easily suggested, but the group divided easily into two 3-groups. Time, .208 sec. The 6-group was less easy than it was with the previous rate. Time, .137 sec. The clicks grouped readily by three or four. Higher grouping of 3-groups by two or three required a suggestion to start, and it seemed to continue of itself ; 4-groups might be grouped by the pendulum- swing movement. Every sixth was accented. The 6-grouping was necessary and pleasant. The accented sound took away the effort that had been required before for a 6-grouping. The 6-group might be divided into three 2-groups or two 3-groups. The accented sound always came at the beginning of the 6-group. Time, .167 sec. Every sixth was accented. The 6-group divided easily into three 2-groups or two 3-groups. Time, .323 sec. Every sixth was accented. The grouping was by two. The accented sound grouped the 2-groups by three. The span for a 6-group was disagreeable. It was too long. The accented sound might be over- looked and the series grouped by four. Time, .167 sec. Every sixth was accented. It was less easy to overlook the accented click than before. The accent forced a grouping by three. Time, .263 sec. It was most natural to group by two with the pendulum-swing. Time, .208 sec. A 4-group was most easy. When the subject heard the chronograph, which gave a 6-rhythm compounded of two 3-rhythms, he grouped the sounds accordingly. Time, 134 sec. A 3 or a 4-group was equally pleasant and easy. The sound of the chronograph, which now gave an 8-rhythm com- pounded of two 4-rhythms, compelled a grouping of the sounds accordingly. The following rates were given in rapid succession during a single experiment: Time, .268 sec. A 2 or a 4-group was easy. A 3-group could be suggested. Time, .208 sec. A 3- group was suggested, but a 2 or a 4-group was easier. Time, .17 sec. A 3 or a 4-group was equally pleasant and easy. There was no preference. Time, .116 sec. The series could be grouped by three or four. When every eighth was accented, the grouping was by eight. At first, the 8-group divided into two 4-groups. This disappeared, and the 8-group became pleasant and agreeable. Time, .134 sec. Every eighth was accented. The 8-group divided easily into two 4-groups. The span was too long. There was no satisfac- tion in the 8-group, for the accent did not come soon enough. Time, .208 sec. Every eighth was accented. This was distasteful. The feeling of suspense present before was greater still. Time, .268 sec. Every eighth was accented. The suspense was still greater, and the 8-group broke up into two 4-groups. Time, .116 sec. There was no accent. This rate, which had given before an agreeable 8-group, when every eighth was accented, yielded to an 8- grouping. There was a slight tendency for the 8-group to divide into two 4-groups, the first of which was more emphatic. Subject 17. Some musical talent and training. Accustomed to introspective study. Time, .3 sec. In the first place, the grouping was by two, and almost immediately and without effort it changed to a 4-group. When each click was attended to separately, they all appeared to be of the same intensity. Suddenly the subject began to group by four. He felt a tendency to count it off to himself. Sometimes the 4-group appeared as two 2-groups. Then he thought there was an irregular interval — a difference in the time of the clicks. He then imagined that a fainter sound was heard between the actual clicks. 58 BOLTON : Each click was grouped with the fainter sound following it, and these groups grouped by two. Breathing seemed to accommodate itself to the 4- group; inhalation lasted during one group of four and exhalation during another. When every third was accented and time .208 sec, the subject felt a strong tendency to inhale dur- ing one group and exhale during another. Each group is attended with the feeling of having completed a member of the rhythm. The groups stand out as unities — as wholes — and as each group becomes complete, there is a striving for the next. The subject has a tendency to count the clicks by fours or other numbers. When he attempts to suggest a 3-group, the third click seems to repeat itself thus: 1, 2, 3, 3, — 1, 2, 3, 3, — 1, 2, 3, 3. He succeeded, however, in getting a real 3-group by counting and nodding the head with the accented click. When he attempted to group by five, the accents seemed to crowd along until it brought six into the group. The first three clicks seemed to come in the time of two and the rest were irregular. When he succeeded in get- ting a 5-group, it was accented upon the second. Time, .965 sec. The 2-group was the most natural, but it was imperfect. Time, 1.615 sec. The subject was able by strong effort to group by two, but the sounds seemed more naturally to appear uniform. Subject 18. No musical talent and no interest in music. Time, .352 sec. This was a very pleasant rate. Other rates seemed either too slow or too fast. By no suggestion could any kind of grouping of the sounds be effected. The subject declared that they were all uniform in intensity. Subject 19. Some musical talent and in training at the public school. Time, .268 sec. The subject likened the series to dropping water. It was suggested