DUKE HOSPITAL Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2016 https://archive.org/details/anatomyofbrain01spur THE ANATOMY OF THE BRAIN, WITH A GENERAL VIEW c; / ' k* v- v. mJJ OF THE NERVOUS SYSTEM. BY G. SPURZHEIM, M.D. OP THE UNIVERSITIES OP VIENNA AND PARIS; LICENTIATE OF THE ROYAL COI.LEOE OP PHYSICIANS IN LONDON. TRANSLATED FROM THE UNPUBLISHED FRENCH MS. By R. WILLIS, MEMBER OP THE ROYAL COLLEGE OP SURGEONS IN LONDON. WITH ELEVEN PLATES. LONDON : PUBLISHED BY S. HIGHLEY, 174, FLEET-STREET, and Webb- Street , St. Thomas’s Hospital ; SOLD ALSO BY HILL AND SON, EDINBURGH; AND CHARLES ARCHER, DAME-STREET, DUBLIN. MDCCCXXVI. LONDON: PRINTED BY WILLIAM CLOWES, Northumberlanc -court. Preface ......... vii Section I. — General Considerations ... 1 Of the Existence of the Pulpy and Fibrous Sub- stances ........ 2 Of the Structure of the Two Nervous Substances . 3 Of the Use of the Pulpy Nervous Substance . 6 Of the Origin of the Nervous System ... 10 Of the Mode of Formation of the Nervous System . 16 Of the General Form of the Nervous System . IS Of the Structure and Use of Ganglions . . .19 Section II. — Division of the Masses composing the Nervous System ..... 23 Section III. — Of the Nervous Masses of Voluntary Motion, and of the External Senses . . 36 Of the Nervous Mass of the Vertebral Column . 50 Of the Pretended Cerebral Nerves ... 68 Of the Quadrigeminal Bodies .... S3 Section IV. — Of the Best Method of Dissecting the Brain ........ 95 Section V. — Of the Cerebellum . . . 106 Section VI. — Of the Brain ..... 143 Decussation of the Anterior Pyramids . . 146 Two portions of the Crura and Cerebral Masses 153 VI CONTENTS. PAGE Comparative Anatomy of tlie Brain . . . 159 Structure of the Convolutions . . . .163 Section VII. — Of the Commissures of the Nervous Apparatuses . . . . . . 178* Section VIII. — Of the Communications of the Ner- vous Apparatuses . . . . .195 Section IX. — Of the Anatomico-Physiological Rela- tions of the Nervous Apparatuses . . 204 Explanation of the Plates and Figures . . .216 PREFACE. The affective and intellectual faculties of man, both in their healthy and diseased condition, are unquestionably dependent on the body ; and, among the various branches of anthro- pology, anatomy is the basis of all the others. The organic apparatuses, which are indis- pensable to the mental manifestations, con- sist of the brain, cerebellum, spinal cord, and the nerves of the external senses and vo- luntary motion. To make known the struc- ture of these parts is the special object of this volume. History proves that the structure and des- tination of the nerves were long unknown. Hippocrates and Aristotle, for instance, con- founded, under the same general title, liga- ments, tendons, nerves, and even blood-ves- sels. Hippocrates believed that the nerves terminated in muscles and bones, and pro- duced voluntary motion. Herophilus, who lived nearly three centuries before the com- Viii PREFACE. mencement of the vulgar era, was the first who discovered the connexion of the nerves with the brain, and who looked on them as the instruments of sensation. Erasistratus divided the nerves into those of sensation and those of motion ; the first he derived from the brain, the second from the membranes. Galen held that the nerves of sensation arose from the brain, and those of voluntary mo- tion from the spinal cord. In the sixteenth and succeeding centuries the brain and nerves were subjects of much research, but it is only in our own times that they have begun to be understood, — that their true structure has been discovered, and that new and unthought- of functions have been proved to belong to them. The nervous being the most delicate tis- sues of the body necessarily required ex- tremely careful and often-repeated examina- tion to be understood, and this they could not receive during ages when prejudice op- posed insurmountable obstacles to the dissec- tion of dead bodies. It is therefore easy to conceive why such slow progress was made in the anatomical knowledge of the nervous system. PREFACE. IX Dr. Gall is the original author of a new physiological doctrine of the brain. The discovery of the ground-work of this is all his own, and he had even gone very far in rearing the superstructure before the year 1804, when I became his colleague. From this period we continued labouring in common until 1813, when our connexion ceased, and each began to pursue the subject for himself. The works which Dr. Gall has published in his own name fix the extent of his phrenolo- gical knowledge. My ideas, too, are deve- loped in my own publications : history v/ill assign to each of us his share in the works that have issued under our joint names. It was in the year 1800 that I attended for the first time the private course of lectures which Dr. Gall had been in the habit of deli- vering occasionally at his house for four years. At this time he spoke of the necessity of the brain to the manifestations of mind, of the plurality of the mind’s organs, and of the possibility of discovering the development of the brain by the configuration of the head. He pointed out several particular organs of different memories, and of several sentiments, but he had not yet begun to examine the X PREFACE. structure of the brain*. Between 1800 and 1804 he modified his physiological ideas, and brought them to the state in which he pro- fessed them at the commencement of our travels •f. Dr. Gall having met with a woman, fifty- four years of age, who from her infancy had laboured under dropsy of the brain, and who, nevertheless, was as active and intelligent as the generality of females in her own rank of life, and being convinced that the brain was the indispensable organ of the soul, expressed himself in terms similar to those which Tul- pius had used before him, on observing a person afflicted with hydrocephalus, who ex- hibited good intellectual faculties, viz., the structure of the brain must be different from what it is commonly supposed to be. He now felt the necessity of examining the mind’s organ anatomically. As his medical practice occupied his time, he employed M. Niclas, a student, to dissect for him ; but the * Exposition cle la Doctrine de M. Gall, par Froniep, 3mc edit. 1802. t Bisclroff Exposition de la Doctrine de M. Gall, sur le Cer- eau et le Crane, Berlin, 1S05 ; et Bloede, la Doctrine de Gall suv les Fonctions du Cerveau, Dresden, 1805. PREFACE. XI spirit of this gentleman’s researches was merely mechanical, as is allowed in our joint work, entitled “ Anatomie et Physiologie du Systeme nerveux en general, et du Cerveau en particular*.” Having completed my studies in 1804, I was associated with Dr. Gall, and devoted myself especially to anatomical inquiries. At this period, Dr. Gall, in the Anatomy, spoke of the decussation of the pyramidal bodies, of their passage through the pons Varolii, of eleven layers of longitudinal and transverse fibres in the pons, of the continuation of the optic nerve to the anterior pair of the qua- drigeminal bodies, of the exterior bundles of the crura of the brain diverging beneath the optic nerves in the direction which Vieussens, Monro, Vicq d’Azyr, and Reil *f* had followed, the first, by means of scraping, the others, by cutting the substance of the brain. Dr. Gall shewed further, the continuation of the anterior commissure across the striated bo- dies ; he also spoke of the unfolding of the brain that happens in hydrocephalus. The notion he had conceived of this, however, * Preface to the first vol. p. 16. f Gren’s Journal, 1795, i. p. 102. Xll PREFACE. was not correct ; for he thought that the con- volutions resulted from the duplicature of a membrane, believing that the cerebral crura entered the hemispheres on one side, expanded there, and then folded back on themselves by the juxtaposition of the convolutions. The true structure of the convolutions, and their connexion with the rest of the cerebral mass, were not described until our joint Me- moir was presented to the French Institute in 1808. The mechanical direction which the ana- tomical investigations had taken did not ap- pear to me satisfactory. Guiding myself in my inquiries by physiological views, always comparing structure with function, I disco- vered the law of the successive additions to the cerebral parts ; the divergence in every direction of the crural bundles towards the convolutions ; the difference between the di- verging fibres and those of union ; the gene- rality of commissures ; the true connexion of the convolutions with the rest of the cere- bral mass, and the peculiar structure which admits of the convolutions being unfolded (an event that occurs in hydrocephalus of the cavities), whilst the mass lying at their bot- PREFACE. Xlll toms, and belonging, for the most part, to the apparatus of union, or of the commis- sures, is pushed by the water between the two layers composing them ; lastly, I demon- strated the structure of the nervous mass of the spine, and I flatter myself with having arrived at the best method of dissecting the brain, and exposing its parts. What is my object then in publishing this volume ? Our large work is too expensive for the generality of medical students, and, further, the method pursued in the discus- sions there is only calculated for professed anatomists ; whilst this book will be both less costly, and it will be adapted to the stu- dent as well as to the more advanced anato- mist. Moreover, many new ideas, possess- ing a great share of interest, may now be added ; for since Dr. Gall and I first pub- lished, the study of the nervous system has engaged the especial attention of anatomists and physiologists. I have, myself, continued inquiring, and conceive that I have made se- veral new discoveries. I have, however, copied some passages from the first volume of the large work already mentioned, and also given reduced drawings of several of its xiv PREFACE. plates ; because I think I have acquired a right to this volume, by its publication in our joint names, by my discoveries that form its principal object, and by all I did in further- ance of its publication. All the drawings were executed under my superintendence from anatomical preparations, made and de- termined on by me ; the engraver worked by my directions ; no plate was sent to press without my approval ; the descriptions of the plates, and the anatomical details are mine ; and I furnished the literary notices in regard to the nerves of the abdomen and thorax, to those of the vertebral column, of the five senses, of the cerebellum, and of the brain. Whoever desires more copious historical details than this volume will be found to contain, I refer to our Memoir, addressed to the French Institute, and to the first volume of our great work, commenced in common, and continued .by Dr. Gall singly after the middle of the second volume. The influence our labours have had on the study of the nervous system is incontestable. To be convinced of this, it is enough to ex- amine the state of knowledge in regard to the anatomy, physiology, and pathology of the PREFACE. XV brain and spinal marrow, when Dr. Gall and I developed our ideas on these matters, whether it was by teaching orally, by dis- secting publicly, or by means of our writings. I confess there is great satisfaction in the consciousness of having contributed to the important reform that has been effected in regard to the nervous system. I am only sorry to observe, that many of our ideas are appropriated by the authors of recent publi- cations, without any mention of the source whence they were derived, or of the indivi- duals who first struck them out, or reduced them to certainty by direct proofs. We are commonly enough mentioned, it is true, when such of our assertions as appear weak are the subjects of criticism, but our names are kept in the background when points of importance become the matter of discussion. The public, for instance, by referring to the proper place, may judge whether Mr. J. Cloquet, in his £t Anatomy of Man/’ has been sufficiently explicit in stating, that he has copied every one of the plates of the human brain contained in our large work. M. Ser- res, whose Memoir was deemed worthy of its prize by the Academy of Sciences of XVI PREFACE. Paris, in the first volume of his work, uses our names no fewer than fifteen times, in connexion with a single idea which he fancies he can refute; and generally along with every fact that looks unfavourable to our opinion, he names us, but he always forgets to cite us in relation to very many fundamental conceptions which we had an- nounced at the same time. They who have written to the following effect:— “ M. Serres has proved clearly the erroneousness of M. Gall’s observations, and replaced them by others,” may undeceive themselves by at- tending to the remark I have just made. M. Serres’s publication forces me likewise to request the reader to distinguish between a multitude of words and facts on the one hand, and the corollaries which result on the other. I agree with those who, in works of science, pay especial regard to truths de- monstrable to others, to ideas available in practical life, and to clearness and simplicity of style. To what purpose may serve the following passage, which occurs in the pre- liminary discourse of M. Serres, where, after having said that a monster may be a vegetation of its like, that it may have PREFACE. XVII two heads, two tails, and six or eight ex- tremities, but that it would remain strictly confined to the limits of its class, he ex- claims : — “ This wonderful 'phenomenon is un- doubtedly connected with the general harmony of creation. What may be its cause ? We know it not, and in all likelihood we shall re- main ignorant of it for ever. It is one of the mysteries of creation , whose surface is meted by man, but whose depths are sounded, and known to God alone This phenomenon does not appear to me more extraordinary than that a kitten is not a puppy, or that the crab-tree does not pro- duce pears. If the egg of a bird in its or- dinary state cannot produce a mammiferous animal, why should the germ of this same egg, if it chance to be imperfectly developed, produce a deformity like to one of the mam- malia? Were the case thus, there would be some cause for an amazement, but the univer- sal fact of every animal producing its kind, * “ Cetetonnant phdnomene est sans doute lid a l’harmonie generale de la creation. Quelle peut en etre la cause ? Nous l’ignorons et vraisemblablement nous l’ignorerous toujours. C’est un des mysteres de la creation, dont l’homine mesurela surface, mais dont Dieu seul sonde et connait la profondeur.” XV111 PREFACE. is not, in my eyes, more astonishing than any other natural event. Further, the mass of facts cited, the num- ber of dissections made, ought never to im- pose on us, nor be made a means of concealing the truth. Many of the anatomists who had lived before us dissected some hundreds of brains, and they made a boast of their doings in this way ; but they did not perceive that which I pledge myself to have discovered before I had dissected a dozen ; for instance, the successive additions to the cerebral parts, and the two kinds of fibres, to wit, the di- verging, and the fibres of union. Anatomists and physiologists had certainly looked upon heads without number ; but before Dr. Gall’s appearance, had failed to discover the seat of a single cerebral organ. A solitary indi- vidual, a beggar, enabled him to detect the organ of self-esteem, precisely as the fall of a single apple revealed the law of gravita- tion to Newton. Anatomists had seen many human brains without remarking any differ- ences among them ; these, however, are, to say the least, as constant as similarities. The point that essentially interests science is, the PREFACE. XIX discovery of the truth, and this is then con- firmed and established by all ulterior obser- vations. The anatomy of the peculiar system ne- cessary to the affective and intellectual ma- nifestations, as well as anatomy in general, admits of consideration in several ways. First, it is simply descriptive, that is, phy- sical appearances alone are examined, such as the form, the size, and colour of parts, the tissues which compose them, and their connexions. The nomenclature of the encephalon, of itself suffices to shew that such views had principally guided anatomists in their study of its structure. We still speak of the' brain, of the cerebellum, of hemispheres, lobes, convolutions, and anfractuosities ; of a for- nix, an infundibulum or funnel, and of pi- siform and striated, and quadrigeminal and pyramidal, and olivary and harrow-shaped bodies ; of a pineal gland, of a hippocampus's foot, of a writing-pen, and many other parts, some with very offensive names. Such views are easily conceived to be but little useful in medicine. This is the reason why the gene- XX PREFACE. rality of practitioners are satisfied with know- ing the membraneous envelopes of the ence- phalon, the large blood-vessels, the sinuses, the great masses of the brain and cerebellum, and the principal cavities. Of these views, however, I shall only take such notice as may be necessary to recognize the parts spoken of in my physiological and pathologi- cal considerations. M. Serres, in the first page of his work on the Anatomy of the Brain, says, “Up to the present time, (1824,) no one has dreamt o uniting into a body of doctrine all the know- ledge acquired on the anatomy, the physiology and the pathology of the brain and nervous system. I enter on the attempt to overtake this vast subject Dr. Gall’s and my own works belie this assertion, and they have only to be consulted to prove that all our inquiries were directed into this very channel. We have constantly insisted on the importance of studying the nervous system under all re- lations at once. From the year 1817 to 1823, * “ Jusqu’a ce jour (1824) personne n’a songe a reunir en corps de doctrine les connaissances acquises sur l’anatomie, la physiologie et la pathologie du systcme nerveux, Je vais essayer de parcourir ce vaste sujet.” *-+5 preface:. xxi I regularly delivered “ a Course of Lectures on the Anatomy, Physiology, and Pathology of the Brain and External Senses ,” twice a year. My course was always so announced, according to the custom in Paris, by public placards ; and my auditors must recollect that in my introductory discourse, I uniformly insisted on the importance and necessity of studying these branches in connexion. From the above, it will be evident that M. Serres was mistaken when he published himself “ the first to at- tempt to overtake (essay er de par courir) this vast subject/’ Nevertheless, I most willingly allow that the principal consideration is not the having been the first to examine the ner- vous system : the true merit of the inquirer consists in that which he has effected, that which he had discovered, and justice in these particulars will, in time, be assuredly ren- dered to all. If, on occasion, I seem more especially solicitous in shewing the errone- ousness of M. Serres’s opinions, it is only because these have received the sanction of the French Institute, whose influence is great over the public mind. 2. Anatomy is physiological, when the XXII PREFACE. structure of parts is studied in relation to their functions. This kind of anatomical know- ledge is essentia] to practical medicine ; for without it, the seat of deranged functions cannot be understood. For this reason, there- fore, my anatomical details will always be given in harmony with the physiological ideas I entertain of the apparatus destined to the manifestation of the affective and intellectual faculties. 