GfdrttcU Hniocraitjj ffiihrarg 3tt)aca, Ntm ^arh BOUGHT WITH THE INCOME OF THE SAGE ENDOWMENT FUND THE GIFT OF HENRY W. SAGE 1691 Cornell University Library QP 915 .A3G78 Alcohol: its action on the human organis 3 1924 024 830 212 Cornell University Library The original of tliis book is in tlie Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924024830212 ALCOHOL : ITS ACTION ON THE HUMAN ORGANISM. LONDON: H.M. STATIONERY OFFICE. igia. "IN Noveiii?jer, 1916, the Central Control Board (Liquor TrafiRc), appointed an Advisory Committee "to consider the conditions affecting the physiological action of alcohol, and more particularly the effects on health and industrial eflSciency produced by the Consumption of beverages of various alcoholic strengths, with special reference to the recent Orders of the Central Control Board, and further to plan out and direct such investigations as may appear desirable with a view to obtaining more exact data on this and cognate questions." The Members of the Committee were as follows : — Lord D'Abernon, G.C.M.G. (Chairman). Chairman of the Central Control Board (Liquor Traffic). Sir George Newman, K.C.B., M.D. (Vice-Chairman). Principal Medical Officer to the Board of Education. Member of the Central Control Board (Liquor Traffic). Professor A. R. Cushny, M.D., F.R.S., Professor of Pharmacology at University College, London. H. H. Dai- the use of an appropriate engine ; in the language of Mechanics, part of the " energy," which is liberated when a substance is burned, can be obtained in the form of mechanical work, though with even the most perfect engine a large part is dissipated in the form of heat. The body, just as truly as a steam- engine, obtains the energ>- for carrying out the work performed by its different organs by the combustion or oxidation of different materials ; like the engine, too, the bodj^ obtains the liberated energj- parth- in the form of mechanical work — the work performed by the muscles in moving the body or lifting weights, and by the heart in driving the blood in circulation — but parth- as heat, which maintains the normal warmth of the body. Now all the substances which we recognise as foods can thus undergo combustion in the body, and supply the heat and the power of doing work which distinguish the living from the dead body. Water and salts on the other hand, while essential to life, only supply the conditions necessary for the use of other substances by the body ; but they are not themselves consumed in the process, cannot themselves furnish the energy which the body requires, and therein differ from the foods. Accessory substances. The same ma}' be said of the so-called " acce.ssoiy substances," the importsfnce of which has been made clear during recent 3ears. These are substances of which the nature is still obscure, which must be present in minute quantities in the diet if normal growth and the health>- life of the body are to be maintained, but are not themselves a source of the energy which the bod>' needs. Food as fuel : the several classes of foods. We have advanced a step further, therefore, in our definition of a food. A food is not merely soinethifig which is useful for the normal life of the body ; it is a fuel which the body can use as a source of energy. Leaving the case of alcohol out of account for the present, we find that all the important constituents of our diet, which are capable of acting thus as true foods or fuels to the body, fall into three classes : — 1. The carbohydrates, including starches, gums and sugars. 2. The fats. 3. The proteins, or albuminous substances. If we .make a chemical examination of the body, we find that it contains substances belonging to all these three classes, though not necessarily in any case the same individual substances as the food contains. By the processes which we call "digestion" the body breaks down all the food-materials, when necessary, into simpler, soluble substances. These are readily absorbed, and from them the body builds up again its own carbohydrates, fats and proteins. Special position of the proteins. The proteins are in a special position. They form an essential constituent of every part of the body which is living. The element nitrogen enters into their compo- sition, but not into that of fats or carbohydrates. The body, therefore, cannot form protein from fats or carbohydrates ; to repair the waste of its tissues it must have protein in the food. So that a certain quantity of protein is, in a peculiar sense, an indispensable constituent of a diet. Carbohydrates and fats are not indispensable in the same sense. If the food contains more protein than is required for the repair of tissue, the body can form carbohydrate or fat from the remainder. But when carbohydrates and fats are given as well, as in an ordinary diet, the body requires very much less protein than if it had to be maintained on protein alone. Excess of carbo- hydrate can be stored in the body as a special carbohydrate called " glycogen," a sort of animal starch ; or it can be converted