to him that perhaps some sounds were louder than others, when he said that every fourth seemed louder. Again it was suggested that possibly every third was louder, but the subject would not agree to it. When every third was strongly accented, the grouping was by three. When the accent was dropped out, the subject returned to a 4-group. When he listened to the sound of the chronograph, which was making a double 3-rhythm, he grouped the sounds accordingly. Subject 20. Some musical talent and good training. Time, .268 sec. The sound was likened to dropping water. It was suggested that the clicks grouped together in some way, and the subject replied that they were grouped by four. Again it was sug- gested that some other grouping was possible. This, the subject said, was by three. After reflecting and counting for a moment, the grouping was thus: 1, 2, 3, 1—1, 2, 3, 1—1, 2, 3, 1. The first and third were accented in the 4-group. Subject 21. Physicist. Time, .30 sec. The sound suggested the pendulum. A loud click corresponded to one swing and a soft to the other. He visualized a conical pendulum, which struck at several points in its swing and thus grouped the sounds by other numbers than two. He seemed to attend now to the series of clicks and then to relax and attend again. During the " strains of attention," he might grasp three or four clicks. A feeling of relief followed each strain of the attention. All the muscles of the body seemed to point toward the source of the sound. They alternately contract and relax with the successive strains of the attention. The first click in each group was accented. Subject 22. Some musical talent. RHYTHM. 59 Time, .3 sec. The clicks were grouped by four. Time, .78 sec. This rate was too slow for any grouping. It did not even suggest the clock tick. Time, .156 sec. This rate was too fast for easy grouping in any way. Subject 23. Time, .268 sec. The prevailing group was four. It was difficult to suggest any other. The sound of the chronograph, which gave a 6-rhythm compounded of two 3-rhythms, was scarcely sufficient to break down the tendency to group by four. The subject had worked in the same room with the chronograph, and had become more accustomed to the 8-rhythm than to any other which the chronograph made. Subject 24. Time, .268 sec. The clicks grouped immediately by two. There seemed to be a difference in quality. When every fourth was accented, they were grouped by four. A longer interval preceded the accented click. When every eighth was accented, the clicks were grouped by eight and a longer interval preceded the louder sound. Subject 25. Some musical talent and training. Subject has noticed his tendency to group objects and sounds before the experiment. Objects passing rapidly before the eyes are grouped by eight, those passing slower, by four, and those passing very slowly, by two. Time, .78 sec. Every other sound appeared to be of sharper tone than the rest. The sharper toned click grouped with a weaker and came first. Time, .115 sec. He grouped by eight. When he gave attention to the pulse, he seemed not to hear the clicks coming near or just after the heart-beat. The clicks between the heart-beats were more distinct. No grouping of the sounds would persist long. The accented sound in the group generally came first, but it might come anywhere in the group. Subject 26. Some musical talent and training. Laboratory boy. Time, .323 sec. The most natural form of grouping was by two. The first was accented. When he suggested a 3-group, the rate seemed to be slower, and then the clicks seemed to be of the same intensity. When every third was accented, the accented click came first in the group, and was preceded by a longer interval. In whatever position an accented click stood, it was preceded by a longer interval. With uniform sounds the 4-group was accented upon the first and third ; the first was stronger than the third. Subject 27. Some musical talent and training. Time, .3 sec. The most natural form of grouping was by two. The first was accented. He was able to suggest groupings by three or four. The first sound in either group was accented. By tapping with five fingers, and striking much harder with the fifth he was able to suggest a 5-group. It seemed to be a matter of the imagi- nation largely whether there was a rhythm. When he thought of a clock or some other rhythmical machine, the series tended to group according to the suggestion. The sound was most naturally associated with dropping water. Subject 28. No musical talent. Time, .536 sec. It was possible to group the series by three, four or five. The 4-group was most natural. From early childhood the subject has observed the 4-rhythm in the puffing of the locomotive especially, and in later years the same rhythm has been observed in clocks, metronome, hammering, walking, and in all auditory impressions that approach a regularity in sequence. The rhythm is clearest in the sound of the locomotive. The first and third 60 BOLTON : sounds in the group are accented ; the first is generally more strongly accented than the third. When the sounds of the loco- motive become very rapid there is no definite grouping, simply a periodic rise and fall in