3. Anatomy is peculiarly human, or, it comprehends the other beings of creation. In the latter event, it is entitled comparative anatomy, and this is a field that possesses much interest for the anatomist, physiologist, and practical physician ; I shall, therefore, enter upon it at frequent intervals, always with the view of advancing the knowledge on the affective and intellectual nature of man. 4. Anatomy is entitled pathological , when it treats of the organic changes undergone by parts whether examined in connexion with, or independently of, their deranged functions. Inquiries in this direction belong less to the present volume, than to that I have published PREFACE. XX111 on Insanity. To it, therefore, I refer the reader for details. The object of this compendium, then, is to present the principal views that may be taken of the physiological and comparative anatomy of the apparatus destined to the affective and intellectual manifestations. It will be found divided into nine sections: — in the first, I make some general reflections on the nervous system ; in the second, I speak of the division of the nervous apparatuses ; in the third, I treat of the nerves of voluntary motion and of the external senses ; in the fourth, I discuss the best mode of examining the structure of the brain ; in the fifth, I describe the cere- bellum particularly ; in the sixth, I do the like in regard to the brain ; in the seventh, I examine the commissures ; in the eighth, the communication of the nervous parts with each other ; and in the ninth, I go into some anatomical points connected with physiology. The work will prove that I rather adhere to philosophical views and principles, than to mere description of the physical qualities belonging to individual masses, although this last be the most common, I might almost say XXIV PREFACE. the only, plan that is generally pursued. I have endeavoured, in an especial manner, to class together the parts that constitute parti- cular apparatuses, a practice which to our predecessors was entirely unknown, as is abundantly evident from their nomenclature of the brain and its parts. ANATOMY OF THE BRAIN, fr- Section I. General Considerations. Nerves are whitish cords that pervade the bodies of the more perfect animals ; they are always made up of many filaments, each of which, however minute, is a tube that holds in its interior a peculiar pulpy substance, consti- tuting one among the elements of organization. It is to a multitude of such tubular filaments, enveloped in a common sheath, that the term nervous cord, or nerve, is usually applied. These cords, as well as their component filaments, vary much in thickness and consistency. The last quality depends entirely on the texture of the enveloping membrane. The nervous system comprises two distinct substances : the one gelatinous or pulpy, and usually of a grayish or brownish hue ; the other fibrous, and of a more or less perfect white B 2 colour. They are commonly spoken of in books as the cineritious and medullary substances. Of the Existence of the Pulpy and Fibrous Substances. Comparative anatomists are not agreed upon the constantly-conjoined existence of the two ner- vous substances. Some admit the presence of the white without that of the gray, especially in the spinal mass of many inferior animals, as reptiles and fishes. The nervous ganglions of the asterice are also said to contain no gray matter. The above observation is evidently made in consequence of more attention being given to the colour than to the essential nature of the pulpy substance. Pulpiness, not colour, is its distin- guishing character. There are animals whose humours, or, as was formerly said, whose blood is white. The essential consideration is evi- dently, in this case, the existence, not the colour, of the nutritious fluid. Should it not also be so in regard to the first or pulpy nervous substance, which in the majority of animals is of an ash- gray or brown colour ? Undoubtedly it should, for it is well known to vary in complexion, not only in different species of animals, but even in individuals of the same kind, according to their state of bodily health. The pulpy substance is 3 commonly extremely pale in the brains of those who die of dropsy or pulmonary consumption. The pulpy substance is found in the ganglions, and in the nervous masses of the head and spine of vertebral animals. In the mammiferous classes especially, it occurs on the surface of all the convolutions of the brain and cerebellum, (a circumstance from which it derives its title, cor - tical,) in the masses called striated bodies, and optic thalami, in the interior of the crura of the brain, of the annular pro" £U >erance, of the dentated body, of the cerebellum, of the medulla oblongata, of the spinal cord through its entire length, and of all the ganglions of the body. It never of itself composes an isolated unit or whole, it is always in connexion with the white or fibrous sub- stance. Occasionally it lies in masses of varying magnitude, and again it occurs in layers, or it runs along in slips following the nerves in their course. Of the Structure of the two Nervous Substances. The first nervous substance is pulpy or gelati- nous, and of a colour varying from deep brown to pale ash-gray or white. Its intimate structure is unknown. Ruysh, Vieussens, and almost all the cotemporaries of Haller, regarded it as a tissue of very fine blood vessels. Ackermann of Heidelberg, and Walter of Berlin, in our own B 2 times, have held it to consist of an extremely-at- tenuated prolongation of vessels, in the course of becoming still more minute, ultimately to com- pose the white or fibrous substance. This is a very ancient idea ; it may be traced as far back as the age of Praxagoras, who fancied that the nerves originated where the arteries ended. Al- binus, and, at a later period, Scemmerring, have proved by their injections, that besides very mi- nute blood-vessels, there also exists a peculiar substance in the emeritus nervous mass. The first or pulpy substance, therefore, can only be said to have an immense quantity of blood-ves- sels distributed through it. Vicq d’Azyr believed that he could trace fibres in the pulpy substance ; but what he saw was the white or truly fibrous substance, inter- mingled or uniting with the gray. The second, or white, nervous substance, is essentially fibrous, but it varies much in its de- grees of consistency. Anatomists have differed extremely in their notions of its intimate struc- ture. Some have maintained it to be solid, others have said that it was tubular. Some have found it, like the pulpy substance, composed of globules ; and whilst some have held it possessed of no blood-vessels, others have argued for its entire composition of these. Lewenhoeck, Yieussens, and Steno, believed that the white nervous substance was fibrous. 5 This is the opinion which Dr. Gall and I have espoused. By scraping it in the brain according to the direction of its fibres, these may be seen with the naked eye, and if the scraping mo- tion be oblique or transverse to their course, they will be seen to be drawn from their na- tural direction, or to be torn. If the brain be boiled in oil, or macerated in diluted nitric or muriatic acid, or in vinegar, or alcohol, or if it be frozen, the fibrous structure of its white substance will be rendered extremely apparent. Some, however, say, that the fibrousness is then the consequence of a chemical change. As the same result, however, is constantly obtained, and as the fibres, whichever of the processes be employed, are regularly disposed in correspond- ing situations in a similar manner, the fibrous structure must of necessity be recognised as na- tural and inherent. A few authors have attributed the fibrous ap- pearance of the white substance to the impres- sions of blood-vessels. This mistake may be de- tected at once by comparing the course of the blood-vessels with that of the cerebral fibres. The reality of the fibrousness of the white substance is further opposed, by saying that when the brain is cut, it does not appear, and that it is produced by the force employed to tear the tougher cerebral masses asunder. To this I reply, that it is impossible, by means 6 of a clean and smooth cut, to discover the struc- ture of any extremely delicate and soft part whatsoever. Such a method is not even available in those cerebral parts that are incontestably fibrous, as the pyramidal bodies, the annular pro- tuberance, the peduncles of the brain, &c. M. Bogros, of Paris, read a paper to the Aca- demy of Sciences, on the 25th of May, 1825, in which he maintained that every nervous fibre is perforated by a canal from its origin to its ex- treme termination. The accuracy of this state- ment is far from being ascertained. The white nervous substance is generally called medullary. This name, however, ought to be discontinued for two reasons : in the first i place the idea we form of marrow excludes the conception of fibrousness ; and again, the func- tions of the nervous fibres are so superior to those of the marrow, that it is a pity to designate both by the same word. The name is evidence of the error that was anciently committed, when every thing contained in an osseous cavity was consi- dered as marrow. Of the Use of the Pulpy Nervous Substance. Different opinions have prevailed ever since the times of Vesalius and Piccoluomini, who di- rected the attention of anatomists in a particular manner to this subject, respecting the use and 7 destination of the pulpy or gray substance. It has frequently been regarded as an organ of se- cretion, whether of vital spirits or of a nervous fluid. Unity of sentiment, it is probable, will not be readily obtained upon this particular point: but seeing that the rudiments of each new shoot in trees are developed in a deposition of mucilaginous-looking matter ; that the carti- lages in animal bodies are successively liquid, gelatinous, and cartilaginous, and that several turn into firm bone ; that anatomists, in many in- stances, have agreed in deriving the nerves from ganglions ; that the brain, too, is at first fluid, then gelatinous, and ultimately fibrous ; in fine, that the pulpy nervous substance is always found where the white fibres become more numerous ; that these are implanted, so to say, into it, and that a great quantity of blood-vessels are ex- panded on it ; Dr. Gall and I have said, that to us it appears to be the source or nourisher of the white fibres. Let us, however, distinguish in this, as in every other place, between facts and inferences. Supposing that our ideas con- cerning the inference were really inexact, the peculiarities I have mentioned, and the essen- tial importance of the pulpy substance to the nervous functions, must ever be admitted. Having said that a gray colour is not an essen- tial in the character of the pulpy substance, no ob- jection to our notions of its uses can be derived s from the fact of its supposed absence in the gan- glions of the asterice. A gelatinous or pulpy matter does certainly enter into their constitution, and this is sufficient. The white substance is also said to be present in the brain and spinal cord before the pulpy or gray appears. If by this the existence of the nervous masses that become white prior to an evident separation into two substances, and to the developement of convolutions, be understood, I agree ; but if the rudiments of the brain, ce- rebellum, and spinal cord, be said to be white, and not gray in the first instance, I positively deny the assertion. The nervous masses of the head and spine are pulpy or gelatinous, and decidedly grayish in colour before they are white. Neither Dr. Gall nor I have ever thought of saying, that the portion of the completely-de- veloped nervous system, which is pulpy and gray, gives birth to that which is white and fibrous. We did but intend to announce the fact of a ge- latinous and grayish state of the brain preceding its fibrous and white condition, precisely as we should say of its entire mass, that it is liquid before it becomes gelatinous. Our idea of the formation of the nervous system seems more especially plausible, when it appears to be perfectly analogous to what takes place in the osseous. Bone begins by being gelatinous, it is then cartilaginous, and ultimately solid and 9 earthy. I repeat, however, that no one can he more impressed than myself with the difficulty of drawing general conclusions ; I am, there- fore, very far from anxious to impose such as I do infer upon others. I only insist on the necessity of exactness in regard to the facts related. Putting our theory of the formation out of the question entirely, then, Dr. Gall and I still assert our title to be considered as the first who discovered and made known the general relation that pre- vails in man and the mammalia, between the pulpy and fibrous substances of the brain and its several parts. Messrs. Foville and Pinel Grandchamp have of late inferred, from pathological observations, that the superficial cineritious substance of the brain presides over intellectual functions, and its white and deep-seated gray mass over loco- motion. I am disposed to set much store by patholo- gical observations, yet I do not see that we dare place unlimited confidence in them alone. Truth is, I conceive, universally harmonious ; truth consequently cannot have been attained in any case until anatomy and physiology and patho- logy accord exactly. Now, who will maintain that the locomotive powers of animals are great in proportion as their brains contain more of the white nervous substance, and as the striated bo- dies and the supposed origins of the optic nerves * * 10 (thalami) are large ? Or who will say that the locomotive capacities of inferior tribes, in whose brains the gray substance predominates, are less remarkable than their intellectual endowments ? These positions are alike untenable. Dr. Gall and I suppose that each nervous apparatus is composed of the two peculiar substances, the pulpy and the fibrous, and that both are necessary to produce an instrument adequate to perform a particular function. Of the Origin of the Nervous System. The brain has very generally been regarded as the sole and common origin of every part of the nervous system. Even the old anatomists, who classed the brain along with the viscera of the chest and belly, and treated of its structure in their chapter of Splanchnology, mistook it, how- ever strange the error may seem, for the source of all the nerves. In their eyes, the spinal cord was a prolongation of the cerebral mass, and the great sympathetic, and the nerves of the abdo- men and thorax, were continuations of the ence- phalon and spinal cord. This erroneous view, as it was espoused, was especially defended, by observing that the commands of the will issued from, and that all consciousness resided in, the brain. These facts, however, do, in truth, prove no more than the communication of the nervous 11 masses of the body with those of the head. The muscular fibres, we see, are excited by the nerves, but they are not, therefore, continua- tions of the nervous filaments. The notion, however, was so palpably erro- neous, that anatomists were not long in calling its soundness in question, more especially in re- gard to the nerves of vegetative life. Winslow *, for instance, separated the great sympathetic from the spinal cord and brain; he even regarded the bundles that run between its ganglions or masses, as simple branches of communication ; and went so far as to say, that all the ganglions ought to be considered as peculiar origins of nerves, and, consequently, as so many little brains. Scemmerring likewise observes, that the sympa- thetic, having an independent existence, may rather be said to go to, than to come from, the spinal cord. He adds, that it never forms a trunk in any wise proportionate to the number and size of its communicating branches, and that it never loses itself among muscles, but follows the course of the blood-vessels f . Bichat has expressed his opinion in the most positive manner upon this subject. “ The ideas of anatomists,” says he “ upon this important perve, seem to me very little accordant with what * Anatomy. f Hirn und Neryen. t Sur la Vie et laMort. 12 nature proclaims to be just. All agree in repre- senting it as a medullary cord, extending from the head to the os sacrum, sending various branches, in its course, to the neck, the chest, and the ab- domen; having, in short, a distribution analogous to the spinal nerve, from which, or from those of the neck, it is said by some to derive its origin. Whatever the name chosen to designate it may chance to be, sympathetic, intercostal, tri- splanchnic, the mode of considering it will still be found to remain unchanged. “ This mode I regard as altogether erroneous. In fact there exists no such nerve as these names are used to signify. That which is taken for a nerve is, in truth, but a suite of communications between different nervous centres, situated at va- rious distances from each other. “ These centres are the ganglions, scattered through the different regions of the body. They have all an independent and isolated action. Each is a particular magazine, sending a multi- tude of ramifications, to carry into the respective organs the irradiations of the centre whence they proceed. “ What anatomist,” he continues, “ has not been struck by differences among the nerves ? Those of the brain are larger, whiter, more dense, less numerous, and offering few varieties ; whilst extreme tenuity, great number, especially around the plexuses, grayish colour, peculiar softness 13 of tissue, frequently occurring varieties, are, on the other hand, the distinguishing characters of the nerves that issue from the ganglions. The only exceptions in either case are in the branches of communication between the cere- bral nerves, and in a few of the twigs that unite the little nervous centres.” Bichat thought it essential to realize these views in the descrip- tive anatomy. That commonly given is not cal- culated to convey an exact notion, either of the nervous centres, or of the nerves that emanate from them. Comparative anatomy, and acephalic monstrosi- ties among the mammalia and man, furnish in- controvertible proofs of the brain not being the origin of the nervous system at large. To com- pare the nervous systems of different tribes is a task attended, with much difficulty ; but very many of the inferior animals have nerves, al- though they have nothing that may be likened to a brain. Their nervous system, consequently, cannot have had the origin commonly assigned to it by authors. There are also many anatomical descriptions, to be found in books, of acephalic monsters, of the more perfect animals, and of the human kind, whose nervous system, notwithstanding the ab- sence of brain, was quite perfect. That nerves may exist without a brain is therefore established as a truth beyond the sphere of doubt. 14 Some writers, however, relying on the autho- rity of Morgagni, Haller, and Sandifort, have maintained that the brains of acephali exist in the first instance, but that dropsy of the cavities destroys the parts, which, at birth, are found wanting, along with the membranes and bony covering. But no one ever saw an acephalic child whose brain and skull exhibited traces of such destruc- tion. The integuments of the upper part of the head, where the destructive process is princi- pally supposed, are commonl/'entire and healthy. Neither has any one in these cases discovered the cicatrices of ulcers, nor traces of erosion, nor of simple absorption. The bones that com- pose the basis of the skull, when they exist in acephali, are smooth and round at the edges. The whole inferior part of the encephalic mass, too, and the optic, auditory, olfactory, and other nerves, occasionally occur in a perfectly sound and natural state. How did these nerves and tender cerebral parts resist the action of a fluid that dissolved or caused the absorption of mem- branes and firm bone? It must be allowed then, that the brain may be primitively wanting, just as may the legs or arms, and that the nervous ap- paratus of the body does not derive from the brain. The first anatomical principle in regard to the nervous system therefore is, that it is not an unit , 15 but consists of many essentially different parts, which have their own individual origins, and are mutually in communication. This principle Dr. Gall and I regard as essen- tial to our physiological researches and deduc- tions. No anatomist before us was ever so much interested in demonstrating its truth. We con- ceive that we have proved it satisfactorily in re- gard to the brain, as Bichat had done before us, to the nerves of the chest and belly. In addition to our first anatomical principle, there are three subjects that require to be taken into consideration separately ; these are, — 1st, The mode in which the individual parts of the nervous system are formed ; 2d, the order of developement of these parts ; and, 3d, their reci- procal relations. Dr. Gall and I, in our publications and anato- mical demonstrations, have always spoken of the mode and order in which the cerebral parts are formed and developed ; however, we never touched upon these points but in a general way, our attention being more especially given to the consideration of the plurality of the nervous ap- paratuses, of their communications, and of their mutual relations, the whole in harmony with our physiological and philosophical inquiries. Mr. Tiedemann, and, after him, Mr. Serres, have treated in a particular manner of the formation and successional developement of the several parts composing the nervous system. Of the Mode of Formation of the Nervous System. I have no intention of entering into any dis- cussion upon Encephalogejiy. My researches have not been of extent sufficient to qualify me either to admit or to reject the opinions promulgated by M. Serres upon this subject. According to this gentleman, the spinal cord, the cerebellum, and the brain, are developed from the circum- ference towards the centre, and not, as ana- tomists had hitherto admitted, from the centre towards the circumference. He tells us, that there are many centres of formation, and that each apparatus is composed by several pieces, which are joined together, the extremities com- mencing, the middles terminating, the union. According to him, too *, the arteries exert a par- ticular directing influence over the develope- ment of the nervous system. The spinal cord, he conceives to be formed under the guidance of the intercostal, the cerebellum under that of the vertebral, and the brain under that of the carotid arteries. The vessels of the