into fat and stored in that form. Excess of fat in the diet can similarly be stored as body-fat. The well-nourished body, therefore, always contains a good reserve of these food-substances or fuels. If no food is taken for a prolonged period, but only water, the body draws on its own reserves of fuel to maintain the supply of energy. It uses first the fat and carbohydrate, which are stored for this purpose, and when these are exhausted consumes the proteins of the less important tissues, such as the muscles of the limbs and trunk, to maintain the supply of energy for those which are more essential for the continuance of life. Ordinary food-stuffs used for tissue repair^ or can be stored in the body. We have, then, another property of all these three classes of food-substances. They can not only serve as fuel for the body's immediate requirements of energy, but can furnish material for the repair of the natural waste •jf the living tissues, or for replenishment of the fuel- reserve which the body normally maintains. The body not only can use them, but can store them for future need. Are we to regard this also as an essential character of a "food," and refuse to apply that term to substances which can be used as immediate fuel, but cannot be changed by the body into glycogen or fat for storage ? The question is a very important one for our present purpose, because on the answer to it depends our con- clusion as to whether alcohol is a " food " or not. On II the whole it has appeared to us more logical to admit that such immediate fuel-substances are " foods " also ; for although they cannot themselves be stored, or changed into something which can, by meeting the immediate demand of the bodj' for fuel, they prevent the deple- tion of the reserve. Their addition to a diet will, in the same way, allow the body to use for replenishment of its fuel-reserve carbohydrates and fats which it would otherwise have to use immediately. So that such sub- stances have, in fact, the same effect in a diet as an additional supply of carbohydrates and fats ; they have, to a limited extent, the same effect as substances which are undoubtedly foods, though they do not produce it in exactly the same way. Physiological classification of food-substances] We shall, therefore, adhere to our simple definition, that a food is a fuel ivhich the body can use as a source of energy. But we must further recognise that the question is not a simple one : that there are degrees in the scale of importance applicable to food substances, of which we can distinguish three. Class I. Substances which can serve as fuel, to supply the energy required for the life of the body, and which can also supply the material required for the repair of the living structure. Theoretically the body could supply all its needs from such substances, together with water, salts, and the requisite traces of the " accessory sub- stances" mentioned above (p. 8). To this first class belong the proteins alone. Class 2. Substances which can serve as fuel directly, and can also be used to replenish the fuel-reserve of the body. To this class belong the carbohydrates and fats. Class 3. Substances which can serve as fuel, but are only available for immediate use. They cannot be used to form new materials for the body's fuel-reserve ; but 12 they can help to maintain that reserve bj- reducing the need for drafts upon it, and by allowing substances of Class 2 to be used for its replenishment. Instead of asking the bare question, " Is alcohol a food " ? we shall obtain information of much greater importance and interest by asking, " Does alcohol belong to any of the above classes of food-stuflfs, and if so, to which " ? Alcohol a fuel food which is only available for immediate use. We shall find that the evidence leads to the conclusion that alcohol is a food of Class 3 ; indeed, it is the only important constituent of an ordinary dietary belonging to that class. We must make the position quite clear. If any- one chooses to maintain that the word " food " should be restricted to substances included above in Classes i and 2, he is perfectly entitled to do so. He would then be justified in stating that alcohol is not a food, provided that he made perfectly clear the limited sense in which he was using the word. We have chosen, for reasons stated, to allow the word a more extended application. We shall review the evidence which shows that alcohol is a food in this wider sense, and indicates the class of food-stufFs to which it belongs, and the limits imposed on its value as a food by its other properties. No one has the right to quote us as making the state- ment that alcohol is a food, unless he defines, as we do, the exact sense in which the word is used, and the limits within which the statement is true. Definition of metabolism. In discussing the food-value of alcohol we shall frequently have occasion to use the term " Meta- bolism," and it will be convenient to define its meaning now. The word metabolism is used by the physio- logist to denote the sum-total of