intensity. Time, .268 sec. This was especially favorable for the 4-group, and the 3-group could not be easily suggested. Time, .208 sec. The 4-group was most natural. It was possible to suggest a 2-group by striking heavily on every other sound. The grouping, however, was very monotonous. Both the 3 and the 5-group were very difficult. Time, .268 sec. Although the 3-group was difficult at this rate before, it could be easily sug- gested this time. Time, .17 sec. The clicks were grouped by four and the 4- groups tended to group by two with the pendulum- swing movement. If the grouping was held down to a plain four, it be- came unpleasantly monotonous. Time, .134 sec. The series tended to appear in the form of a periodic rise and fall in intensity. The periods were about equal to the time of an 8-group, and with a slight voluntary effort the series grouped by eight. The 8-groups tended to group by two with the pendulum- swing movement. During a subsequent experiment with the same rate, the subject felt a ten- sion in the eye muscles which grouped the series by eight ; four sounds occurred during the upward movement and four during the downward. Subject 29. Some musical talent and training. Time, .268 sec. When the subject thinks of a clock the series groups by two, but when he thinks of hammering, the clicks appear to be of the same intensity. He could suggest a 3 or 4-group, but the 2-group was most natural. Time, .208 sec. He finds it easy to count almost any rhythm as far as nine. The longer rhythms tend to divide into shorter ones. The subject found it difficult to keep from thinking of a clock tick, which suggested the 2-group. Time, .17 sec. The subject still grouped by two and thought the rate seemed to be faster when he grouped by two than when he suggested other groups. Subject 30. No musical talent. Time, .268 sec. By no suggestion was it possible for the subject to effect any kind of grouping of the sounds. It appeared as a dead monotonous series, with which he could not avoid the association of a pile-driver. Many other persons who simply came in as visitors, were experi- mented upon with results which confirmed the foregoing records. No especial account was taken of them. More than fifty persons in all were experimented upon, and only two failed to effect some kind of grouping in the clicks which they heard. In general it may be said that the younger and less educated yielded more easily and quickly to the suggestion of a rhythmical grouping. The first point in the preceding records to which attention is called is the rhythmical grouping of the sounds. The grouping was the same in every case. It was accomplished by accenting regularly certain sounds more than others. The weaker or less accented sounds seem to run together with the stronger, and to form organic groups which are separated from one another by intervals which are apparently longer than the interval which separates the individual clicks. Such rhythmical grouping has been observed frequently at other times by many persons. Several of the subjects testify RHYTHM. 61 -to have known of their tendency to group the puffs of the locomotive, even in early childhood, and they have taken great delight in it. With us this habit of grouping the puffs of the locomotive when it was starting slowly or pulling up a grade became so strong, even in early childhood, that it led to all kinds of speculation as to the cause. The puffs are grouped by four. The first and third are accented, the first generally stronger than the third. No other grouping ever seemed possible until it was found in the experimental work that the tendency to group by four was only a habit or association. The puffs of a locomotive may now be grouped by two or three, but the association of the drive-wheel making one revolution to four sounds renders any other form of grouping than by four difficult. When the engine runs very fast, the sounds seem to rise and fall in intensity at regular intervals. A kind of rhythm is also observed in the noise of mill- wheels. The winnowing machine and feed cutter, such as are found upon many farms, produce a rhythmical sound which few persons fail to observe. Long association in early childhood with such rhythms stamps them upon the mind so firmly that they become a mental habit. Children either fancy or perceive rhythms in many sounds ; they indicate this by their attempts to reproduce the sound of machinery or of locomotives. Some railroad engineers believe their engines sing tunes. The same engine under like circum- stances always sings the same tune. Several experimenters have also observed this same group- ing of rhythmic sounds. In the work undertaken by Dietze 1 in Wundt's laboratory upon the Umfang of con- sciousness, this rhythmical grouping of the sounds of the metronome was observed and employed to determine the length of the mental span. The grouping was accomplished by intensifying voluntarily certain sounds and subordinating others to it. By grouping the sounds first by eight and then the groups of eight by five, it was possible to grasp forty sounds. Wundt says it is impossible to restrain this group- ing absolutely. It may be confined to a 2-group, beyond which it cannot go within certain limits. Four sec. is the lower limit, and .11 sec. is the upper limit. The most favorable rate is .2 to .3 sec. Wundt refers this grouping to the ripen- ing of the concept on the wave of apperception. As we shall see later, it is possible to restrain this tendency to group sounds. The difficulty was with Wundt's apparatus. The two sounds heard during a complete swing of the pendulum 1 "Wundt, Physiologische Psychologie, Vol. II. p. 73. 