spinal cord, he says, ap- pear first, and the outline of this part is soonest * Tom i. p. 568, et seq. 17 apparent. The common carotids appear next, and then the internal vessels of that name, which are distributed to the crura or legs of the brain, and to the bodies called quadrigeminal, and these are now evolved. The vertebral arteries reach the cranium last, and the cerebellum is the latest formed of all the nervous masses. “ The brain,” he states, “ is developed from before backwards, and the cerebellum from be- hind forwards, according to the direction of the blood-vessels. From this it comes that the cal- lous body is evolved from behind forwards, as the arteria callosa gradually appears.” I intend in another place to cite M. Serres’s notions upon the formation of some particular apparatuses. Here I only observe that he dif- fers from M. Tiedemann upon a very principal point ; whilst M. Serres proclaims a successive development from the circumference towards the centre, M. Tiedemann publishes the reverse of this as the truth. I also confess that a great number of M. Serres’s opinions appear to me little probable. Hr. Baron* lately shewed the body of an hydrocephalic child to the Academy of Medicine in Paris, and proved that though the carotid arteries existed, the anterior cerebral lobes were wanting. M. Serres had made too hastily a contrary report upon this very case to * Bulletin des Sciences Mcdicales, Juin, 1825, p. 175. c 18 the Philomathic Society the day before Dr. Baron made the dissection. In recognising the whole importance of ence- phalogeny, or the doctrine of cerebral formation, it still appears to me that its laws cannot be at variance with those that may be demonstrated in the brain when it has arrived at maturity of growth. I farther opine, that conclusions drawn from the structure of the adult brain, are to be preferred to such as are founded on it in its em- bryotic state. I shall, in more places than one, have occasion to refer to this argument, and es- pecially when I come to treat of the anterior commissure and callous body, to the end that I may rectify the erroneous opinions promulgated by M. Tiedemann. My great and sole object is to know the structure of the brain, such as it may be demonstrated in harmony with physiology and pathology. Of the general Form of the Nervous System. The form of the nervous system, it may be conceived, varies according to the radiated, glo- bular, or elongated configuration of the ani- mal body, of which it is a constituent. It can- not be alike in the star-fish, worm, and cater- pillar. (PI. i. fig. 1.) Generally then, and even as its particular parts are concerned, the ner- 19 vous system exhibits the greatest varieties of form. Its masses* too, occur aggregated, as in the skull, and spinal canal, or in the form of nerves properly so called, or collected into knots differ- ing in form, size, consistency, and colour, and en- titled ganglions. Nerves frequently emanate from distant and opposite sources to unite, anastomose, or twine together, and then to separate and run off in different directions. This is the particu- lar arrangement to which the term plexus is ap- plied. Ganglions and plexuses often form an in- tricate tissue together. Of the Structure and Use of Ganglions. Ganglions are bodies composed of the two nervous substances : the white or fibrous, and the gelatinous or pulpy, into which the first is plunged. The pulpy matter of the ganglions is commonly of a gray colour, of different degrees of intensity ; sometimes, however, it has a yellowish, a reddish, or a whitish cast ; it is easily distinguishable from the nervous filaments which it surrounds. The fibres of the white substance, the second element of the ganglions, anastomose repeatedly ; they also cross each other frequently, sometimes in every direction, or in their course parallel with that of the nerve upon which the ganglion is formed. Ganglions C 2 20 of this last kind are commonly oval-shaped, but those in which several nerves meet and inoscu- culate, have for the most part very irregular forms. Anatomists have always entertained very dif- ferent opinions upon the uses of the ganglions. Some, with Willis, have ascribed to them the secretion of what they style vital spirits ; others, with Yieussens, conceive them destined to se- parate and to strengthen the nervous fibres ; many of the moderns again, Bichat, Reil, and others, think with Johnstone, that they serve to isolate from the influence of the brain the parts which receive their nerves from them. The latter authors divide the nervous systems into two parts • a cerebral and a ganglionary. The theory of vital spirits is now forgotten. The ganglions are also proved to do more than merely separate or direct the nervous filaments, as Meckel the elder, Zinn, Scarpa, and others, have supposed. “ A glance,” says Bichat*, “ en- ables us to discover the greatest differences among them, (the ganglions.) There is evi- dently as great a distinction between the gan- glions and the nerves that issue from them, as there is between the cerebral nerves and the brain itself. There is difference of consistency and other outward qualities, and there is difference of properties. Were the nerves that come from the * Anatomie Generate, T. 1. 21 spinal cord merely unravelled in their passage through the ganglions, this would be but a differ- ence of form, not of nature, and their properties, therefore, would remain the same. Why has not nature placed ganglions on the nerves of the limbs as on those of internal parts? If there be only a resolution of nerves into finer filaments irr ganglions, why is there no constant proportion between the fibres that enter on one side, and those that issue from the other? Did the nerve that penetrates the superior cervical ganglion from above expand in its interior, and having re- united its filaments form the cord that issues from below, it ought evidently to be of precisely the same size at its exit as at its entrance. Such a re- lation, indeed, between the nerves of the oppo- site extremities of ganglions, ought to be quite general. But a very cursory examination shews that a contrary disposition almost invariably ob- tains. The size of the ganglions should be re- lative to that of the nerves, whose expanded fibres are said to compose them Why then are the intercostal ganglions so small, and the trunks which unite, or rather, to use the common phrase, originate, and then leave them so large ? And why, on the contrary, is the superior cervical ganglion so voluminous, and its nervous branches so minute ? How can the frequent interruptions among the ganglions in the human kind, which in a host of animals are quite regular, be ex- plained, if the nervous filaments which enter 22 be continuous with those which issue from them ? How comes it that the ganglions and their nerves bear no exact proportion to the cerebral nerves, if the latter give them birth or expand in them ? Why, in fine, has not the pain, that is transmitted by both kinds of nerves, the same character ?” Scemmerring has made reflections very similar to these of Bichat. Notwithstanding all this, the opinion in regard to the use of the ganglions, which Johnstone, Bichat, and Reil entertained, and which others have adopted from them, is by no means exact. They do not interrupt the reciprocal influence of the brain and nerves of the spinal cord, nor of the brain and viscera of the chest and belly, either in the healthy or pathological state of the body. They most certainly do not prevent im- pressions made on parts supplied with nerves from them, or diseased sensations of the viscera from being felt. On the contrary, the ganglions would appear essential to the structure of nerves of sensation. They, however, abstract the parts they furnish with nervous energy, from the influence of the will. They also originate ner- vous fibres ; and serve, farther, as points of com- munication between different nerves. Lastly, as the existence of a nervous fluid is not impos- sible, nay, as in all likelihood such a fluid does exist, the ganglions may probably aid in its se- cretion or evolution, and modify its circulation or distribution. 23 Section II. Division of the Masses composing the Nervous System. We have already seen, among the general con- siderations, that the nervous system is not a simple unit, but an aggregation of parts, that originate and may exist separately, but which are in intimate communication, as was required on account of the influence their functions exert mutually. The functions of the abdominal vis- cera, for instance, act upon those of the brain, and those of the brain influence digestion, circu- lation, the secretion of bile, &c. The nervous system, therefore, cannot be compared to a net, as is sometimes done, if, by this, similarity of nature in its constituent parts be implied. On the contrary, as differences are evident, divisions become indispensable. But the task of determining the instruments specially dedicated to particular functions, is one of great difficulty. The ordinary division of the nervous masses is indisputably defective. A true one can only rest on the nature of the functions performed. These functions are naturally sepa- rated into two grand classes : vegetative, or orga- nic functions, and phrenic, or functions having 24 place with consciousness. The nervous masses be- longing to the first of these are, in common lan- guage, the great sympathetic nerve, and the gan- glions and plexuses of the thorax and abdomen. Bichat has divided them into a multitude of appa- ratuses, individually necessary to the offices of the particular viscera. This division is, in my appre- hension, founded in nature. I regard the nervous masses of vegetative life as independent of those of phrenic life, in as far as their existence is concerned. I also incline to admit as many dif ferent kinds of nerves as ' different vegetative functions ; I farther recognise their communica- tions among themselves, and with the masses of the phrenic functions. The nervous masses of vegetative life are very simple in the lower tribes of beings ; they are more numerous as we mount in the scale, and as the functions of vegetative life become compli- cated. Their general arrangement, as was to be expected, varies according to the form and dis- position of the viscera in species and individuals. If visceral functions, locomotion, and manifesta- tions of sensibility be united, it is also conceiv- able that to separate the peculiar nerves of these dissimilar operations from each other, w T ould be next to impossible. This consideration shews why some anatomists have compared the nerves of the caterpillar and worm to the great sympa- thetic; whilst others have likened them to the 25 intervertebral ganglions, or to the spinal cord itself. In the vertebral classes of animals, and espe- cially in the mammalia, the nervous masses are distinctly separated, first, into those of vegetative life, and, second, into those of phrenic functions. Each of these may farther be subdivided into two parts : the first into nerves of viscera, or, as M. de Blainville * expresses himself, into a vis- ceral portion, and into nerves of communication ; that is, an apparatus which establishes sympathy among the visceral nerves mutually, and be- tween these and the nervous masses of the ex- ternal senses, and of the affective and intellec- tual functions. The second, or nervous masses of phrenic life, again, are commonly divided into the brain, pro- perly so called, the cerebellum, medulla oblon- gata, and spinal cord, together with the nerves of these four parts. M. de Blainville ranges the nervous masses of phrenic life under two titles : the one he styles Central, the other Ganglionary . “ The first, or central,” says he f , “ is susceptible of three de- grees of development; is always situated above the intestinal canal, begins with the oesophagus or pharynx, and is prolonged, more or less * Bulletin des Sciences par la Societe Philomatique de Paris, 1821, MarsetAvril. t Op. cit. p. 44. 26 backwardly, so as to correspond to a greater or smaller number of rings of the body when they exist. “ The second, or ganglionary,consists of a very variable number of pairs of ganglions, disposed in a slightly different manner on each side of the first. “ The central portion is always composed of two similar halves, situated, the one to the right, the other to the left, and more or less inti- mately united, or drawn together by means of the peculiar apparatus, styled commissure. It is divided into two rigorously similar portions : a ver- tebral and a cephalic, included, as their titles im- ply, the one in the vertebral canal, the other in the cranium. Both contain gray and white sub- stance, and longitudinal and transverse commis- sures. “ The cephalic portion consists, 1st, of an in- ferior bundle called pyramidal, which passes un- der the annular protuberance, continues long dis- tinct from the cerebral peduncles, and runs to terminate in the anterior, or olfactory lobe, of the brain. 2d. Of a superior bundle, which con- tinues, in the first place, under the name of pro- longation of the quadrigeminal bodies, to the cerebellum, and running on the outer sides of the internal geniculated bodies, is lost on the hemispheres ; and, in the second, of a deep bundle, which may be followed to the mammil- 27 lary bodies, and from thence into the optic tha- lami. The bundles named peduncles of the pineal gland, which are expended on the optic thalamus, are also to be regarded as belonging to this cephalic portion.” The same author subdivides his second or ganglionary portion into ganglions without, and ganglions with, external apparatus. Among the former he reckons the olfactory masses, the hemi- spheres properly so called, the quadrigeminal bodies, and the cerebellum. The olfactory nerve, according to him, is a cerebral mass, in which his inferior central bundle terminates. The he- mispheres, he conceives, form another ganglion, having no external apparatus, but one transverse, (the callous body,) and two longitudinal commis- sures (the peduncles of the brain and the fornix). It is to this ganglion that the convolutions be- long. He farther regards the striated bodies as ap- pertaining to the convolutions, but remarks that, for certain, their fibres do not originate there to go to the hemispheres. The quadrigeminal bodies are the third, and the cerebellum the fourth, ganglion, without external apparatus. The title, visceral part of the nervous system, ap- pears to me well chosen, but I do not think that the division into central and ganglionary parts, or the other subdivisions proposed, deserve the same approval. In the first place, I cannot see that they are 28 based on anatomical observation ; I do not be- lieve, for instance, that M. de Blainville can de- monstrate the continuation of the inferior bundle, or pyramidal body, into the olfactory nerve, nor that he can trace what he calls the deep bundle ( faisceau profond ) of his central portion into the mammillary bodies. Neither do I think that his divisions and subdivisions of the nervous mass pertaining to phrenic life, are consistent with physiological facts; but this in anatomical classification is quite essential : it is even acknow- ledged by M. de Blainville himself as its basis. “ We ought,” says he *, “ to consider the ner- vous system as subdivided into as many parts as there are grand functions performed by the ani- mal body.” According to the ideas of M. Desmoulins, the parts which constitute the complete cerebro-spinal system of mammalia, are — 1st, the spinal cord ; 2d, the cerebellum, which is itself composed of three parts ; 3d, the optic lobes, or quadrigemi- nal bodies ; 4th, the lobes of the brain ; 5th, the olfactory lobes. These five parts are admitted by M. Desmoulins not to be always complete in the other classes of vertebral animals, and also to be wanting, individually, without detriment to the rest. The sturgeon, lamprey, frog, and some other creatures, have, he says, no cerebellum ; the skate and shark no cerebral lobes ; many * Op. cit. p. 40. 29 bony fishes no olfactory lobes ; in short, he concludes, that generally speaking, the notion concerning the unity of the nervous system is a chimera. In opposition to M. Demoulins’s views, Dr. Bailly has maintained that every vertebra, or every segment, through the whole length of the animal body, contains the same nervous elements ; that belonging to each of the vertebrae of the head and spine, there are nerves of sensation, of motion, of digestion, and of an intellectual system, charged with the task of appreciating the impressions communicated by the others, and of producing determinations. In my apprehension, the cerebro-spinal system is not a simple unit, but a compound of many distinct apparatuses, each of which has particular functions, which, being taken together, constitute phrenic life. The several instruments are formed after a general plan, but the physical qualities, as the density, form, size, and colour, of all, indivi- dually, differ not only in the classes and in the species, but also in the members composing each kind ; the number of particular organs is likewise greater or smaller in the classes and species, ac- cording to the amount of the primary functions, or faculties; lastly, each of the apparatuses is simple, or it is compound. No one, however, can be said to be perfectly simple ; each is, at least, double, or one of a pair. 30 The organs of motion and of touch are evi- dently multiplied. It is essential, in dividing and subdividing the parts of which the cerebro-spinal system is com- posed, to keep in harmony with physiology and pathology, and to distinguish between what is common to all, and what is peculiar to each of them. The first grand division must embrace the instruments of motion, and of the five external senses; the second comprise the organs of the affective and intellectual functions. I think they do wrong who confound the spi- nal cord with the cerebral masses, and designate both by a common title, such as encephalon or brain. It is long since Dr. Gall and I pointed out this error, but it is still very generally sanctioned, by the French Academy of Sciences, for instance, so late as 1820 , its prize having been accorded to the memoir of M. Serres upon the following title. — “ Donner une description comparee du cerveau dans les quatre classes des animaux vertebres, et particulierement dans ceux des reptiles et des poissons, cherchant a determiner l'analogie des differentes parties de cet organe, ou marquant soigneusement les changes des formes et des pro- portions eprouvees, et suivant aussi profondement que possible les racines des nerfs cerebreaux/’ Here the brain and its nerves only are evidently spoken of. M. Serres, however, deemed it pro- per to consider the nervous mass of the spine as SI well as of the cranium, and he has designated these two systems by the same name, without any objection being made by the Academy to this arrangement. Let us turn to the facts that prove the brain and the spinal cord to be perfectly distinct and independent of each other. These facts are ana- tomical, physiological, and pathological. I here assume as an established point, that the functions of the spinal cord differ entirely from those of the brain. The development and demonstration of this truth belong to physiology. I have, accord- ingly, examined it particularly in my work on Phrenology. Physiological experiments and pa- thological facts tend equally to show that the brain and spinal cord are masses possessed of distinct functions that cannot be confounded. This volume being destined solely to the discus- sion of anatomical views, I shall, at present, confine myself to such evidence as anatomy affords of the mutual independence of each of the nervous masses mentioned. And first — all that has been said, in a general way, upon the origin of the nerves, and their existence, independently of the brain, applies particularly in the case of the spinal cord. The spinal cord sometimes exists in part, sometimes entirely, when the brain is altogether wanting. If, to this, it be objected that the brain had been removed by absorption, the answer I have given 82 to the same proposition, in reference to the nerves in general, must be repeated here. There is, also, one, and but one, case on record, in which the brain existed, without the spinal cord*. It occurred in a child whose head or heads are pre- served in the Hunterian Museum, in London ; this child was born with one skull placed vertically upon another. Each of these contained a brain invested with its usual membranes ; the dura mater of each brain adhered closely to that of the other, and both were supplied by blood- vessels issuing from common trunks. Parts, con- sequently, as the brain and spinal cord that exist, or not, independently of each other, cannot con- stitute one and the same apparatus. 2. The nervous masses of the spinal canal and cranium bear no regular proportion to each other. Man, with liis voluminous brain, has a smaller spinal cord than the ox or horse, whose brain is so much less. Bartholin in former times, and Scemmerring in our own days, made this remark ; nevertheless, they both continued to speak of the spinal cord as a process or continuation of the brain. 