the chemical changes that take place in the bodJ^ The engineer who wishes- to measure the efficiencj' of his engine, keeps careful record of the fuel consumed, the mechanical work done, and the amount of energy lost as heat. Similarly the physiologist conducts on the living man or animal what is called a " metabolism experiment," though the measurements and analyses required are far more numerous and elaborate than those which are needed for the relatively simple mechanism of the steam-engine or the petrol-motor. A man, or some other animal, is placed in an observation chamber. Everything that he eats or drinks, and the air that he breathes are measured and analysed. All the products of metabolism, coming out in the breath, through the kidneys or by the bowels, are measured and analysed with the same care. The amount of work performed and of heat lost are recorded, as well as the weight of the subject at the beginning and end of the observation. From all these elaborate data informa- tion is obtained as to the mode of working of the body, regarded as a machine, and a study can be made of the effect of adding a particular substance, such as alcohol, to the food. We shall see that by such means evidence has been obtained which enables us to reply to the above questions whether alcohol is a food, and to which class of food-substances it belongs. Definition of drug action. Apart from serving as food-materials, in the manner already indicated, substances can influence metabolism in other ways. There are substances which, when they are taken into the body, can accelerate, retard, or otherwise modify the delicate action of various parts of its mechanism, independently of any contribtition to the supply of energy. Such action we propose in this statement to term "drug-action," and to refer to such substances as " drugs." 14 IV/mt is meant bv the term ^^ drug.'' The word "drug" is used in different senses by different classes of people. In popular phraseology it often means a substance of a deleterious nature. As we use it here, it should not be taken to imply a necessarily harmful action ; we use it simply to mean a substance which tem- porarily modifies the activity of the bodily organs, includ- ing, of course, the brain and nervous system, otherwise than by increasing the supply of available energy. As an example of a pure drug-action, in this sense, we may take that of nitrous oxide — the anaesthetic gas used by dentists. When this gas is breathed the patient rapidly loses consciousness, the rate of his breathing and of his heart-beat, the activitj^ of his muscles, are all conspicuously modified. When admin- istration of the gas is stopped, he rapidly rids himself through his breath of all the nitrous oxide which he had absorbed, and his brain and organs return to their normal state of activity. The nitrous oxide has pro- foundly modified the bodily functions without being used up or in any way entering into the chemical changes which we call metabolism. The effect of another familiar anaesthetic, ether, differs from this, not only in the fact that it is more prolonged, but because a small proportion of the ether taken into the body is burned ; so that ether to a very small extent enters into metabolism, and to that extent its action is that of a. food, in the sense mentioned above. Nobody would think of describing ether as a food, or of suggesting it as a constituent of a diet ; its food- value is trivial, and its drug-action predominant. When we come to consider alcohol, the case is different ; but the difference is only one of degree. Alcohol is a common article of diet, and its food-value has been the subject of much controversy. But quite apart from any conclusion we reach on that point, as the result of our examination of the evidence, we have to consider its drug-action. We shall give reason for deeming this the most important of the actions of alcohol, as taken by the majority of those who use it. Importance of drug action of alcohol. For, though alcohol does, in the sense already indicated, act as a food, it is seldom taken primarily for that reason. It is to its drug properties and to the agreeable taste of the fermented liquors that we must look for the origin of the habit of using the drinks containing alcohol and of their position in popular esteem. A large section Of our statement will be concerned, therefore, with the details of the action of alcohol as a drug. We shall discuss to what extent this action is useful to the healthy and to the sick, and to what extent it is harmful. It will be clear also that a final estimate of the value of alcohol as a food must involve consideration of the extent to which its use is limited by the fact of its drug-action. Definition of poison action. Practicalli' every drug when used in excessive quan- tities, or under exceptional conditions, becomes a poison ; that is to say, it so greatly interferes with the normal working of the bodily functions that it en- dangers life. When life is endangered or terminated bj' a single large dose of a