62 BOLTON : of the metronome are not of the same intensity or quality, and hence the impossibility of restraining the grouping by two. Angell and Pierce, x in their experiments upon attention, state that one subject noticed a rhythm in the sounds with which he felt a tendency to muscular contraction — nodding of the head and beating time with the fingers. In neither of these experiments could the experimenters be sure that there was not some difference in the sound which would suggest a rhythm. The importance of an absolutely uniform series of sounds cannot be too strongly insisted upon. A difference in sounds which would ordinarily remain unnoticed, is sufficient to suggest a rhythm. This will be seen when we come to discuss the voluntary changes of the grouping and the ease of suggesting such a change. In the present experiments the greatest precaution was used against any variation in the sounds that would suggest or impose a grouping. The only possible source of such a variation would come from a difference in the resistance between the mercury and platinum. If the mercury were dirty or the platinum points were not sufficiently immersed to form a good contact, or the mercury were to adhere to the points as they were withdrawn, a difference in the intensity of the sound might be heard. The mercury was carefully cleaned every few days, or fresh mercury put in. The platinum points were filed smooth and kept brushed. Strong elastics were attached to each key, so that when the keys were released there was no delay about reacting. If then there were any varia- tions, since there were five sets of keys, it ought to recur every fifth sound ; but as a 5-rhythm was always found very difficult, and a 2, 3 and 4-rhythm easy, we have strong ground for believing that any variations except those which were intended were so small as to have no influence upon the rhythmical grouping. We have, then, the testimony of all the subjects that the clicks seemed uniform in intensity. Subject 2 always heard a uniform series for a time after a change of rate, or at the beginning of a new experiment. His tendency to group was so strong that he could avoid it only by imagining some one pounding in the distance, or some objective thing that was perfectly uniform. Sub- ject 3 did not feel any tendency to group the sounds until after he had tried several suggestions. Subject 9, taking a critical attitude, was inclined to believe for a time that the clicks were all of the same intensity. After a few moments it required an effort, which was like " looking long into the American Journal op Psychology, Vol. IV. pp. 534 and 539.. KEYTHM. 63 future," to avoid a grouping. " I find no rhythm," he says, " as long as I hold my breath and stick to it." When sub- ject 11 gave close and critical attention to the sound, there was no grouping. In order to get a notion of a rhythmic series — one of uniform intensity — subject 12 turned his atten- tion " backward " and saw a series of images to which he was adding one all the time. He throws his attention upon what comes, and studies the nature of the sounds to see if the timbre remains the same. Subject 1 7 says that when each click was attended to separately, they all appeared to be of the same intensity. He said he experienced no such difficulty in avoiding a rhythm as the statement of Wundt had led him to suppose. Subject 25 could group the sounds, but he was more inclined not to do so. If he suggested a grouping, it did not persist. Subject 27 found it more natural to associate the sound with dripping water. Subject 29 made the series appear uniform when he thought of hammering. Subjects 18 and 30 could not effect any grouping at all. Upon this evidence we may safely rely upon having secured a series of impressions that was uniform for sensation. It is also true that though the rhythmical grouping of a series of uniform sounds is difficult to avoid, this tendency may be restrained within the limits spoken of by Wundt. Our own experience tallies with those above. When the attention is directed to each single impression, and an attempt made to study the timbre, it is possible to restrain the rhythmical grouping of the sounds. But when the series is attended to as a whole, this grouping takes place involuntarily. The character of the sound employed in the experiments of Dietze differed greatly from that used in these experiments. The click of the telephone is about as simple and instantaneous a sound as it is possible to produce. The plate in the telephone vibrates a very short time. For that reason its chief characteristic is intensity ; it does not persist long enough to establish its pitch and timbre. The mind has very little to work upon. It can construct variations only in intensity, for which