3. The spinal cord is well known not to de- crease in size as it descends in the vertebral canal, and as it sends off nerves. Its volume is even augmented, where its nerves are largest and * See Philosoph. Trans, for 1790, March 25th, and for 179S, Dec. 13 th. S3 most numerous; this is obvious, especially to- wards its sacral end, as may be seen by turning to PI. i. fig. 2, where this part of the spinal cord of a fowl is represented; and yet the spinal cord is commonly considered as a prolongation of the white substance of the brain and cerebellum ! 4*. The direction of its nerves, especially in the mammalia, proves to a certainty, that the spinal cord is not a continuation of the nervous mass of the cranium. Every pair of spinal nerves is made up by several bundles: (PI. i. fig. 3; a part of the spinal cord of a calf ;) some of these issue from below, and run upwardly; others come from above, and proceed down and outwardly. Now it would be absurd to suppose that these bundles were continued, or derived either from the lower or upper extremity of the spinal cord. They un- doubtedly originate at the place whence they issue individually. In regard to the fifth, sixth, seventh, and other pairs of nerves, styled cerebral (PI. vi. fig. 1), their direction also shows that they do not come either from the brain or cerebellum. Santorini, when speaking of the course of the fifth pair, remarks, that after descending from the brain, it turns back and runs upwards ; and he adds — “ if it do not probably come from below, entirely like the accessory nerve.” The proofs, confirmatory of the mutual inde- pendence of the spinal cord aud hemispheres of D 34 the brain and cerebellum, serve also to demon- strate the propriety of separating the nerves, styled cerebral, from the brain itself. The evi- dence in this, as in the other case, is anatomical, physiological, and pathological. For the reasons already given, I here rest on anatomical testimo- nies alone ; for information on the others, I refer to my physiological and pathological treatises*. To proceed, then, we see monsters, occasionally, born without any of the proper cerebral masses, but with olfactory, optic, and acoustic nerves, either severally or altogether perfect ; and on the contrary, these nerves, individually, or generally, have been found in a state of atrophy, whilst the brain was sound and well developed in all its parts. There is no proportion, whatever, between the cranial nerves and the true cerebral mass. Many animals have them much larger in propor- tion to their brain than man. I repeat, therefore, that I divide and subdivide the nervous masses according to their offices: first, into nerves of vegetative functions ; secondly, of external sense, and locomotion ; lastly, of affec- tive and intellectual operations : these last inhere in parts, which I style truly cerebral, super- added to the nerves of the senses. We shall, afterwards, see that the masses be- longing to the last-named class of functions require * Phrenology ; or, the Doctrine of the Mind; Lond. 1S25. Observations on Insanity ; Lond, 1816. 35 subdividing into instruments of particular func- tions, after the manner of the external senses. This principle of the plurality of the nervous apparatuses is the basis of these anatomical con- siderations, and is indispensable to the physiolo- gical researches in which Dr. Gall and I have so long been engaged. It has been, and is still, contested, and at the same time it is brought for- ward as new. A reference, however, to the date and matter of our publications, will assign it to those who can claim it by right. The general idea of a plurality of organs, indeed, must be allowed to be very ancient, and not the discovery of any modern author ; before Dr. Gall appeared, however, none of the cerebral functions had been specified, and before our combined investigations were made public, the structure of no special apparatus had been demonstrated ; for the parts of the encephalon that bear distinguishing titles, do not, by any means, constitute particular organs. The special determination, as well anatomical as physiological, consequently, belongs to Dr, Gall and myself. D 2 36 Section III. Of the Nervous Masses of Voluntary Motion and of the External Seiises. These masses include the spinal cord, with its nerves, and the nerves of the head, distinguished by the title, cerebral nerves. They may, therefore, be divided and spoken of as spinal or vertebral, and as cranial. The former, principally contained in the canal of the vertebral column, are more con- siderable in size, and are entirely destined to the functions of feeling and voluntary motion ; the latter are less voluminous, but besides nerves of voluntary motion and of feeling, they include nerves of taste, hearing, sight, and smell. Anatomists have, at all times, separated the nerves of four of the senses, to wit — smell, taste, sight, and hearing, from those appropriated to feeling; they have also regarded the third, fourth, and sixth pairs of nerves as especially destined to motion ; but they have confounded all the other trunks of the medulla oblongata and spinal cord, and supposed the whole to pertain to feeling. It is long since I maintained the necessity of subdividing these nerves, and of admitting se- parate fibres for the functions of touch and 37 of motion. My opinion was founded on facts, and reasonings anatomical, physiological, and pathological. It was long ago remarked that feel- ing and voluntary motion were not always im- paired or annihilated simultaneously ; sometimes the one, sometimes the other of these functions was seen to be totally lost, whilst the other remained unimpaired. The conclusion then fol- lowed, and it had already been seized by Erasi- stratus of Alexandria, that there were nerves of motion, and nerves of feeling. Pathological facts, therefore, first fixed the attention of physicians on this point, as they also gave the first idea of a decussation of nervous fibres in the brain, and of the peculiar structure of the convolutions. To the above, some have replied that the ad- mission of two sorts of nerves to explain the isolated lesion, whether of voluntary motion or of feeling, was unnecessary ; the loss of motion, they say, is consequent on a minor degree of cerebral affection, the destruction of feeling on disease of a graver and more extensive description. This objection is purely speculative, and is to- tally unsupported by experience. To make it even plausible, it should surely be capable of stand- ing the test of physiological proofs in the healthy state, that is to say, sensibility and volition should be shown to be mere degrees of the same power, volition being the lower of the two ; but obser- vation is very far from confirming such an opinion. 38 Physiology, and analogy, in reference to the other senses, appear to determine feeling as a function entirely distinct from voluntary motion, and to prove each of these functions as manifested by the medium of a special organ or nerve. The functions of touch are active and passive, like those of the other external senses. The muscles are called into action, would the internal faculties employ any of the senses to cognize impressions from without. They aid touch in feeling, as they assist sight in looking, hearing in listening, and the olfactory nerves in smelling. The nerves of voluntary motion, i. e., of its organs, the muscles, cannot propagate impressions of touch, nor the nerves of the skin those of motion. The impres- sions propagated by the nerves of motion come from within, as those transmitted by the nerves of feeling are derived from without. The muscles, alone, produce sensations of fatigue or weariness, and these have no relation to the nerves of feeling • O we may be excessively fatigued, and at the same time, have the sense of touch extremely vigorous. Physiological and pathological facts being strik- ingly and universally favourable to the idea of distinct nerves of feeling and motion, and four of the senses being evidently provided with peculiar nervous apparatuses, I have always maintained the probability of the same law obtaining, in regard to the fifth, and even invited anatomists to use their endeavours to verify the point, and to de- 39 monstrate the muscular as distinct from the cuti- cular nerves. To the above notion, it has been objected, that the nerves of feeling and of motion issue together from the spinal cord, and, as a consequence of this, that the nature of every fibre, of the same pair, must be essentially the same. To this I always answered that the objection is neither valid on the strength of the fact, nor on that of the inference from it. For the spinal pairs are composed of two roots, the one dorsal, the other abdominal ; and further, each root is, itself, com- posed of many bundles. I also stated that this structure was less conspicuous in the fifth cranial nerve than in the spinal pairs ; but that, never- theless, the difference of its bundles, and their dissimilar functions, were recognised. One, all allow, is appropriated to taste, whilst the rest belong to sensation in general. In conformity with the above view, the glossopharyngeal has always appeared to me destined to the third sort of the lingual functions, that is, to general feeling. The above ideas were published in my English work, “The Physiognomical System in 1815; and in 1818, in my French book, entitled, “ Phrenologie j\” In all the courses of lectures I have delivered since these works issued from the press, I have broached similar ideas, and en- couraged those of my auditors, whom opportunity * First and Second Edition, p. 23. t Op. cit. p. 236. 40 favoured, to enter on the inquiry, and to endea- vour to trace the nervous fibres from their peri- pheral expansions to their origin in the spinal cord. The subject has, indeed, been advanced by the labours of Messrs. C. Bell and Masrenclie. o Mr. Bell has recognised a difference between the nerves of sensation in general and those of re- spiration *. He admits, in the first instance, that every apparatus operating a single function, is provided with but one nerve ; that parts which receive two nerves, whose origins are differ- ent, perform two functions ; and that organs, whose nerves are derived from several sources, effect various, and not merely stronger, func- tions of one specific kind. He regards as respi- ratory nerves the pneumogastric, with its vocal branch ; the glossopharyngeal ; the accessory of Willis ; the facial ; the hypoglossal, and the dia- phragmatic (the phrenic). Mr. Bell has further established a difference in function between the facial nerve, (portio dura of the seventh pair,) and the branches of the fifth pair. He has proved by his experiments, that the facial gives no impression of pain when it is cut, but that the effect of the operation is to abstract the parts upon which it is ramified from * On the Nerves ; giving an Account of some Experiments on their Structure and Functions. Read before the Royal Society of London, July 12, 1S20. 41 the influence of the respiratory motions. Lesions, on the contrary, of the fifth pair, produce pain, and the parts it supplies become insensible, when its branches are cut across. He still further cites cases, in which sensibility remained unim- paired, although the side of the face was com- pletely paralyzed in its movements. Mr. Bell has given his attention more particu- larly to the influence exerted by the facial nerve on respiration, and by the function of respiration on parts that receive branches from this source. The facial he styles, in particular, nerve of ex- pression. Being a nerve of motion, however, it may, independently of any additional considera- tion, be entitled a nerve of expression. That it may be influenced by respiration, no one will probably deny, but it may also unquestionably act independently of this function. Mr. Bell, indeed, has not in this instance adhered rigo- rously to his principle recognised in theory, and mentioned above, viz., that no nerve performs two functions-— the facial, on his own data, can only be a nerve of one kind of function, I chanced very lately to see a case where the voluntary motion of the right side of the face was lost, whilst its sensibility remained ; both feeling and motion on the left side were intact ; the movements of the tongue were also natural ; but if the person attempted to spit, the saliva was 42 always thrown to the right by the action of the left and healthy half of the cheeks and lips. M. Serres gives the case of a young man, who was constitutionally epileptic, and troubled with a slight ophthalmy in the right eye. The latter affection increased gradually, the cornea became opaque, the sight decayed more and more, and was, in fine, entirely destroyed ; the other organs of sense and motion on the same side were suc- cessively paralyzed ; the eye, eye-lid, nostril, and half of the tongue, lost their powers, whilst on the left they remained in a state of perfect integrity. This patient died. On dissection, there was found, first, an organic change of the ganglion of the fifth pair; it Avas enlarged, of a yellow colour, and vascular beloAV ; second, a conversion of the nerve itself, at its insertion into the annular protuberance, into a yellow gelati- nous-looking matter, similar to the ganglion, and penetrating, in little processes, the substance of the protuberances in the direction of the nervous insertions. The muscular branches of the nerve, however, Avere found healthy on the othenvise affected side ; the process of mastication, there- fore, had never been injured. The difference in the functions of the facial nerve and fifth pair, affords a new proof of the distinction betAveen nerves of feeling and nerves of motion. 43 M. Magendie * found that sensibility was de - stroyed when the dorsal roots of the spinal nerves were cut through, and that motion suf- fered when the abdominal origins were the subjects of his experiments. He also observed that the abdominal side of the spinal cord was less sensible to pricking or cutting than the dorsal ; but that the introduction of a probe along the axis of the part did not seem to have any influence either upon motion or sensation. The case of a man, who died in his sixty-sixth year at Charenton, seems to corroborate these ideas. During the last seven years of his life his organs of motion had been paralyzed, but those of sensation remained uninjured ; his in- tellectual faculties were almost annihilated, and his excretions were all involuntary. Thus re- duced, he died. On opening the body, the pyra- midal and olivary bodies were found pulpy, and of a dirty gray colour. The same change was observed along nearly the whole of the anterior surface of the spinal cord, and penetrating through almost the whole thickness of the fibrous bundles that compose it. The abdominal roots of the spinal nerves were still visible, but their consistence was much diminished. The dorsal surface of the cord, on the contrary, and its in- vesting membranes, were in a healthy condition. * Journal de Physiologic Experimentale et Pathologique, tom. iii. I am, however, rather inclined to question the accuracy of the report in reference to the brain and cerebellum, when they are stated to have been in a natural state ; for I observe that the skull was eburneous, and three times thicker than common. Such changes of the cranium are, I believe, constantly accompanied by alterations in the encephalic masses. M. Magendie also mentions a singular case, observed by M. Rullier, of a man who died at the age of forty-four. Up to his last hour this person possessed great moral energies, strong generative powers, free motion of his lower limbs, and perfect sensation in his upper extre- mities. The arms, however, were rigid, their muscles being permanently contracted, and often painful. They were rotated inwards, and pressed to the sides of the body, from which they could not be separated, but with some consider- able effort. The fore-arms were in a state of uneasy pronation, the hands flexed, and all the fingers bent. On dissecting the body after death, “ the spinal cord, examined with care, ap- peared in its natural state, from its upper end as low down as the exit of the fourth pair of cervi- cal nerves. The dorsal surface of its two lower thirds was also healthy, but between the por- tions named, and through a space corresponding to the branching of eight or nine pairs of nerves. 45 (six or seven inches in extent,) there was a very decided alteration. The spinal cord was there so extremely soft and diffluent, that the sheath formed by the dura mater seemed filled with a true fluid, which, indeed, flowed upwards or downwards, as the body was inclined. A punc- ture being made through the sheath, a consider- able quantity of fluid instantly escaped.” The last passage of this report, in reference to the puncture of the dura mater, does not seem to me very exact ; because in a preceding sen- tence it is stated, that “ below the part of the arachnoid, which adhered to the spinal cord, its proper membrane seemed charged with a great number of vessels, both arteries and veins, all loaded with blood.” This description leads me to conceive that the dura mater was slit lengthwise, and separated from the arachnoid and proper investing membrane of the spinal cord. If this were not so, the dura mater must then have adhered to the two inner membranes, and the puncture, consequently, could not have been confined to the first alone. I'am confirmed in this opinion by what M. Magendie says, when he adds, that the man had probably lost a good third of the nervous matter of the spinal cord, that the communication between its cervical and dorsal parts was, so to say, maintained by the membrane only ; for there seemed to remain no more of its entire thickness than a layer of white 46 substance, scarcely two lines broad, and pro- bably altered in its structure. The state of the nervous pairs, corresponding to the part of the spinal cord that was destroyed, ought also, in my opinion, to have been men- tioned. Information on the structure of the re- maining thin layer of nervous substance would likewise have been interesting ; was it composed of longitudinal fibres, serving as a means of communication between the upper and lower portions of the spinal cord ? Is it at all probable, that this office was performed by the sheath as stated ? If it was, the sheath, most certainly, was neither the arachnoid nor the dura mater. The nevrilema, that envelopes the pulpy nervous sub- stance immediately, could alone execute such an office. M. Boulay, jun., veterinary surgeon at Paris, relates the case of a horse, whose hind legs were completely paralyzed, whilst their sensibi- lity was extreme. On opening his body, the whole of the lower part of the spinal cord was found soft and diffluent. There were no traces of change in the superior portion. The nervous substance of the lumbar and sacral pairs of nerves was but little consistent, and their sheaths were red and inflamed. The distinction of the nervous roots into dor- sal and abdominal, accords with the two sorts of function performed by the spinal mass. This 47 subject, however, still requires elucidation ; for M. Magendie remarks, that “ when the posterior roots, covered even with their sheaths, are irri- tated, signs of extreme suffering are manifested ; and, what is particularly deserving of notice, contractions of those muscles that receive nerves from below the place so irritated are excited ; these contractions, too, only occur on the side of the body the nervous fibres of which are pricked.” According to the observations of the same author, the abdominal surface of the spinal cord is not altogether insensible when irritated. The communication between the spinal cord and the nerves of the vegetative functions is also known to be established by means of fibres, which communicate directly with the abdominal roots of the spinal nerves ; nevertheless, the will has no influence over the functions of the vis- cera. Moreover, the fibres of the dorsal roots are evidently larger than those of the abdominal ; both are in proportion to the volume of the parts to which they are distributed, and both send off branches that run into muscles. It is improbable, therefore, that the dorsal roots are solely des- tined to general sensation. Neither does it seem to me at all likely, that the spinal cord and its nerves are mere conductors of sensation, and of volition in reference to motion. I rather con- ceive that they aid in maintaining the powers of those parts to which they are distributed ; for in- 48 stance, that the muscles, or instruments of mo- tion, acquire their power, in part, through the in- fluence of their nerves, whilst the will to make the muscles act resides in the brain. Thus I do not believe that the only office of the spinal cord, with its nervous roots, is to establish a commu- nication between external impressions and the brain, and between the brain and the instruments of motion — the muscles. To me it seems pro- bable, that a very