drug, we speak of the effect as " acute poisoning," and we shall have to discuss the phenomena of acute poisoning which a single large dose of alcohol will produce. On the other hand there are many drugs which, when taken habitually for long periods, produce structural and other changes of important organs, which ultimately lead to permanent ill-health, and may be the cause of death. We call such a condition " chronic poisoning." It should be noted that the chronic effects may have no relation whatever to the symptoms of what we have called the " drug-action," or i6 of acute poisoning by the same substances. Each in- dividual dose, from the succession of which the chronic poisoning results, may be so small as to produce but slight symptoms of drug-action, and those of an agreeable and apparently favourable nature. Short of dangerous illness or death, there are minor defects and infirmities, affecting perhaps only some organ or system which is not essential to the life of the individual, which may result from chronic poisoning. The body may, through the prolonged action of a poison, acquire a weakness, a lessened resistance to disease or injury, which is imperceptible in health, and only becomes manifest when the subject is exposed to infection or violence. We shall have to discuss the possibility of the body being affected in any of these ways by the habitual use of alcohol. 17 CHAPTER II. Alcohoi, as a Food. Absorption of alcohol. In considering the value of any substance as a food, it is necessary to know in the first place, to what extent, and with what rapidity it is absorbed from the stomach and intestine ; for it is not until the substance has passed into the blood, by the circulation of which it is distributed to the tissues which can make use of it, that the true action as a food begins. However easily combustible a substance may be out- side the body, unless it is soluble in the blood, or can •be changed by digestion into something which is solu- able, it cannot act as a food. A substance like paraffin, for example, although it burns readily in air, is completely insoluble in the fluids of the body, and when it is taken into the stornach, it passes unchanged through the intestine and does not act as a food at all. Alcohol, on the other hand, is completely soluble in water, with which it mixes perfectly in all proportions, so that it does not need to be rendered soluble by digestion. We should expect it, therefore, to be absorbed quickly and completely ; experiment has shewn that this is the case. Analyses of the contents of the stomach and intestines, made at different intervals after a dose of alcohol has been swallowed, sl^ew that about one-fifth of it is absorbed from the stomach. In the uppermost section of the small intestine one tenth is absorbed, but the main absorption, accounting for one half of the quantity taken, occurs in the middle part of the small intes- tine, the remaining one-fifth being absorbed in the third and last section. By the time the alimentary contents i8 reach the large intestine, all the alcohol has been taken up from them into the blood. The absorption of alcohol is, therefore, complete, and, in comparison with that of food-stuffs needing pre- liminary digestion, such as meat, it is conspicuously rapid. The actual speed of absorption seems to vary with a number of conditions, such as the form in which the alcohol is taken, the extent to which it is diluted, and the time in relation to meals ; but the interaction of the different factors is evidently very complicated, and there is no evidence so clear as to warrant a definite statement concerning the effect of an}- of these factors. Excretion. The second point on which information is required, in estimating the food-value of a substance, is the extent to which the body can use it after it has passed into the circulation. We can obtain an indication of this by determining to what extent it leaves the body again un- changed. The processes by which the body discharges materials which it does not use further are collectively termed " excretion " by physiologists. Mention has been made of the case of sugar, an}' ordinary quantit}' of which is burned completelj' in the body, so that, except in some diseased conditions such as diabetes, no sugar can be found amongst the waste materials in the uriue. Sugar, therefore, is used by the body to its full value. We may contrast with this the case of saccharin, a complex coal-tar derivative, which is sometimes used in place of sugar, on account of its intensely sweet taste. Saccharin, in ordinary quantities, is absorbed completely from the stomach and bowel, but the whole amount taken can be recovered unchanged in the urine ; so that saccharin is not a food at all, and its whole value is in its sweetness. Alcohol, being a substance which readily evaporates. 19 can leave the bod\- to some extent in the breath,^ as well as in the urine, and both must be analysed in order to determine the proportion of a dose of alcohol which is excreted.