reason the carrying power is greatly reduced. The sounds can be subordinated with respect to intensity only, and unless great intensive variations can be made, the mind will lose its grasp, and the grouping break up into single impressions. This phenomenon was observed several times, and in particular by subject 15. The sound of the metronome which Deitze employed is full and rich and has greater carrying power. Any experiments upon the carrying power of the mind must take into consideration the character of the sound. Dietze was able, by strong voluntary effort, to carry the grouping much farther than any subject in 64 BOLTON : this experiment was able to do with the clicks of the tele- phone. The explanation is to be found partly in a differ- ence between the two sounds and partly in a different method. The subject in these experiments was requested to group the sounds, not by voluntary effort, but only so far as it was found easy and spontaneous. There was no attempt to force the grouping as far as possible, or even to force the grouping at all. It was the spontaneous and involuntary grouping that was studied. In a study like this, which is purely introspective, the experimenter must rely upon the integrity of his subjects. There is and can be no test of the accuracy or truth of the results, except the uniformity which they show. If, however, each subject is unaware of the object to be obtained by the experiment, and of the opinions of every other subject, and renders his judgment without any interest in the results or without any preconceived notions of the experiment, the judgments are no more subject to error, and have about the same value as judgments in psycho-physical experi- ments. Certain attitudes, habits, and characteristics of mind do, however, affect results in certain ways which are injurious to the experiment. Some attention was paid to the attitude and method of the subjects in making judgments. A few words in regard to this may not be out of place. There are three classes of psychological subjects. The first includes those persons who yield immediately to any suggestion that is offered. This attitude results, then, from a social practice. In society, people do not wish to antagonize others. They instinctively give assent to any opinion. In an experimental investigation, if the operator will just give the slightest hint of his theory or preference they will add the weight of their opinions. If the operator leads them into giving an opinion which is opposed to his theory, ' ' consistency becomes a jewel;" they stick to their opinion stoutly. If the experi- ment shows plainly that they are wrong and it is preposterous to hold such a view, they make a compromise with their former position and try to excuse themselves for having been led astray. They remain respectfully silent afterward and avoid, if possible, giving an opinion. If they are forced to make a judgment, they do it tentatively ; they are not sure. Of a number of possible views they cannot make up their minds which is the correct one. They generally hair- split until they find out someone's opinion and then agree with that. The second class of subjects includes those who take a mod- erately critical attitude. They are concerned in others' opinions in so far only as other • opinions suggest different points of view. They give their own opinions when they RHYTHM. 65 have considered all the phases of the experiment that are sug- gested to them. They are unconcerned about the outcome of the experiment. They are not dogmatic ; they might have a different opinion under different circumstances or with further consideration. In the light of the evidence before them, they hold to a certain view. The third class includes those persons who are excessively critical. They incline always to an opposite view. The experiment is not conducted properly to suit them ; they are not in their best mood for judgment. They are sure to take ground against some one's opinion. If they cannot get any clue to others' opinions, they are doggedly silent or quibble, and refuse to answer except they qualify their answers to such an extent that the answer means nothing. This class of subjects is intellectually dishonest. If they are compelled to answer, they indulge in hair-splitting differences between their opinions and those of some others. When the experimenter is compelled to rely entirely upon the judgment of his subjects, he must study them carefully and use the opinions of certain subjects in so far only as he finds that they harmonize with the general results. It is a fact which every psychologist must understand that certain classes of persons are incapable of introspection. The first class to which we referred are unfitted, because of habits of too free judgment and of always agreeing with others. The third class are rendered unfit for introspection from habits of too free judgment in regard to matters that concern themselves, and from an unnatural bias toward the negative. They are inclined to make too much of their individual opinions. In making out the results, the investigator cannot rely much upon individual opinion. Where there is almost perfect uniformity, the results may be given in