small part of the spinal cord suffices for these purposes ; the particular por- tion, or organ, however, cannot, in the present state of our knowledge, be specified. The ex- periments of M. Magendie prove the abdominal roots to include the conductors of volition ; but as each of these is composed of two halves, the one superior, the other inferior, and, in man, of two distinct cords, it would be interesting to re- peat and to extend the experiments, and by cutting the halves separately, to ascertain whether both propagate the dictates of the will ; or if this task is limited to one, to that, namely, which does not communicate with the inter- costal nerves. The ganglions of the intercostal nerves, as well as those of the dorsal roots of the spinal cord, may possibly prevent the will influencing the functions of the parts, to which these nerves are distributed. The set of experiments instituted by Dr. Bel- lingeri, and detailed in a Memoir read before the 49 Royal Academy of Sciences at Turin, in Fe- bruary, 1824, do not tend to throw any new light on this interesting subject. They confirm the general idea upon the presence of separate nerves of sensation and of motion in the spinal cord ; but they, farther, accord motion to the nerves that issue from the dorsal roots. Dr. Bel- lingeri says, his experiments prove, 1st, that the posterior roots of the lumbar and sacral nerves produce the motions of extension in the lower extremities ; 2nd, that the posterior roots alone preside over sensation ; 3rd, that the anterior roots produce the motions of flexion in the sa- cral extremities, and do not aid in perceiving external impressions ; 4th, that the posterior bundles of the spinal cord preside over the mo- tions of extension of the inferior extremities, and have no connexion with perceptions of touch ; 5th, that the white substance of the spinal cord, and the nervous fibres that arise from it, are ap- propriated to motion ; and, 6thly, that the gray substance of the cord and the nervous fibres that spring from it, belong to sensation. I have copied these statements from the ‘ Revue Encyclopedique,’ vol. xxi. p. 723. The ‘Bulletin des Sciences Medicales,’ for June, 1825, also records these experiments, the inferences from which are in opposition to those deduced by M. Magendie from his own. I do not think with Dr. Bellingeri, that the E 50 vertebral nerves can be divided into those which come from the white, and those which issue from the gray, substance of the spinal cord ; because, on examining the structure of this part and its nerves, I find that the origins of the latter inva- riably present the same appearance ; they are universally implanted, as it were, into the gray substance of the cord. From all I have said, it must be evident that I do not doubt the presence of two distinct species of nerves among those that issue from the spinal cord in particular, one propagating impressions from without, the other conveying the dictates of volition, in regard to motion, from within. This subject, however, is obviously involved in much obscurity, and will require ulterior and farther investigation to be rendered clear. Let us now examine the structure of the ner- vous mass of the spine, known under the title of spinal cord, or spinal marrow. CHAPTER I. Of the Nervous IS lass of the Vertebral Column. The spinal cord is found in all vertebral animals, whilst in the avertebralia, such as worms, in- sects, the Crustacea, and mollusca, the nerves form masses, which are separate and distinct in 51 the ratio of their functions. The digestive powers of the avertebralia commonly predominate over those of general sensibility ; and the same gan- glions that supply the viscera with nerves, also supply the muscles. In the vertebralia, on the contrary, where sensation and motion play prin- cipal parts, there are particular nervous masses destined to each kind of function. The spinal cord having always the same func- tions, may be conveniently compared in the four classes of vertebral animals. In all, its structure is essentially the same ; modifications only are to be observed. General Considerations on the Spinal Cord of Man and Animals. The nervous mass of the spine is composed of two similar halves, one on each side of the mesial plane of the body. They are parted to a certain depth by two longitudinal clefts, the one, of course, on the dorsal, (pi. i. fig. 5,) the other on the abdo- minal surface, (pi. i. fig. 4, s — s ), and united be- tween these fissures by a commissure, or appara- tus of union, (pi. i. fig. 6 and 7. 9- — 9-). This com- missure is pierced in its interior by a canal, which is more or less distinct in different animals, (pi. i. fig. 8 and 9,) and is especially visible during the earliest periods of life. The canal of the spinal cord has given rise to E 2 muc h discussion, and nothing certain is even now concluded in regard to it, neither as to the space it occupies, nor to the mode of its formation. Be- fore explaining my own views upon this point, I shall quote a passage, published in the first vo- lume of our large work on the “ Anatomy and Physiology of the Brain and Nervous System.” “ M. Demangeon and Messrs. Devilliers, uncle and nephew, afforded us the opportunity of exa- mining a case of Spina bifida, conjoined with considerable hydrocephalus. From the second to the fourth lumbar vertebra, the spinous pro- cesses of which were wanting, there extended a membraneous pouch about two inches in diame- ter. .During the eighteen days which the child survived its birth, a great quantity of fluid exuded continually from this pouch. Having cut away some of the other vertebrae, we observed no swell- ing of the dura mater, neither did any fluid escape when we slit open this membrane lengthwise. It was only between the arachnoid and the proper vascular coat, that a small quantity had accumu- lated, and it communicated with the pouch. “ The spinal cord was of its usual form. We shook the head, and turned it in every direction, but could perceive no communication between the water collected in the ventricles of the cerebral hemispheres and the spinal cord or its membranes. “ To learn, positively, whether or not any communication did exist between the pouch and 53 interior of the spinal cord, we cut this across in the neck ; it appeared in the usual state : on blow- ing, however, through a pipe upon the transverse cut, each half of the cord presented an openings about the magnitude of a middle-sized goose-quill. The two canals were separated by the commis- sure. We could not inflate the spinal cord through its entire length at once. We could only effect this partially. Downwards, the air penetrated freely for three inches, but there the canals were no farther pervious, so that no communication existed between them and the pouch. Neither could we perceive that there was any fluid in the canals. The pouch, itself, was formed partly by the dura mater and tunica arachnoides. Its supe- rior edge touched the lower extremity of the spinal cord. Although we had blown into the two halves of the spinal marrow in so easy and uniform a manner, we still could not determine whether the canals were to be considered as an effect of disease or not. We, therefore, examined the spines of children at the period of birth, of others somewhat older, and of adults ; in all we disco- vered a corresponding organization. We, however, found that the canals did not expand before the blow-pipe so readily in children somewhat ad- vanced, and in adults, as they did in subjects in the earliest infancy ; we, therefore, to show the fact, prefer the bodies of newly-born children. In other respects, precisely similar openings on 54 each side are to be observed in all ; the interior surfaces too, being always smooth. If the blow- ing into the canals be continued from below until as many as six or eight lines are opened, and only five or six lines are cut off in succession, so that the opening may be always maintained, the canals may be followed into the medulla oblongata, the tuber annulare, under the corpora quadrigemina, through the crura cerebri, and even on to the supposed optic thalami and the commencement of the corpora striata. Although no opening be observable into the fourth ventricle, nor into the aqueduct of Sylvius, nor into any other of the ce- rebral cavities, this structure, nevertheless, would incline us to admit the possibility of a fluid being secreted by the canals, and producing a true dropsy of one or both cavities of the spinal cord. “ Each half of the spinal cord then may be con- sidered as a membrane doubled on itself, along the middle line of which the gray substance, although without apparent division, may, by a slight blast, be separated, so as to form the sides of a canal. “ No violent separation can here be supposed ; for, in the first place, it is effected with perfect ease, whilst in other situations, and even where the gray substance is much softer, no disjunction can be operated without the greatest difficulty. Secondly, where a separation of parts is the con- sequence of violence, the edges are ragged and uneven ; whilst in the case cited, the surfaces are 55 continuous and smooth. Thirdly, the openings in the situations mentioned, are always found in the same places, are perfectly equal, being sometimes quite round, sometimes oval-shaped, sometimes slightly angular, and running now ho- rizontally, then vertically, obliquely, or in a semi- circular direction, according as the curves of the medulla oblongata, of the pons Varolii, of the crura cerebri, of the optic thalami, or the parts that surround the canal determine. If the air be propelled too forcibly, the canal will burst where the gray substance is the most slightly covered by nerves, as at the places where the nerves issue, although the tearing off of these does not, of itself, give any outlet to the air.” Since the above was written, I have seen rea- son to change my opinion, and I now consider the canals to be mere effects of the blowing. I have produced them in birds and fishes as well as in man. They are not, however, I must here observe, to be confounded with the true canal of the spinal cord — the canal that exists in the inte- rior of the commissure or apparatus of union. This is quite uniform, and is occasionally the seat of disease, getting filled with serum. It is more or less distinct in animals of the inferior classes, and in the embryos of those of superior tribes. In the human foetus, it is commonly visible during the first four or five months from conception. After this date it is generally, though not inva- 56 riably, obliterated. This explains how its exist- ence has been at one time admitted, at another denied, and how, in extraordinary cases, it has even been found in advanced old age, as Charles Etienne, Columbus, Morgagni, Senac, Portal, and other authors have observed. The canal of the spinal cord appears to be analogous to the aqueduct of Sylvius. M. Serres has said that the spinal marrow in young embryos consists of two cords, which unite first in front and compose a gutter, but which coming together, speedily get blended behind. The interior of the spinal marrow is then hollow, and forms a long canal, which is gradually filled up by the deposition of successive layers of gray substance secreted by the pia mater, which has insinuated itself into the canal. This account does not appear to me correct and conformable to nature. I allow that the two cords unite at the bottoms of the clefts, but I likewise maintain that each contains gray matter in its in- terior, in the direction of the two nervous roots that are implanted. But were the spinal mass developed, and its canal obliterated, as M. Serres would have us believe, by the deposition of gray substance in successive layers, this process ought, evidently, to go on in the interior of each of the cords composing it, and not in the canal between the two similar halves. The corresponding parts of each half of the 57 spinal cord are voluminous in the ratio of their implanted nerves. This proposition is strikingly exemplified, by comparing the spinal cord of ani- mals, whose superior are larger than their inferior extremities, with that of others, whose inferior are of greater size than their superior limbs. I have already said that there is a mass of gray substance contained in the interior of each half of the spinal cord, proportionate, in quantity, to the volume of the corresponding parts of the same, and to the nervous roots that issue at the place. The gray substance is disposed in a crescent form on either side of the apparatus of union, towards which the concave aspects are turned. (PI. i. fig. 9 and 10). The white fibres of the spinal cord, on both abdominal and dorsal surfaces, follow the gray substance throughout its extent, so that there are two nervous roots in each half : one dorsal, another abdominal. (PI. i. fig. 9 and 10.) The dorsal roots, generally, and the entire dorsal mass (PI. i. fig. 9 and 10), are more considerable than the abdominal roots and corresponding half. The dorsal roots communicate with the intervertebral ganglions (pi. i. fig. 3, 4, a— a), and the latter are proportionate to the former. No division of the spinal cord into dorsal and abdominal portions is effected by two such chan- nels as separate it into halves laterally; it is, at most, only marked by the ligamentum d&ntatum 58 which attaches the cord, on either side, to the dura mater. (PI. i. fig. 3, 4, 5, and 6.) Neither is it proper to look on the lateral portions of the spinal cord, as each composed of three bundles extend- ing from its one to its other extremity. They are much rather to be considered as nervous mem- branes folded on themselves, and forming a tube, containing gray substance. (PI. i. fig. 10.) The fissures on the abdominal and dorsal aspects of the spinal cord, are quite constant. The first is shallower, but more conspicuous, than the other. (PI. i. fig. 10.) The abdominal nervous roots of the spinal cord communicate with the nerves of the thorax and abdomen ; those of the dorsal surface fre- quently inosculate, and filaments from one pair of nerves often run to join the fasciculi composing another. (PI. i. fig. 5.) Among the general considerations on the spinal cord, the question respecting its uses still remains for examination. Anatomists have been in the frequent habit of speaking of a central mass in the nervous sys- tem, from which they conceive all its other parts to arise. The brain, cerebellum, and spinal cord, have very commonly been so entitled. M. de Blainville, however, separates the brain and ce- rebellum from the central mass. These he ar- ranges along with the external senses and inter- vertebral ganglions under the general title, Gan- 59 glionary Portion of the nervous system, and he confines the central mass exclusively to the spi- nal cord and medulla oblongata. In his view, the ganglionary is only added to, and not derived from, or produced by, the central portion. The two nervous roots of the spinal cord are, he says, mere fibres of communication between the cen- tral part of the nervous system and the nerves of sensation and voluntary motion, which originate in the intervertebral ganglions. To me anatomical, as well as physiological, facts, seem to militate against this opinion of M. de Blainville. In the first place, the inter- vertebral ganglions belong to the dorsal roots only of the spinal nerves. Our author, conse- quently, forgets to mention the origin and use of the abdominal roots entirely. But is it not pro- bable, that both dorsal and abdominal roots arise in the same manner? The filaments of the spinal roots, too, are in proportion to the corresponding masses of the cord whence they issue. Now, as the abdominal roots have no intervertebral gan- glions, and as they are evidently detached from the spinal cord, ought we not to conclude that the dorsal roots are so in like manner ? Another proof of the untenableness of M. de Blainville’s notion, exists in the size of the ner- vous filaments of the spinal cord. The apparatus of communication is never so large as the parts that communicate ; what a difference, for ex- 60 ample, between the volume of the nervous masses of vegetative and of phrenic life, and their communicating fibres ! Neither is M. de Blainville’s opinion supported by physiology. The ganglionary portion, he con- ceives to be sufficient for the performance of its own functions, that is to say, adequate of itself to feel, and to cause the execution of voluntary motion ; what then becomes of the central por- tion ? what is its use ? Admitting it to establish communications between the different parts of the nervous system, it cannot, however, exist solely for such a purpose ; its volume is by much too considerable to permit such a supposition to be entertained. Again, it most certainly is not destined to the affective and intellectual operations, for there is no proportion between them and the developement of the spinal cord. On the contrary, it is certain that general sensa tion and voluntary motion are in direct relation to the volume of the nervous masses of the ver- tebral column. Animals, eminently endowed with these two functions, have always a considerable spinal cord, and its parts proportionate to the organs which receive their nerves from thence. The several parts of the spinal cord are also augmented, as the apparatuses of sensation and of motion are increased in number or size. Further, the spinal cord is developed at a much earlier period of life than the cerebral masses ; * 61 it has acquired solidity and firmness, while the brain is still pulpy and devoid of fibres; and in harmony with this law, children display great muscular activity, — their love of bodily exercise is insatiable, before their mental faculties appear in any degree of vigour. M. Serres, who admits all the organic systems, the nervous in particular, to be developed from the circumference towards the centre, maintains that the nerves are fully developed when the spinal cord and brain are still in a fluid state. In conformity with this his hypothesis, he denies that the spinal cord gives origin to its nerves. These, according to him, are only implanted into it. Dr. Bailly conceives, as I have said, the same nervous elements to belong to every particular vertebra, i. e . , nerves of sensation, of motion, of digestion, and of the intellectual functions. He speaks of eight cords that compose the intellec- tual system of the vertebral column : the infe- rior mediam cord, continuous with the pyramidal eminence, and terminating anteriorly in the ce- lebral hemisphere ; the lateral inferior cord, end- ing in the internal layer of the corpora quadrige- mina ; the superior lateral cord, which termi- nates in the cerebellum ; and the inferior median cord, finishing in the lateral convolutions of the medulla, — convolutions which are most largely 62 developed in cartilaginous fishes, and which cor- respond to the ribbon of gray substance of mam- miferous animals. These eight longitudinal cords, four on each side, according to this au- thor, exercise functions analagous to those of the cerebral hemispheres, the internal slip of the quadrigeminal bodies, the cerebellum, and the lateral convolutions of the fourth vertricle in fishes, or the gray band in the mammalia. The medulla oblongata, in the same gentleman’s view, presents alternately an intellectual cord and a ner- vous pair. These physiological suppositions will, I make no doubt, share the fate of so many others, that are now forgotten. In my opinion, the spinal cord is, 1st, the origin of the nerves styled spinal ; 2nd, an apparatus that contributes to muscular and sensitive powers ; and 3rd, a means of communication between the cerebral opera- tions, the sense of touch, the power of motion in general, and the functions of vegetative life col- lectively. Particular Considerations in regard to the Spinal Cord. Before the development of extremities in the embryo, in animals also that naturally have none, and in cases of monstrosity where they are want- 63 ing, the spinal cord has no enlargements. These appear with, and are in proportion to, the extre- mities.' In the lower animals the spinal cord extends farther into its bony canal than in the human kind. Up to the fourth month of the human embryo’s existence, it runs to the extremity of the coccyx ; but the peculiar structure, termed cauda equina, which is more remarkable in man than in any other animal, becomes more and more apparent after this period. The caudal ap- pearance is produced by the sacral and lumbar nerves. The spinal cord of the human kind com- monly terminates by one or two little knobs, (of which the superior, when there are two, is the larger,) opposite to the second lumbar vertebra, and it is then attached by means of a tendinous slip to the bottom of the vertebral canal. The spinal cord is commonly divided into four portions: a cervical, dorsal, lumbar, and sacral. Each of these detaches several pairs of nerves, the number of which varies in different species of animals ; in man it amounts to thirty pairs, five being sacral, a like number lumbar, twelve dorsal, and seven cervical. The spinal cord of man, as of animals, is en- larged, and contains a larger quantity of gray substance at those places where the great nerves of the extremities are detached, than at any 64i oilier. Although many anatomists, M. Serres among the rest, deny that the spinal cord of man and animals is enlarged at, and contracted be- tween, the origins of each pair of nerves, I still adhere to the opinion which, in conjunction with Dr. Gall, I published long ago ; and I again ap- peal to nature for confirmation of its accuracy. These alternate enlargements and contractions are more or less conspicuous, in the ratio of the volume of the nervous pairs ; they are, for in- stance, more apparent in the ox and horse than in man. If, however, the human spinal cord, stripped of its dura mater, and arachnoid cover- ing, be held profile-wise against the light, the undulated line of its edges will be abundantly obvious. In the large work on the “ Anatomy of the Brain and Nervous System,” we have given drawings of this structure from the spinal cord of the calf and of man. PI. i. fig. 3, is a portion of the spinal cord of a calf. Several anatomists have spoken of numerous transverse folds to be observed, especially on the abdominal surface of the spinal cord ; these be- come very distinct when the cord is taken from out its bony canal, and one end is brought towards the other; they disappear entirely, however, when the part is stretched. Such folds, there- fore, appear to result from the bending ot the cord, and not to belong to any original pecu liarity of organization. 