^ In all the experiments of this kind which have been made this proportion has been found to be a small one, though it is not constant. It is increased by anj^ influence which quickens the absorption of alcohol from the stomach and intestine, or the formation of urine by the kidneys, or which augments the vigour of the respiration. But under the conditions most favourable to this loss of unchanged alcohol in the breath and urine, it never amounts to more than one-tenth of the quantity swallowed. Usually much less is lost in this manner — on an average about one-fiftieth ; and in people who, be- taking alcohol habitually, have acquired the power of using it up more rapidly than those unaccustomed to it, as little as one-hundredth part of a moderate dose of alcohol may escape in the breath and the urine. The quantit}' of alcohol excreted as such by the body is always small, therefore, and may be trivial. What ' It should be noted tljat the odour of the breath, associated with the taking of alcoholic liquors, is mostly due to other substances than alcohol, which has but a faint odour. ^ There is no evidence that alcohol is ever eliminated in appre- ciable amount in the sweat. Traces of the drug have been found in the milk, in experiments in which large doses of alcohol were given to cows and goats ; and some observers have also detected its presence in the milk of women of alcoholic habits. It is probable, however, that this occurs only when large quantities of alcohol are taken, and that even then, the amount excreted is too small to have any influence on the health of the infant. In this connection it may further be mentioned that the available evidence gives no support to the popular belief that alcoholic beverages, and particularly malted liquors, promote the production of milk, and are therefore to be recommended to nursing mothers. Whether in the form of spirits or of beer, alcohol does not seem to have any effect whatever on either the quantity or the quality of this secretion. (R. Rosemann, in Pfliiger's Archiv, Vol. 78, 18S9). 16066 c 20 "becomes of the remainder ? This question must be answered before any conclusion can be drawn as to the food-value of alcohol. How the body uses alcohol. Though little of the alcohol escapes as such in the urine and breath, the residue does not remain long in the body. In from fifteen to twenty-four hours after a dose of alcohol has been taken, the whole of it has disappeared completely. The destruction of the alcohol, indeed, begins as soon as it reaches the blood. Samples of blood can be taken from a man or other animal at intervals after a dose has been swallowed, and the proportion of alcohol in each sample can be determined by analysis. In this way it has been found that for the first few hours the alcohol is passing into the blood more rapidly than it is destroyed, so that the concentration in successive samples rises. After an interval, varying with the dose among other conditions, the concentration reaches a maximum, and usually remains at about that level for some time. This maintenance of a level concentration does not mean that the absorption has been completed, but that it has been ■overtaken by the destruction, and that, for the time being, the two processes practically balance one another. Then the rate at which alcohol disappears becomes greater than that at which it is entering the circulation, so that the concentration in the blood declines, until after about 24 hours no more alcohol can be discovered anywhere in the body. The maximum concentration which alcohol reaches in the blood bears a regular relation to the original dose : thus, it was shewn by Grehant' that when an animal had taken a dose of alcohol equivalent to one - thousandth of its body weight {i.e. a dose of I cubic centimetre per kilogram), the highest pro- ' Gr^hant. — Comptes rendus de la Soci^td de Biologic, 1881, .1896, and 1899. 21 portion subsequentlj- found in the blood was about I per thotisand ; with a dose of 2 cc. per kilogram the blood contained about 2 per thousand, and so on. This would, at first sight, appear to mean an equal dis- tribution of the alcohol throughout the body. It does not mean this, however ; for when the maximum concen- tration is attained in the blood, some part of the dose has already been destroyed, while some of it is still unabsorbed. 