tabulated form ; but a large space must be given to merely individual opinion. We have then to inquire first in regard to the certainty of a rhythmical grouping of a series of absolutely uniform sounds. The point does not need argument ; the preceding records show how strong is this tendency. Only three out of fifty or more persons tested would agree that it was easier to hear each click separately. In addition to the records given above, several subjects were asked to give a written statement of their impressions of the experiment. In one case definite questions were asked in writing. (A) u As far as I can recall my impressions at the different occasions on which I listened to the series of sounds from your apparatus, they appeared to me always as a sequence of groups containing the same number of elements. The excep- tional cases where the impression was that of a sequence of 66 BOLTON : single sounds, were those in which the period of the sequence was at its longest. For any given rate there was in general one certain number of elements of which the groups more naturally consisted than any other: but I found, too, that the sequence took on instantly the character of almost any other grouping that was suggested, whether by word or sound. As to the psychological nature of this phenomenon of grouping, it is a difficult matter to give an opinion. I found the effort to determine whether or no there were any recurrent differences of sensation in the sequence a great strain upon the powers of attention. The grouping had in general the appearance of being forced on the mind by the sounds rather than that of being imposed on them by it." (B) "A series of clicks may be given in such manner that by giving the closest possible attention they seem to be uniform both as to intensity and interval. This degree of tension (of attention) can, however, be maintained for only a few seconds. When the attention is moderate, the clicks tend to fall into rhythmic groups, the number of clicks falling into a group varying with the rate of the clicks. Slower than a cer- tain rate no rhythm is felt. With more rapid rates two clicks form a group, the accent falling on the first and an interval occurring after the second. Faster still, four clicks form a group with accent, primary on the first and secondary on the third, and an interval after the fourth. This seems a very pleasing rhythm to me, more so than any other. A still more rapid rate gives eight in a group. This becomes visualized quite strongly in my case. It is exceedingly difficult for me to hold the series of clicks out of some of these rhythms. They fall into one or the other types (according to rate) almost irresistibly. At some rates I was able to get a 3-rhythm, accented strongly on the first." (C) " With regular ticks within certain limits, I do not perceive them as distinct separate ticks, but from the first I group. With slower rates, the grouping is two by two, which passes very easily into four, subdivided into two. With faster rates, the tendency is to perceive the grouping into fours, divided into two, or to perceive the grouping into threes. The quicker the rate, the larger the number of ticks entering into the groups up to about six. Below the lower limit, the ticks are first perceived separately with a tendency to fall into twos, this tendency decreasing as the rate decreases. Above the upper limit, the grouping becomes vague and the tendency is to perceive the ticks as separate and individual. In general the grouping can be changed within certain limits." RHYTHM. 67 "The groupings influence one another. There is a tendency to become habituated to a grouping. A grouping heard in one rate is likely to repeat itself in a subsequent rate. It is difficult to be perfectly passive when one knows he is to find a rhythm. ' ' (D) ' ' It seems to me easier to group the clicks unless they are very slow ; but I do not find it so difficult to perceive them singly as I should have inferred from Wundt's remarks on Dietze's experiments. Having now tried many times when the grouping was strongly present, subjectively (voluntarily) or objectively, I think I am a little more inclined to discover groupings. It seems to me that I do not lengthen, but rather intensify one or more of the sounds. Perhaps, however, the change is more in quality than in intensity, or perhaps an accompanying impulse of the diaphragm, stress in the men- tal counting, etc., etc. Possibly, however, I do also lengthen the stressed sound at the same time; but the lengthening is not so clear as the stress. I infer from my experience as a subject that the rates from 1 or 2 per sec, up to 6 or 8 per sec. , are best ; probably about 4 per sec. being the best. The fast rates are better for groupings by four, the slower for groupings by two. Three-groups, 5-groups and higher groups do not occur spontaneously with me, though 3-groups are not hard to start by counting. Perhaps 2-groups go easiest of all with me. There is a sense of expectation of ' hope deferred ' when the rate is too slow — or, at least, a feeling of 'too slow,' like traveling in a slow train, although you have plenty of time. 1 ' This probably increases with the length of the group. The