65 On either side, and at some little distance from the great median fissure, two superficial channels may be observed on the dorsal surface of the human spinal cord (pi. i. fig. 5, a — a.) They ex- tend as far as, or a little way beyond, the first dorsal vertebra. The two bands between the median and these superficial fissures, are de- veloped at the earliest period ; their structure is even complete, when the rest of the spinal cord is still a grayish and pulpy mass. The bands on the external edges of the dorsal and abdominal median fissures, appear, in general, to attain ma- turity of structure before any of the lateral masses. By opening the mesial abdominal fissure, and stripping off the vascular tissue, the nervous fibres will be seen running lengthwise and parallel to the lateral bands, (pi. i. fig. 6 ;) but in the dorsal cleft, they will be found descending perpendicu- larly from the surface towards its bottom. (PI. i. fig. 7.) The structure of the apparatus of union has not the same appearance at the bottom of both fissures. If the edges of the one on the dorsal aspect be gently separated, two white bands will be disco- vered running lengthwise (pi. i . fig. 7, S- — 9-) , almost as occurs in the middle line of the great cerebral commissure (raphe of the corpus callosum) ; at the bottom of the abdominal fissure, on the con- trary, filaments will be observed running trans- versely from the sides towards the median line. F 66 These bundles do not meet, they rather interlace, each terminating on either side between the two that come from the opposite side. (PI. i. fig. 6,^ — &.) Besides the peculiarities of its shortness com- pared with the vertebral canal, and of its termina- tion in the cauda equina, the spinal cord of man presents another striking peculiarity in the direc- tion in which its nerves are detached. The direction in which the spinal nerves are sent off, varies widely in different classes of animals. In man, their course is downwards and outwards ; this depends evidently on the vertical posture, by which man is distinguished from the other mam- malia, and on the shortness of his spinal cord compared with its bony canal. In the human kind, only the first pair of cervical nervous roots on the dorsal surface (pi. i. fig. 5), and the two first pairs upon the abdominal (pi. i. fig. 4), have one set of bundles coming from above, down- wards, and another from below, upwards ; all the other pairs are detached more and more obliquely downwards, to gain the vertebral holes by which they issue, in proportion as they arise nearer to the sacral extremity of the spinal cord. In other animals, on the contrary, whose spinal nervous masses occupy the entire length of the vertebral canal, the nerves are sent off directly opposite to the intervertebral spaces at which they issue. (PI. i. fig. 3.) I have still to speak of the superior extremity 67 of the spinal cord. By the words spinal marrow or cord, most anatomists understand the nervous mass that extends throughout the spinal canal up to the occipital hole; others, Messrs. Soem- merring and Chaussier among the number, say that it goes as far as the annular protuberance. They consequently include the pyramidal and olivary bodies, the accessory, vocal, glossopha- l’yngeal and hypoglossal nerves, all the mass, in short, commonly named medulla oblongata , They speak of two portions — one cranial, another spinal, of the spinal cord; the first being, of course, con- tained in the skull, the second in the canal of the vertebral column. The organization of the spinal nervous mass cannot be said to change entirely when it enters the cranium: several pairs of nerves are still de- tached precisely in the same manner as they are in the spine; the nerves too, in both cases, per- form similar functions. The proper spinal masses are also intimately connected with those of the medulla oblongata. In this last part, however, the abdominal fissure is interrupted by the decussa- ting bundles of the pyramidal bodies ; the size of the mass is also very sensibly increased here, and there is no regular proportion between the proper spinal cord and the mass that extends from the occipital hole to the annular protuberance. More- over, the roots of several cerebral nerves, as they are styled, are found in this last mass, as also the F 2 68 rudimentary parts of the brain and cerebellum. Lastly, the word spinal refers to a particular situa- tion — the spine. These reasons may suffice to make us limit the title spinal cord to the mass included in the vertebral canal, and extending from the occipital hole or commencement of the pyramidal decussation to the cauda equina or horse’s tail. CHAPTER II. Of the pretended Cerebral Nerves ; or, of the Cranial Nerves of the External Senses and Voluntary Motion. It is usual for anatomists to consider the ner- vous mass lying between the occipital hole and annular protuberance, as distinct and particu- lar. In former times, this part was taken for a prolongation of the brain and cerebellum, and, therefore, called medulla oblongata. In man it is composed of parts, named severally, pyramidal bodies, which are two in number, anterior (pi. vi. fig. 1., 1 — c.) and posterior (pi. xi. in the middle line) ; olivary bodies (pi. vi. fig 1 , a.); restiform bodies (pi. xi. e. e.) ; accessory nerves (pi. i. fig. 3, 4, and 5; pi. vi. fig. 1,2 — 3); hypoglossal nerves /pi. vi. 4) ; vocal nerves (ib. 6) ; glossopharyngeal 69 nerves (ib. 7)7 < auditor y nerves ( ib - 9 ) ; facial nerves (ib. 11); ana abductor nerves (ib. 10.) At the conclusion of thP last chapter, I remarked that it was a mistake to coi?f° und die part called medulla oblongata and the spinal cord together. I here subjoin that the medulla oblongata j s no t a separate and particular nervous ma.ss : it gives origin to the nerves above-mentioned, atfd also to the fifth pair; but there is yet one portion of each of its halves which belongs, decidedly, to the cerebellum, and another which pertains to the brain. The volume of the medulla oblongata yaries greatly in the different classes of animals ; its size is determined by the nerves that arise from if* and by the bundles that proceed to the cerebellum and brain. Its increase beyond the size of the spinal cord, is more remarkable in reptiles than in fishes, in birds than in reptiles, and is espe- cially considerable in the mammalia, which, gene- rally, have the medulla oblongata as well as spinal cord proportionately larger than man, because the nerves detached from these parts are larger in them than in the human kind. At present, I mean only to speak of the sup- posed cerebral nerves. The pyramidal, olivary, and restiform bodies will be examined along with the cerebellum and brain of which they are parts. The opinions of authors upon the origin of the nerves of the head, are very various. In general 70 they are derived from the brain, hence their name cerebral. But some writers limit the title brain to the hemispheres, and join the striated bodies, the optic tbalami, the cerebral legs and annular protuberance to the medulla oblongata; or, otherwise, they look on all these as parts of the spinal cord, and then say that no nerve what- ever arises from the brain, that is, from the hemi- spheres immediately. By thus extending the limits of the medulla oblongata and spinal cord, how'- ever, parts that belong essentially to the brain are included. The principle we lay down, there- fore, that no nerve originates in the brain, and that every nervous part has its own origin, so that the nerves can no more be derived from the brain than can the various nervous pairs from each other, cannot possibly be confounded with any of the foregoing opinions of authors. The proofs we adduce in support of our posi- tion, as to the independent origin of the various nervous parts, are constantly the same ; all, there- fore, that has been said to demonstrate the spinal cord to be no continuation of the brain, applies to the nerves of the head, and proves that they do not owe their being to the brain, and that no one pair derives its existence from any other. In the first place, the nerves of the head bear no pro- portion to the brain in size; and then, these nerves exist in acephalous monsters, whose brain never had being. Moreover, the course taken 71 by the nerves towards the cranial holes through which they pass, proves in the most positive man- ner that they are not continuations of the cerebral fibres. In examining the individual nerves, I shall speak of their several peculiarities, as origin, structure, and connexion are concerned. The nerves of the head, as of the spine, have been long classed into pairs. The number of pairs reckoned, how r ever, varies considerably ; sometimes they are said to be seven, sometimes eight, or nine, or according to M. Soemmerring’s method of counting as many as twelve. How- ever, the mode of indicating the several pairs of nerves numerically, and speaking of the first pair, the second pair, the third pair, and so on, pos- sesses no practical advantages, because, besides the number, the functions of each pair must still be learned. It is better, therefore, to designate each pair of the cranial nerves in succession from its functions or its destination. Some authors have divided the nerves accor- ding to their places of detachment, into nerves of the brain, of the cerebellum, of the annular protu- berance, and of the medulla oblongata. This attempt at classification was necessarily very de- ficient, for it is based upon an error in regard to the origins of the nerves ; the actual place of origin being confounded with that at which they issue immediately from the general mass. 72 Of the Accessory Nerve. The accessory nerve is found in man and the other mammalia. (PI. i. fig. 3, 4, 5, and pi. vi. fig. 1, 2 — 3.) It arises from the cervico-spinal mass and medulla oblongata. Its filaments come from the dorsal surface of these parts, and vary in number, in thickness, and in length, not only in different individuals of different species, but even on the two sides of the same subject. The first filaments are detached at various heights, sometimes higher, sometimes lower, in the spinal cord, issuing from the level of the seventh cervi- cal pair in one instance, and from that of the fifth in another. The accessory recedes from the spinal cord and medulla oblongata, as it ap- proaches the pneumogastric nerve, along with which it escapes from the cranium. I have al- ready said, that Mr. Charles Bell arranges the accessory among the nerves of respiration. In contributing to this function, it produces mo- tion, and is influenced by the will ; nevertheless it is detached, as we have seen, from the dorsal surface of the spinal cord. Of the Pneumogastric Nerve. The pneumogastric occurs in all vertebral! ani- mals. In man (pi. vi. fig. 1, 6), it issues by nu- 73 merous filaments between the olivary, (ibid a,) and the restiform (e e) bodies, nearer to the lat- ter, however, than to the former. It unites with a great number of other nerves, a circumstance that has obtained for it the title vagus. Its branches run to the larynx and pharynx, to the thyroid gland, the vessels of the neck, and the great conduits of the heart, to the lungs, the liver, spleen, pancreas, stomach, and duode- num. As some of its principal branches are dis- tributed to the organs of voice, and as its le- sions derange the functions of these parts, it has also been called the vocal nerve. The commu- nications by its means established, and its ex- tensive distribution, explain the sympathies that exist between the throat, lungs, stomach, heart, &c. Of the Glossopharyngeal Nerve. In the mammalia and man, (pi. vi. fig. 1, 7,) this nerve comes off from the medulla oblongata, just before the pneumogastric, by a great many filaments, which, speedily uniting into one or more bundles, compose a trunk that runs to be ramified on the pharynx and muscles of the tongue. It appears to be destined to general sensation. 74 , Of the Hypoglossal Nerve. The hypoglossal in man arises partly near the olivary and pyramidal bodies, and partly lower down, by several filaments that are detached, and get united after the manner of the cervical nerves.- (PL vi. fig. 1, 4.) It supplies the tongue with motive power, and acts in mastication, de- glutition, speaking, singing, &c. Of the Abductor Nerve of the Eye. This nerve arises in all mammiferous animals, from the abdominal surface of the medulla ob- longata. (PI. vi. fig. 1, 10.) In some, as the horse, ox, and deer, it mounts all the way along with, and by the side of, the pyramidal bodies, in the form of a band, which, on reaching the annular protuberance, divides into two. In the human subject it is generally covered by some transverse fibres of the protuberance. Its dis- tribution, as its name implies, is to the abductor muscle of the eye. Of the Facial Nerve. The facial is detached from the spinal cord in the same manner as the nerves I have just dis- cussed. Its true origin is readily seen in the 75 lower animals ; but in man, (pi. vi. fig. 1 , 11,) it (or some of its filaments at least) seems to come from the annular protuberance. This happens in consequence of its being covered entirely, or partially, by the transverse fibres of the part mentioned. The distribution of this nerve is to the muscles of the face ; it also communicates freely with all the three branches of the fifth pair. Of the Motor Nerve of the Eye. The filaments composing this nerve, (pi. vi. and pi. x. fig. 1, 15,) issue from the blackish body of the cerebral legs (pi. x. fig. 1, 30) ; these unite and go to supply the superior, inter- nal, inferior straight, and inferior oblique, muscles of the eye, and the elevator of the upper eye-lid. It is detached in all vertebral animals, from the cerebral crura behind the tuber cinereum, or ash- coloured tubercle, (pi. vi. 17,) which is situated immediately backwards from the junction of the optic nerves. Of the Nerve of the Superior Oblique Muscle of the Eye. This nerve, (pi. vi. 13,) springs by several fila- ments behind the posterior pair of quadrigemi- nal bodies in the mammalia, (pi. xi. fig. 1, o,) and 76 behind the bigeminal bodies in the other classes of animals. It has no prerogative over the other nerves of motion in expressing the affections and passions ; the name patheticus, or ogling nerve, therefore, which it has obtained, is misapplied. Although a nerve of motion, it arises from the dorsal surface of the nervous mass. Of the Trigeminal Nerve. The trigeminal (pi. vi. fig. 1, 12) arises from the medulla oblongata. Its place of origin is ap- parent in those animals, as reptiles, birds, and fishes, which have no annular protuberance, and also in the mammalia which have it of small size; but in man, the monkey, and other tribes, where this part is very large, the trigeminal seems to arise from it; the nerve, however, is, in fact, only covered by some of its transverse fibres, (pi. viii. fig. 1, i — k.) and the origin of the nerve is, in fact, the same in all animals. To shew it in the hu- man kind, the fibres of the annular protube- rance that cover it, must, of course, be dissected away. The trigeminal nerve is distributed to every part of the face, to the muscles of the forehead, of the eye-lids, nose, lips, jaws, and ears ; it communicates with the organs of all the five senses, and of voluntary motion, and brings these and the other parts of the body, the face, 77 neck, trunk, and extremities, into relationship mutually. One branch of the trigeminal is ramified on the tongue, and is regarded as the true gusta- tory nerve. It is, therefore, destined to cognize an impression of a specific kind, viz., taste. If some feel disposed to regard taste as a sort of touch, they must, at least, admit the impossibility of confounding or assimilating this species of per- ception with sensation in general. • Of the Auditory Nerve. The auditory (pi. vi. 9), as well as the nerve last discussed, must exist in the avertebral tribes, but its origin and peripheral expansion only become distinct and complex as we ascend in the scale of beings. The origin of the audi- tory nerve is in many fishes confounded with that of the facial and trigeminal ; in some kinds, however, it is more distinct, and is the same as in reptiles and birds. The auditory nerve always arises laterally and posteriorly to the cerebellum. In the mammalia it comes from the fourth ven- tricle, and traverses in its course a band of gray substance, lying between the restiform body and the cerebellum. In man certain white lines may commonly be seen in the fourth ventricle, (pi. ix. fig. 1. t.) which Piccoluomini was the first to con- sider as the origins of the auditory nerve. These 78 lines, or streaks, are almost imperceptible in some subjects, but in others they are very numerous and distinct. They are observed to vary on each side, being occasionally larger and more nume- rous on one than on the other Sometimes they occur disposed in rays, sometimes in pencils, and, again, they run in parallels, often lying higher on one than on the other side. They at one time appear as little flattened bands, at ano- ther as rounded and salient cords. Piccoluo- mini’s opinion of their use seems now to be very generally adopted ; it is certain, indeed, that some of them do unite with the auditory nerve, but others run to the anterior cerebellar lobes, and others dip into the middle of the cerebellum. Although many of the mammalia have much larger auditory nerves than man, these white fibres are nevertheless wanting in all. In them, there is a broad band on either side, extending from the one auditory nerve to the other, imme- diately behind the annular protuberance, and above all the ascending bundles, except the py- ramidal bodies. (PI. iv. fig. 2, c.) This band ap- pears to be an apparatus of union or commissure. It does not give origin to the facial nerve, as some anatomists have supposed. Of the Optic Nerve. Since the days of Eustachius and Varolius, the 79 majority of anatomists have derived this nerve (pi. vi. fig. 1, 20.) from certain masses, which have, in consequence, been entitled thalami of the optic nerves. (Pl. x. fig. 1, p .) Others, however, followed some of its fibres backwards to the anterior of the quadrigeminal bodies. (PI. xi. fig. 1, n .) And it is easy in the mammalia to do this. A broad slip issues from the anterior quadrigeminal body of either side, which turns round upon the outer edge of the thalamus, simply superimposed upon the cerebral crura, but attached to the neighbouring masses along its external edge, as far as its junction with its fellow of the opposite side. (PI. viii. fig. 1, w, iv, iv.) From the tuber cinereum, (pl. vi. fig. 1, 17.) the optic nerve receives many additional fibres, which join it in right lines, and without decussating. (Pl. viii. fig. 2, 20.) It is the opinion of many anatomists, that the optic nerves, at their junction, unite intimately without decussating ; many others, on the con- trary, think that either nerve crosses to the side opposite to that on which it had arisen. The numerous cases described by authors, and the facts which Dr. Gall and I have collected, prove that atrophy of one optic nerve is continued on the opposite side after the junction of the two. We, therefore, agree with those who recognise a partial decussation of the optic nerves in man and the mammalia. The fibres of the outermost 80 portions appear to continue their course onwards without decussating. It was a great error to consider the eminences called optic thalami, as the sources of the optic nerves. There is, in fact, no proportion what- ever between these parts and the nerves of vision. In the horse, ox, sheep, &c., the optic nerves are as large as in man, but the thalami, in the human kind, are much larger than in these animals. A very cursory examination of the structure of the thalami also shows a mere superficial layer attached to the optic nerve and the whole of the interior fibres proceeding in a divergent manner, backwards, to be distributed to the cerebral convolutions. (PI. x. fig. 1 , p. P. P.) When the optic nerve is affected with atrophy, the corresponding thalamus is diminished only in as far as the nerve itself is lessened; the inte- rior of the thalamus suffers no change, but the atrophy of the nerve continues on to the anterior quadrigeminal body which belongs to it. I once found, in the brain of a woman who had died insane, the thalamus of the left side half converted into pus; the corpus striatum of the same side was also much shrunk, but the optic nerve was healthy, and resembled, in all respects, its fellow of the opposite side, in the vicinity of which no organic change could be detected. The anterior pair of quadrigeminal bodies were also in their natural state. 81 The two thalami are rarely of equal size ; the one on the left side is commonly the smaller. It, therefore, becomes necessary to guard against attributing to atrophy of an optic nerve an ap- pearance that depends on natural conformation. Until Dr. Gall and I showed the mistake, the optic tubercles of birds and reptiles were con- founded with the thalami; these tubercles (pi. xi. fig. 2 and 3, n.), however, correspond to the ante- rior pair of quadrigeminal bodies. The parts, called thalami (pi. xi. fig. 2, p.), also exist in the two classes of creatures mentioned, besides the true optic ganglions, (ib. n .) It is a difficult matter to say whether the long elevated bodies that occur immediately behind the crossing of the optic nerves in fishes (pi. ii. fig. P, 6, 8, 16), and correspond to the optic ganglions of birds, that is to say, to the pair of ganglions (pi. iii. fig. 5, 6, 7, 10, and 11, n.) which comes im- mediately after the cerebellum, or whether they ought to be likened to the bodies which are styled mammillary. (PI. vi. fig. 1, 16.) The optic nerves of fishes seem to arise from, at all events they communicate with, these bodies. By comparing pi. ii. fig. 4< and 6, 16, with pi. iii. fig, 12, n., the optic ganglions in birds, and especially in mam- miferous animals, will be seen to be separated and pushed upwardly and laterally by the medulla oblongata, cerebral legs, and annular protube- rance. Moreover, the optic nerves in fishes adhera G 82 to several other parts, as they do in the higher classes of vertebral animals and in man. It is from this circumstance that a great part of the other masses, especially those that follow the cerebellum, are called optic ganglions; but these bear no proportion to the optic nerves (pi. ii. fig. 5, 7, and 10, n.), and to me, it seems more rea- sonable to allow, with M. Arsaky*, that the complicated structure of the optic ganglions in fishes, explains the functions of the derived nerves, which, in reptiles, birds, and the mammalia, are evidently aided in their functions by the other parts with which they communicate. The implantation of the optic nerve in fishes into the inner edge of its ganglion, which causes the greatest part of this mass to appear before and above it, bears a striking analogy to the adhesion of the same nerve to the brain in other classes of animals, and confirms the idea in ques- tion. It is rendered still more probable, as the same peculiarity of structure is found in con- nexion with the olfactory nerves of fishes. And further, the oblong bodies, which, in fishes, lie behind the crossing of the optic nerves, cannot possibly be supposed analogous to the mammillary bodies of mammiferous tribes, because these last belong to the fornix, and this is a part which fishes have not. I may still add that the mammil- * Dissertatio de Piscium Cerebro et Medulla Spiuali; Halce. 1313. 83 lary bodies bear no proportion to the optic nerves, and that the oblong bodies of fishes above-men- tioned do, regularly. The oblong bodies of fishes, probably, corre- spond to the gray tubercle, (tuber cinereum) of the mammalia. This tubercle, in the higher classes of beings, always sends fibres to the optic nerves, which, after this accession, advance of increased size in their course. (PI. viii. fig. 2 — 17.) The connexion of the optic nerve with such a number of cerebral parts, renders its exact origin uncertain. To appreciate this truth in its full force, it will be necessary to reflect on what I have still to say of the quadrigeminal bodies. Of the Quadrigeminal Bodies. The title, corpora, or tubercula quadrigemina, is applied to two pairs of round elevations, situated behind the legs of the brain. They are only found in the mammalia and in man. (PI. xi. fig. 1, n, o .) They are joined together by a transverse band (ib. x), and they communicate with the part called valve of Vieussens (ib. y ), with the bundles that proceed from the medulla oblongata, and with the pretended optic thalami (ib. p.). I have already spoken of the large band, belonging to the optic nerve, that issues from the anterior pair of these bodies. Being in communication with the bundles which come from the posterior G 2 84 . part of the medulla oblongata, there can be no doubt but the quadrigeminal bodies have their origin lower down in this nervous mass ; but as the optic nerves arise from the anterior pair in the mammalia, and as these nerves issue in birds from a couple of ganglions, separated from the general cerebral mass, (for, in birds, they are only united to the bundles that proceed from the me- dulla oblongata) the analogy and office of these ganglions cannot be called in question. The destination of the posterior pair of quadrigeminal bodies is much less obvious. M. Serres* thinks that the bigeminal tubercles of fishes, reptiles, and birds, and the quadrigemi- nal bodies of mammiferous animals and man, are the same mass, destined, in all, to originate the optic nerves, and that the division into two pairs happens merely from a transverse furrow, which, as it runs more or less forwards or backwards, equalizes them, or causes in one case the ante- rior, in another the posterior pair to predominate. The posterior are intimately connected with the anterior tubercles, it is true ; but there is a white band which issues from the anterior pair, joins a small collection of gray substance, called oorpus geniculatum externum (pi. viii. fig. 1, q), is thereby increased in size, and then continues its course into the optic nerve ; in like manner there * Anatomie du Cerveau ; Preface. Rapport de M. Cuvier. 85 runs a band from the posterior pair, which unites with a mass of gray substance, entitled corpus geniculatum internum (pi. xi. fig. 1, r-)> and gain- ing, consequently, in size, afterwards dips under the optic nerve, and is continued on towards the middle cerebral lobe. ' This structure proves, at the least, that the anterior and posterior pairs of the quadrigeminal bodies are no parts of one and the same nervous mass. M. Serres also fancies that the corpora quadri- gemina serve as a basis, according to which the other parts of the encephalon are determined (“ les tubercules quadrigeminaux servent de base a la determination des autres parties de I’encephale ); ” and he investigates their relations with many particular cerebral parts. His assumption seems to me as incorrect and untenable as the one I have just examined, according to which, the quadrigeminal bodies are parts of one mass similar to the bige- minal tubercles of birds and reptiles. “ The quadrigeminal tubercles,” says M. Serres, “ are developed in all the classes, and in every family of each class, in a ratio, directly as are the optic nerves and the eyes. Fishes have the largest quadrigeminal tubercles, and the most remarkable eyes and optic nerves. The very considerable size of their quadrigeminal bodies, indeed, has led anatomists, up to the present time, into the error of supposing them to be the hemispheres of 86 the brain. After fishes come reptiles, then birds, next, among mammiferous animals, the rodentia, and in succession, the ruminantia, the carnivora, the quadrumana and man.” I have already shown, that in fishes generally, the ganglions called optic, are disproportionate in size to the nerves of vision ; and I have said that part of these masses was, probably, destined to other functions. The optic nerve of the carp (pi. ii. fig. 4, 20) is smaller than that of the roach (pi. ii. fig. 11, 20), but the so styled optic ganglions (ib. n.) exist in these fishes in an in- verse proportion. “ The spinal marrow and the corpora quadri- gemina,” says M. Serres in another place/ £ are so rigorously developed in the ratio of each other that the size .of the first being given in any class or in any of its families, the volume of the latter may be determined with precision,” But the bigeminal tubercles in the carp (pi. ii. fig. 5, n.) are much larger, in proportion to the spinal cord, than in the eel (pi. ii. fig. 1) and the roach (pi. ii. fig. 11.). M. Serres, himself, has given representations of the optic apparatus in the cassowary, ostrich, and other birds, much larger in proportion to the spinal cord, than it is in the many mammiferous animals whose brains he has figured; and the disproportion between the developement of the quadrigeminal bodies 87 and spinal cord, is even greater in the dolphins and porpoises, than in the ox, camel, and horse. The most cursory glance over the physiology of living beings also shows the utter erroneousness of M. Serres’s position. Powers of voluntary motion and of touch never bear any direct pro- portion to the faculty of vision. The mole is, certainly, far more remarkable for its muscular strength than for its eye-sight, and the owl for its powers of vision than for its bodily vigour. I shall discuss the several relations which M. Serres believes he has found between the quadri- geminal bodies and other parts of the nervous system, as I treat of these in succession. M. Bailly, in his Memoir on the Comparative Anatomy of the Nervous System in the four Classes of Vertebral Animals, maintains that the corpora quadrigemina anteriora et posteriora, the corpora geniculata externa et interna are mere parts of the optic lobe of inferior classes. He speaks of the unfolding of the quadrigeminal bodies in fishes, and of two systems of fibres very different from each other: the one exterior and belonging to the optic nerves ; the other interior, and being the ex- pansion of a cord of the medulla oblongata. “ In fishes and reptiles,” says he, “ the internal have a much greater relative developement than the external fibres ; in mammiferous animals, the ex- ternal fibres alone remain ; the internal exist as mere rudiments.” 88 Dr. Gall* also says he is “ convinced that the posterior pair of the corpora quadrigemina is a ganglion, for the purpose of reinforcing or perfect- ing the optic nerve.” He adds, “ the modes in which these two pairs assist the function of vision must differ, for they occur in various proportions to each other, in the different species of animals ; in some, indeed, the posterior two are scarcely perceptible, or are even entirely wanting, although vision be perfect, as happens in the case of birds.” This diversity of opinion is to be accounted for, by recalling to mind the delicacy of the cere- bral organization, and the intimate connexion of the parts with each other. When anatomists ob- serve one or two parts connected together, they very commonly conceive the one to be derived from the other. By-and-by I shall treat, in a separate section, of the communications of the nervous masses, and of the importance of this arrangement. Meantime, I shall pass my opinions respecting the quadrigeminal bodies in review before my reader. The mammalia alone have quadrigeminal bo- dies. Both pairs, however, have not, I conceive, similar offices, for there is no proportion between them indiscriminately, nor the bundles of fibres which issue in different directions from each. The fibres of the anterior pair, as I have said, join the *Sur les Fonctions du Cerveau. — Ed. in 8vo. Vol. vi. p. 55. 89 optic nerves, those of the posterior plunge under the optic nerves, and are lost on the middle lobes of the brain. The optic nerve communicates, by means of superficial bands, with the posterior pair of the quadrigeminal bodies, with the internal and external geniculated bodies, with the mid- dle and anterior cerebral lobes, and with the mass called tuber cinereum. Reptiles and birds have only one pair of rounded tubercles, before and by the sides of the cere- bellum. (PI. iii. fig. 2, 3, 4>, 5, 7, 10 and 11, n .) These are readily seen, by throwing back the cerebellum. On examining their structure, they are found, in the first place, to be hollow (pi. iii. fig. 9, side B. n.), and to detach a super- ficial layer that communicates with the medulla oblongata behind, and with the optic nerve and base of the brain before ; and, in the second place, to be connected by their lower parts with the optic nerves and the cerebral crura or legs. This structure of the optic ganglions in birds and rep. tiles, corresponds to that of the anterior pair of the quadrigeminal and external geniculated bo- dies in mammiferous animals, as in them the one and superficial layer of the anterior pair is con- tinued into the optic nerve, whilst the other and deeper is connected with the crura of the brain. In fishes, the optic nerves always communicate with the basis of the pair of tubercles that suc- ceeds the cerebellum, and this pair is connected 90 with the medulla oblongata ; but it and the optic nerves have not, mutually, any regular or fixed proportion, whilst the optic nerves and the rounded tubercles that lie immediately behind their cross- ing, are constantly developed in the ratio of each other. The optic nerve, in fishes, moreover, com- municates with the basis of the cerebral masses that come after the cerebellum, precisely as it is connected in the mammalia with the anterior and middle lobes of the brain. In proportion, therefore, as the cerebral masses and fibrous bundles, or their successive additions, diminish in number through the four classes of vertebral animals, the primary optic ganglions approach each other, and lie between or among such cerebral parts as still exist; but as the brain gets complicated, and vision exerts an important influence upon its functions, the primary optic ganglions lie backwards, in order that the appa- ratus of vision may, conveniently, be brought into communication with the cerebral parts whose functions it especially aids. Of the Olfactory Nerve , Very different opinions have been, and are still entertained by anatomists in regard to the origin of the nerve of smell. Some of the moderns have described it as arising from the masses called striated bodies ; but there is no proportion, what- 91 ever, between them and the nerve ; and further, porpoises and dolphins have the striated bodies, but no olfactory nerves. The nerves of smell also exist in many acephalous monsters, whose striated bodies are, of course, wanting. The olfactory nerve, in the human kind, has three roots ; of these, the interior (pi. vi. 21) is the shortest, but broadest, and the exterior the longest (ib. 18), for it extends to the bottom of the fissure of Sylvius. These different roots are, as it were, impacted in the cerebral substance ; they approach by degrees, and having met, ad- vance in the form of a single nervous trunk. In man, the nerve parts from the anterior lobe at the place where the convolutions commence, and runs along the cleft formed between the innermost of the anterior and inferior of these, accompanied throughout its whole course by a very distinct streak of cineritious substance. Immediately above the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone, it encounters a considerable quantity of very soft gray substance, with which it forms a sort of bulb. (PI. vi. fig. 1, 23.) The nerve, here, gains a mighty increase in size, and passing by numerous filaments through the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone, it is lost upon the lining mem- brane of the nose. The olfactory nerve of monkies (pi. v. fig. 3), and of seals, resembles that of man very nearly. In the class mammalia, generally (pi. iv. fig. 2) 92 a great many nervous fibres may be seen arising from the anterior part of the middle cerebral lobe ; these join the fibres that spring from the inferior surface of the anterior convolutions, and compose a broad and rounded band, which, remaining attached to the anterior lobe, runs slightly inwards until it arrives at the ethmoid bone, when, pre- cisely, as in man, it meets a large mass of gray substance (pi. iv. fig. 2, 3, and 4, 23), and in- creases in size so much, that after its exit by the ethmoidal holes, it suffices to cover the entire surfaces of the large superior spongy bones. The bulb that is formed over the ethmoid bone, if it be incised, or have a piece taken out, will be found to be hollow. Its internal white layer in the lower animals communicates immediately with the anterior cavity of the brain, so that by blow- ing into the bulb of the olfactory nerve, the air will penetrate and inflate the lateral ventricles. Soemmerring says, that the olfactory nerve of the human embryo, at an early period, is also hollow, and that air blown into it reaches the cavities of the brain. The same experiment will, occasion- ally, though very rarely, succeed in the adult. When we observe the olfactory nerve so very large in the mammalia, whilst the mass of anterior convolutions is but inconsiderable, and, on the other hand, the nerve in the human kind so small and surrounded by the thick masses of the ante- rior lobe, we may conceive why the experiment 93 should succeed so readily in animals, and be so rarely practicable in man. The olfactory nerve, it may be almost unne- cessary to state, is proportionate in size to the extent of external apparatus over which it is dis- tributed. In birds the nerve of smell is detached from the anterior and inner part of the front lobe, but its fibres are distinct from those of the brain ; one of its bands too, which may be compared with the external root of the same nerve in man, runs towards the fissure of Sylvius and the middle lobe. This band, however, is not equally appa- rent in all birds. It is still less distinct in rep- tiles. In fishes the olfactory nerve arises by two very distinct roots ; certain fibres of great delicacy bring it into communication with the foremost cerebral ganglions, and others of a firmer tex- ture and whiter colour connect it with the lon- gitudinal band, which, in these animals, lies in the middle line between the various ganglions, and communicates with the medulla oblongata. (PI. ii. fig. 9 and 13.) Some anatomists have considered the entire mass of the anterior cerebral ganglion in the skate (pi. ii. fig. 8, 1,2, 3), and all the three pairs of gan- glions in the eel (pi. ii. fig. 1, 1, 2, 3), as des- tined to originate the olfactory nerve. But the error here committed is proclaimed by the fact 94 of the origins being always in proportion to the nerves themselves, and there being none what- ever between the masses mentioned and the ol- factory nerves of fishes. In regard to these gan- glions, therefore, that which has been stated respecting those of vision must be repeated : the olfactory nerve is in communication with cere- bral parts, destined to affective and intellectual functions. 95 Section IV. Of the best Method of dissecting the Brain. Before Dr. Gall and I began our researches on the structure of the brain, anatomists, in their dissections and descriptions, had no other object in view than to know the forms of the whole, or of its particular masses, the colours, connexions, and consistency, of its individual parts. To at- tain their end, they were in the habit of cutting down the brain by slices, and examining and noting the appearances presented by each in suc- cession, until they arrived at the base. “ The most accredited method, 5 ’ says M. Cuvier *, “ of the schools, and usually recommended in books of anatomy, is to take away successive slices of the organ (the brain), and to remark the appear- ances offered by each. This is the easiest in practice for the demonstration, but it is the most difficult for the imagination. The true relations of parts, which are always seen cut across, escape not the pupil alone, but the master himself.” Willis f was the first who objected to the * Rapport des Commissaires de l’lnstitut de France, on our Memoir, entitled “ Recherches sur le Systeme Nerveux en General, et sur le Cerveau en Particular.” t Cerebri Anatomia. 