span of the respiratory rhythm is exceeded, and instead of being able to tell off a whole foot of the rhythm with one breath, several breaths intervene between those that mark the accented sounds. With small groups and rapid rates there is a feeling of hurry. The motion is too quick and short. There is none of the repose — the swaying, the grace, the easy fulfill- ment of expectation that a slower rhythm possesses." This rhythmical grouping was a series of efforts to attend to the sound. The grouping results from a sequence of acts of attention. When the attention is directed to the sensation, it lays hold upon the first impression with great force and makes it the sole object of consciousness. If this were the only sound, the attention would turn to something else, but as succeeding impressions follow before the first wave of attention has subsided, they are seized upon with less force than the first impression, and are subordinated to it in differ- ent degrees according to the strength of the apperceptive act. Subsequent waves of attention follow the same process as lon^ 68 BOLTON : as the will directs the attention to the phenomenon. The attention accommodates itself to a certain number of impres- sions, which fall easily within the period of a wave, providing nthere is no objective difference in the impressions. If there is a regularly recurrent difference, this becomes the signal for a new act of attention , providing only that the span does not exceed or fall much under the normal period of a wave. If this recurrent difference follows at too great intervals, the attention breaks up the span in two portions, the one more emphatic than the other. If it follows at too small intervals, these periods fall together into group, first of two and then of larger numbers. The too great interval is marked by a feel- ing of suspense, and the too short interval by a straining after something more. The number of uniform elements which may enter into a member of the sequence is not determined wholly by the time interval which separates them. Previous mental habits and associations influence the number of elements in the members of the sequence. All individuals are more habituated to two and its multiples than they are to three. There are also many associations which will suggest groupings by two and four. All ordinary muscular movements follow a rhythm of two. The associations of four are far more frequent than those of three. For this reason to a large extent, groups of two and four prevail. Several subjects have described this effort of attention in a manner which deserves notice and which shows very well the nature of the act. Subject 7, speaking of his grouping by eight, says he is not able to ' ' round up ' ' until he comes to eight. There was feeling of completeness about the 8 -group with a certain rate. Subject 9 says there is a slight feeling of muscle tension in the ear, sometimes in the back of the scalp. He attends, relaxes, and attends again. There is an innervation of the muscles connected with attention. Subject 12 describes his feeling about the grouping as a tendency " to go back " when he has heard three or four clicks, as the case may be. This is a *' mouthful " — a unity, and, when he has one, he seeks to get another. Subject 11 describes his feeling as a series of efforts •of attention. He grasps and grasps again Subject 17 says ■each group is attended with a feeling of having v completed a member of the rhythm. The groups stand out as unities — as wholes — and as each group becomes complete, there is a striving for the next. Subject 21: "I attend now to the series of clicks, then relax and attend again. During the strains of attention, I may grasp three or four clicks. A feeling of relief follows each strain of attention. All the mus- RHYTHM. 69 cles of the body seem to point toward the source of the sound. They alternately contract and relax," This is the rhythm in the attention to which a reference was made above. The view taken, then, was that only one undi- vided state of consciousness might arise during each pulse or wave of attention, and that the number of objects which can be grasped in that state must form an organic unity or be pre- sented as a single object — have the appearance of a unit. A given number of auditor]/ impressions within certain- time limits, when presented in such a ivay that there is a kind of subordination among them with respect either to time, intensity, pitch or quality, or with respect to any two or more of these properties, always stand as a unit in consciousness. They form an organic unity which is the essential condition of a number of impressions entering into a state of consciousness. If such organic unity does not exist and it is possible to make it, the mind imposes such an arrangement upon a given number of the elements that they may enter into a state of consciousness. The essential con- ditions of forming such a unity among sounds is a regular temporal sequence within limits which shall be named here- after, and perfect uniformity in intensity, pitch and quality. Eegular variations within limits with respect to intensity,. pitch or quality, or to any two, or to all of these together, will effect a subordination among them sufficient to constitute an organic unity. There is a temporal limit within which these variations must occur in order to form such a unity. The test of how many auditory impressions might be grouped together was the ease and pleasure which the subject found in doing so. If he were compelled to keep up a con- stant suggestion of a particular number in order to group the clicks so, noaccount was taken of it. If, after suggesting a grouping, it Should persist until some other suggestion was made, the rate was considered favorable for that form of group- ing. The subjects have described some groupings as most natural, easy or pleasurable, and others difficult or displeas- ing. The groupings which were spoken of as natural, easy or pleasurable, are gathered together in the following table, with the time, to determine what rates have been found best adapted to the different forms of grouping. In the following table are brought together the judgments of all those subjects with whom extensive observations were made. The number of the subject is given in the first column at the left hand, and in the columns to the right are given the rates in thousandths of a second, at which a certain form of grouping was found pleasant and easy. The designations at the top of columns 1, 2, 5, 8, 10 and 13 are sufficiently clear. 70 BOLTON : The others require further explanation. In column 3 are given those rates at which the subjects found a 2-group more easy, but there was a straining for a larger group, or the 2-groups seemed to group by two. The rate was a little fast for a 2-group, and yet it was not more pleasant to group by four. In the same way certain rates were found at which a 3, 4, 6 or 8 -group was easier than any other, but it was a little too fast for simply grouping by these numbers, and hence the groups tended to group by two. This was generally spoken of as the " pendulum -swing movement." Still other rates were found at which a 4, 6 or 8-group was more pleasant, and yet the rate was too slow, and the group tended to divide into two smaller groups. In column 15 are given those rates at which there was no distinct grouping — simply a periodic intensive change in the series. Bates at which there was no appearance of a group are given in column 16. Multiplying the average rate for each form of grouping by the number of clicks in a group, we get as the length of groups : Lower limit for no group, 1.581 sec. Average variation, .29 sec. Average length of 2-groups, 1.590 " a (< .328 " it a a g it 1.380 " tt tt .204 " n tt u a u 1.228 " a it .068 " it It It Q tl 1.014 " tt it .028 " a tt it g a 1.160 " a a .025 " The foregoing table shows that the lower limit for the rhythmical grouping of sounds is near 1.58 sec. Some sub- jects are able at times to group sounds that are separated by this interval, but as a general rule spontaneous grouping has ceased. The records give several instances where the sub- ject has visualized the pendulum with this rate, but he had a feeling that the pendulum reached its full swing before he heard the click. The upper limit at which spontaneous rhythmical grouping ceases cannot be far from .115 sec. Several subjects declared their inability to make definite groups at a rate less than this. Others perceived only a periodic rise and fall in the intensity of the sound ; there was no definite grouping. Between these limits there was some form of rhythmical grouping which depended in a large measure upon the rate. The average of all the rates at which a grouping by two was found easy and natural has been taken and multiplied by two to find the average length in time for the 2-groups. The same has been done for groups of three, four, six and eight. The averages for groups of all forms are found not to differ greatly, when we consider certain facts which influence the length of the group. The average length of 2 and 3- groups is somewhat greater than the average for groups of 1 2 3 4 ■3.5 5 6 7 ■ejB 8 9 10 11 12 II -3+j 13 14 15 16 h 32 is 4a to £ fe ■» D fe CO P. 03 b 03.S 00 Til '& >> in O 03 ft i • 3 & ft' ft ft ft ft„Q ft— ft ft ftj? ft ft,c ft"" ft ft fta •u X be OOP 3 S 3 o u 3ft 3 © 3 0,3 3 O t. So. £3 3 ® 3 Org O 3 O 2ft P3 3 i% 282 3 O §§■ 3 02 C5 oy £S >0 © 9^ >o O T>" O G? 3 >a O s / ABC ABC ABC 6 groups. | ACB ACB ACB 4-groups. ACBC ACBC ACBC Of series of four intensities, the following out of the many possible forms were tried : m™™. / ABCDABCD -groups. | ADCBADCB In the following table are given the rhythmical groups which each subject made of the series upon which he was tried. At the top of the table, in each column, are letters which indicate the order in which the different intensities recurred in the various series. The number of each subject is given in the left-hand column. If a subject has given a stronger intensity to a click than it actually possessed (said that a B intensity was equal to an A, or a C to a B), it is printed in full-faced type. Where a subject has remarked upon a longer interval, either following or preceding the strongest so and, this is indicated by placing a dash either before or after the strong sound : PQ pq PA PA <3 o opq O i i Opq Opq o i o O 53 6 < < < pq «jo < ^