96 practice of considering, as distinct parts, all the forms accidentally produced by such a mode of dissection. He himself viewed the cerebral parts in their connexions. He says too, that the anatomical demonstration of the brain should be begun at the basis ; by basis, however, let me observe, he understood the striated bodies and thalami. Ascending from these to the superior parts, and returning on the inferior in succes- sion, his attention was confined to the larger masses, which he designates by names that indi- cate physical qualities only. “ From the striated bodies,” for example, he says, “ the legs of the medulla oblongata are prolonged ; remaining apart for a short way, they then approach and get blended into a common stalk, composed, as it were, of two peduncles or stems.” As the Committee of the French Institute have conceived themselves authorized to assimilate the method of dissecting the brain, described in our Memoir, with the plan pursued by Aarolius and Yieussens, and have interpreted these au- thors in a way which their language will not bear, I hope it will not be found amiss, if I extract a few passages from our Memoir, follow- ing them by some observations on the report. “ The brain,” say the Committee, “ is at- tacked from below ; the medulla oblongata is pursued across the bridge of Varolius, through the thalami of the optic nerves, and the striated 97 bodies, when its fibres expand and compose the hemispheres. The hemispheres also, if we choose, may, by tearing their lateral attachments to the crura cerebri, be unfolded, the medulla oblongata and cerebellum be split longitudinally, and each half of the former shewn as a sort of stem implanted into the hemisphere of its own side, like the stalk of a mushroom into its cap.” They add, “ It is probable that this method would have had more vogue, were it not ex- pressed in a rude drawing by Varolius, and had not the work of Vieussens remained, it would be difficult to say wherefore, in a sort of discredit, which it by no means deserves.” In reply to this we cite the following passages, from the works of the two authors mentioned above *. “ The generality of anatomists,” says Varolius, “ think the spinal marrow begins at the occipital hole only ; I can bring proof to the contrary. It arises on the one hand below the ventricles of the brain, and on the other from the middle and inferior part of the cerebral basis. In the same manner as the brain, from out its substance, first produces that considerable trunk, the spinal marrow, from which the ocular nerves soon arise ; in the same manner the cerebellum pushes from itself a considerable process, which * Constantii Varolii Anatomicse, sive de Resolutione Corporis Humani Libri Quatuor. Frankforti, 1591 ; and, Vieus-* senius, Neurographia Universalis. H 98 I call bridge of the cerebellum, out of which the auditory nerves then issue “ That the sense of touch may inhere in all parts, and that all parts may convey images of objects, cognized by touch, to the primary sensorium, there are four roots issuing from the brain and cerebellum to form a considerable trunk, the spinal marrow, from which nerves are sent off and distributed to every part of the body f.” “ For my part, seeing that there were several cerebral organs situated about the base of the head, and that the brain by its weight (especially in the dead) compressed these against the skull, I deemed the ordinary mode of dissection liable to many in- conveniences. This is the reason why I am in the habit of commencing the dissection at the opposite part of the head ; that is to say, at the basis of the brain, and by so doing, each of its organs is so clearly exhibited, that it seems as if nothing further could be desired. This method, however, which differs from the usual one, is also very difficulty.” Let any one read Varo- lius’s letter to Mercurialis, and he will be con- vinced, from his entire description, that he con- fined himself to the various forms and appear- ances visible in the brain and cerebellum ; that he did no more than attack the brain mechani- cally, turning and returning it without order or * Varolius, p. 26. f Ibid. p. 36. + Ibid. p. 140. 99 method, going from the cerebellum to the optic and olfactory nerves, and from the optic and ol- factory nerves coming back to the cerebellum. Let me add, that Varolius himself says, that the mass he calls spinal marrow is comprised be- tween the annular protuberance and the cere- bral hemispheres ; and further, that he conti- nually speaks of the spinal cord as a production of the brain. What the Committee of the French Institute make Varolius say, consequently, is not to be found in his works. Let us now turn to and review the method of dissection practised by Vieussens, which, ac- cording to the Committee of the French Insti- tute, is “ the same that Varolius employed, but with greater order a?id details In his first thirteen plates he shews nothing but sections of the brain from above downwards. He begins his demonstration with the convex part of the hemispheres, and then passes to the corpus callosum. The better to expose this, he cuts off the entire superimposed hemispheres by a horizontal sweep, and then by properly trimming the mass that is left, he forms, what he calls, the centrum orale, in which he concentrates all the medullary fibres that, ac- cording to him, arise from the cortical sub- stance of the brain, and from which he makes nervous fibres descend to every part of the body. He then passes on to the transparent partition (valvula Vieussenii), that has since gone by his H 2 100 name, to the fornix, to the choroid plexus, to the nates, to the testes, &c. &c. “ We have,” he says, in one place expressly, “ explained in a clear and complete manner, all that concerns the superior part of the brain and medulla oblongata; we have also examined the cerebellum externally and internally; we have only then, following the order of dissection which we have adopted, to examine what is found without and within the basis of the brain, properly so called, and the spinal marrow.” “ After the exact explanation,” he proceeds, “ of all that is to be seen on the upper part of the brain and spinal marrow, or that belongs to the cerebellum, in order to find with ease, and to describe with care that which is found at the basis of the brain, properly so called, and of the spinal marrow, we take away the cerebellum by cutting its peduncles transversely, and turn over the brain, freed from its convex parts by partial sections, and then Ave show the divided trunks of the anterior arteries of the base of the brain — the ten pairs of nerves — the infundibulum — the two white prominences situated near the infun- dibulum — the two processes of the cerebellum towards the medulla oblongata, which run into the major annular protuberance of Willis — the pyramidal bodies — the olivary bodies — and the spinal nerves which join the par vagum.” It was always from his oval centre that Yieus- 101 sens began his sections and descriptions of the brain. His principles, indeed, did not permit him to follow any other method of demonstration. In our answer to the Committee of the Institute, Dr. Gall and I have gone more deeply into de- tails ; but the passages cited above will suffice to shew that the methods of Varolius and Vieussens are directly opposed to our manner of dissecting and considering the brain and its parts. Vieussens, in deriving all the nervous fibres from his oval centre, proves himself to have had no idea of the successive reinforcement of the cerebral masses. I may, indeed, say generally, that an examination of all the anatomical works published before our time, and an inquiry into the various modes in which the brain has been dissected, whether in public or in private schools, will not fail to con- vince every candid mind that there is not even a hint at the anatomico-phvsiological views which we have given to the world. These views some modern anatomists have adopted, but we still ad- vance our claim of right to be considered as the discoverers and introducers of a new method of dissecting the brain — as the first demonstrators of the anatomy of its masses in harmony with their physiology. What, then, is our mode of investigating the structure of the various cerebral masses? I have already shewn (in the preceding section), that we consider the nerves commonly entitled cerebral. 102 as independent of each other, and that we regard the masses of the cerebellum, and brain, properly so called, as added to the nerves of the five senses and of voluntary motion. This point of doc- trine established, we view the brain not as an unit or single organ, but as an assemblage of particular apparatuses destined to special and determinate functions, after the manner of the nerves of the external senses. To this it may be said, that several anatomists have spoken of many peculiar parts, that they have even designated these by appropriate names, consequently, that our ideas on the plurality of apparatus are not new. There is no doubt whatever, but that all anato- mists have recognised distinct parts in the brain, and given them names according to their physical qualities. They have found hemispheres, convolu- tions, cavities, striated bodies, pea-shaped bodies, stalks or legs of the brain and cerebellum, writing pens, rams’-horns, semicircular tape-worms, pyra- midal and olive-like bodies, &c., &c. Now we, in shewing that the individual masses, so named, do by no means constitute special apparatuses, performing peculiar functions, differ from all the anatomists who have gone before us. We were, also, the first to prove the relative proportions that exist between several of the cerebral masses, and to examine them in their mutual relations. If I continue to make use of the mechanical nomen- 103 clature, to speak of parts in particular, which can no longer be considered as special apparatuses, it is only for the sake of being more readily un- derstood. My connected description will shew what masses I look upon as peculiar organs. Our physiological views do not, it must be evident, allow us to go on cutting the brain into slices ; this procedure, indeed, ought rather to be entitled a destruction, than an anatomical demon- stration of the cerebral structure ; it is precisely as though one should pretend to dissect a leg or an arm, by slicing down these members transversely, or to shew the structure of the thoracic and abdo- minal viscera, by treating the trunk in a similar manner, and giving names to the appearances exposed after each successive slice. We commence our dissection at the place where the proper cere- bral masses are added to the nervous parts already described; we trace them in their continuations, and in their connexions mutually, and with the nerves of the five senses and of voluntary motion; in short, we proceed in the dissection of the brain in a manner precisely analogous to that which is followed in the anatomical demonstration of the other parts of the body. Besides the above general anatomical principle as regards procedure, it is important to know that on account of their extremely delicate organiza- tion, the structure of several cerebral parts may 104 be more easily and clearly exposed by means of scraping than by cutting. This is the reason why I frequently prefer the handle to the blade of the scalpel for removing parts that cover those whose course I would shew, — for instance, the passage of the pyramidal bodies across the annular protuberance — the continuation of the anterior commissure through the striated bodies into the middle lobes of the brain, and of the anterior pillars of the fornix, onwards to the mammillary bodies and interior of the thalami. The brain should be removed from the cranium, care being taken not to tear the crura at the superior edge of the annular protuberance, (an accident that is very apt to occur,) nor to injure the medulla oblongata at the lower edge of the same part, and to cut the spinal mass so low down as to obtain, besides the entire medulla oblongata, the upper part of the true spinal cord. The brain thus freed from the skull, is then to be put into a plate, with the basis uppermost. The cerebellum and medulla oblongata having lost the support of the bone, now fall backwards. (PI. vii. fig. 1.) In this position, all the appear- ances presented by the base of the brain are visible. Having considered the cranial nerves in the manner described in the preceding section, the structure of the true cerebral masses is to be examined, commencing with that of the cerebel- 105 lum. As I treat of the several parts, I shall always indicate the procedure that appears to me the most convenient for exhibiting their ana- tomy *. * The method of dissecting the brain which M. Laurencet of Lyons proposes, seems founded on imaginary notions, rather than on the observance of nature and fact. According to him, the nervous system is like two trees reversed, the branches of the one being continuous with the roots of the other, or after the manner of the sanguiferous system. The spinal cord, he says, consists of four bundles, which, in the medulla oblongata, are the anterior and posterior pyramids. The anterior, after their decussation, he supposes to continue across the pons Varolii, the crura cerebri and corpora striata, towards the corpus callo- sum and the convolutions, from thence to the fornix, to the thalami, corpora quadrigemina, cerebellum and posterior pyra- mids. The number of nervous fibres is assumed to be every- where the same ; there are only bulgings and contractions in succession. M. Laurencet cuts the parts, and then tells us how they are formed. 106 Section V. Of the Cerebellum. To avoid all risk of confusion, I repeat once more that I separate the nervous mass of the spine and the cranial nerves from the brain, and that I confine this last appellation to the entire nervous mass, added to the nerves of the external senses and of voluntary motion. I also repeat that the first anatomical principle of the nervous sys- tem, generally, applies to the brain in particular, that is to say, this mass is not a simple unit, but a collection of many peculiar instruments. As this proposition is of great importance, I shall examine it here at some length. It is to be established by anatomy, physiology, and pathology. The phy- siological and pathological proofs of its truth, are contained in the second section of my Work on Phrenology, wherein I treat of the plurality of the organs. In this place, consequently, I shall confine myself to illustrative anatomical considerations. That the cerebral parts are more or less nume- rous in different tribes of animals, is a fact which cannot be gainsaid. Many writers, among others, Dr. Gall*, say that the faculties of animals are * Anat. et Phys. du Cerveau, t. iii. p. 364. 107 multiplied in proportion as their brains are complicated. Were this remark universally correct, it would serve as a positive proof of the brain’s being an assemblage of organs. But without reckoning the difficulty, not to say the impossibility of deter- mining, anatomically, even in birds and mammi- ferous animals, the constituent parts of the brain, and admitting that as true which mechanical ana- tomy demonstrates, viz . : — that the brain is made up of a greater or smaller number of bundles, it must still be observed that each particular bundle cannot, legitimately, be assumed as composing a peculiar organ. There are several cerebral masses which, although more or less compound, do not, therefore, cease to be mere units. Take the cere- bellum as an example. This is extremely simple in fishes, and very complicated in man, neverthe- less it is but a single instrument in both. The same law applies to several other cerebral parts, which, although exceedingly complex, only compose the instrument of a single function. Thus the first fact showing the structure to be more or less complicated, is no satisfactory or conclusive evi- dence as to the plurality of the cerebral organs — this induction is still only problematical. Dr. Gall derives another anatomical proof of the principle under discussion, from the analogy that subsists between the organization of the brain and that of the other nervous systems. This analogy, however, is very limited. The 108 spinal cord affords no example of it. Although composed of many parts, or numerous pairs of nerves, its functions are but repetitions of two of different kinds: viz . , sensation and motion. But the particular organs of the brain must be as distinct as the acoustic, optic, and olfactory nerves. A better anatomical illustration follows from the fact of all parts of the brain not being de- veloped simultaneously, and of their volumes severally bearing no regular proportion to each other. The size of the cerebellum, for instance, is not in any direct ratio to that of the brain, neither are the three lobes of the cerebral he- mispheres proportionate to each other. The same law applies in regard to all the individual parts of these lobes. These proofs, founded on the non-simultaneous development and non-proportionate volume of the individual portions of the encephalon, are strengthened by facts, which shew that the cere- bral parts may severally be wanting. M. Jade- lot was so kind as to shew Dr. Gall and me an hydrocephalic child, in the Hopital des Enfants Malades, at Paris, many of the superior convo- lutions of both hemispheres of whose brain were wanting, so that there was a hole communicating on each side with the lateral ventricles. The edges of these holes were smooth, and all the appearances bespoke a congenate or primary de- 109 feet of organization. The brain, of which a drawing is given in pi. v. fig. 5 and 6, belonged to a girl, who died, aged seventeen, and was idiotic from birth. She died in the asylum at Cork. Dr. Abell, of that town, and Dr. Cheyne, of Dublin, had the goodness to send me the na- tural skull, and casts, in plaster, of the brain and bust. A comparison of this brain with one of a healthy and well-constituted individual (pi. vi. fig. 1 and 2), will show its anterior lobes to be exceedingly deficient, and the convolutions that commonly exist in the upper region of the forehead to be wanting altogether. It is even less complicated, and more poorly developed, especially anteriorly, than the brain of the ourang- outang. (PI. v. fig. 8 and 4.) Mr. Stanley, of London, preserves a similar idiotic brain in spirits. The anatomical evidence, from the want of pro- portion among, and the non-simultaneous develop- ment and entire absence of, the cerebral parts, although plausible, is not, however, decisive in proving the plurality of the organs. The tes- timony these facts supply only becomes conclu- sive, when they are united to physiology. Alone, they do not prove that the functions of the parts, whose development is neither simulta- neous nor proportionate, and Avhich, individually, may be wanting, are dissimilar. — The branches of a tree shoot in succession, yet all bear the 110 same [fruit. But those cerebral parts are indu- bitably the same, however dissimilar in physical appearance, if like functions appear with their presence, increase with their growth, are vigorous in proportion to their masses, and wanting with their absence. Cerebral parts, on the other hand, differ if like functions do not appear with their development, be not manifested in vigour corresponding to their volumes, and exist or not independently of their presence or absence. Certain it is, therefore, that liow important so- ever it may be to classify the cerebral organs, anatomy alone would never enable us to attain such a consummation. The aid of physiology is indispensably requisite. Now Dr. Gall and I claim the merit of having been the first to com- pare the relations between the development of different cerebral parts and peculiar functions ; and our physiological anatomy of the brain proves, that the parts indicated in books of descriptive anatomy as distinct masses, such, for instance, as those styled pons or bridge, pyrami- dal and olive-like bodies, thalami, mammillary bodies, callous or hard body, &c. & c., do not constitute particular organs. Some anatomists have expressed doubts as to the possibility of proving the presence or ab- sence of individual parts in the human brain, especially in the hemispheres and their convolu- tions ; because, say they, the physical appear- Ill ance of these is not invariable. But, provided essentials be not confounded with modifications, the very reverse of the above assumption may easily be proved. The lobes are always to be distinguished from one another, and certain con- volutions from others, with the same certainty as the annular protuberance is to be discrimi- nated from the crura of the brain, the quadrige- minal from the mammillary, and the pyramidal from the restiform or olive-like bodies. The ge- neral form and direction of the convolutions, even of the human brain in its complication, are, in fact, remarkably regular. Thus, the transverse convolutions of the superior lateral and middle parts of the hemispheres are never found run- ning in any other direction, never longitudinally for example. Those that lie longitudinally again, as they do under the squamous suture, behind the temporal bone, and on either side of the ol- factory nerve, are never met with disposed trans- versely. “ Shew Gall,” says Dr. Rudolphi *, “ the or- gans of theft, of murder, and of the religious sentiment separated from the cerebral mass, and be sure he would not know them.” Dr. Gallf, in his reply to this, contents himself with say- ing,