1 It so happens that the balance of absorption over destruc- tion brings the maximum concentration in the blood to the value which would be obtained by an immediate dis- tribution of the whole dose through the whole of the tissues of the body; but the correspondence is accidental. Products of combustion of alcohol. What has become of the alcohol ? Only a little of it can be found passing out of the body ; and yet, after 24 hours none can be found in the body. There being no evidence of its change into some other substance which the body can retain, the supposition is natural that it is completely " oxidised " or burnt, producing, as when burnt in air, carbon dioxide and water, which pass out in the breath and urine. The method by which this supposition can be put to the test of direct experiment, must now be described. All food- substances contain carbon and hydrogen, and therefore yield carbon dioxide and water when they are oxidised in the body. The carbon dioxide leaves the body very quickh' in the breath, the rate and depth of breathing being normally so adjusted as to keep the amount of it in. the blood nearly constant (see p. 68). The quantity of carbon dioxide and water formed, and, therefore, the amount of carbon dioxide passing out in the breath, varies from minute to minute, with changes in the activity of the muscles and other organs. A mere measurement of the rate at which carbon dioxide is turned out in the breath, after alcohol has been taken. will not give much information as to the extent to which alcohol is oxidised. For, even if variations due to the activity of the muscles or other organs were excluded, an increase in the production of carbon dioxide, following a dose of alcohol, would not necessarily mean that the alcohol was being burnt. It might mean merely that the taking of alcohol caused the body to burn more fat, carbohydrate or protein. Another measurement is needed as well, namel>', the rate at which oxygen is used up in the air which is breathed. The Respiratory Quotient. When a carbohydrate, such as sugar, is burnt in the air, the volume of oxygen which disappears from the air is exactlj' the same as the volume of carbon dioxide which is formed. This is not true of any other food-substances ; when fats or proteins are burnt, the volume of oxygen used is always larger than that of the carbon dioxide which is produced. And there is a difference of the same kind, but greater, when alcohol is burnt. Readers with a slight knowledge of chemistry will readily understand the reason of this difference. Carbo- hydrates, fats and alcohol are all composed of carbon, hydrogen and oxygen. The amount of oxj^gen present in a carbohydrate is just sufficient to combine with all the hydrogen to form water, so that when a carbohydrate is burnt onl\- sufficient extra oxygen is required to com- bine with the carbon ; and when oxygen combines with carbon, the \olume of carbon dioxide formed is equal to that of the oxygen used. Substances like fat contain a smaller proportion, of oxygen, insufficient to combine with all their hydrogen, so that, when they are burnt, they use up a volume of oxygen which is larger than that of the carbon dioxide which is formed. The differ- ence is still larger in the case of alcohol, which contains a smaller proportion of oxygen than the fats. 23 The same difference between carbohydrates aud other food-substances appears when they are used in the body. By analysing the air which is breathed in and that which is breathed out by a man, or other animal, over a long period, the amount of oxygen used and the amount of carbon dioxide formed in the body are determined. These are compared by making a fraction : — Volume of carbon dioxide formed Volume of oxygen used, and this is called the " respiratory quotient." When the two volumes are nearly equal, so that the value of the fraction approaches unity, we know that the body is using chiefly carbohydrates. When the volume of the oxygen used is greater in proportion, so that the respira- tory quotient becomes smaller, we know that some food- substance other than carbohydrate is being burnt. The respiratory quotient resulting from the oxidation of alcohol would be lower than that corresponding to any of the ordinary food-stuffs ; so that, if alcohol is oxidised in the body, the addition of alcohol to the diet should be followed by a fall of the respiratory quotient. Degree to which alcohol is oxidised in body. A series of elaborate and careful experiments of this kind was carried out by Atwater and Benedict^ in America, and their observations have been confirmed by other workers. The results give definite proof that alcohol is oxidised in the body as completely as the carbohydrates, and rather more completely than the fats and proteins; Alcohol a fuel food. It has been suggested that the energy liberated by this oxidation of alcohol cannot be used by the body, but is lost in the form of superfluous heat. This possibility has been examined and disproved. Atwater and Benedict Physiological Aspects of the Liquor Problem, New York, 1903. 24 were able to shew that the bodj' can derive up to oue- fifth of the total energy it requires from the metabolism of alcohol. There can be no doubt, therefore, that alcohol is a " food," in the sense of a fuel that the body can use. We have seen that alcohol is not stored b}- the body. There remains to be considered the extent to which it can replace each of the ordinary food-stuffs. Hmv far alcohol can replace otdinary food-sUiffs : (i) Carbohydrates. Experiments of the kind described above have shewn that, up to a certain point, carbohydrate can be replaced by alcohol. A man was kept on a standard measured diet, rich in carbohj'drates, and his respiratory quotient was carefully determined. A ration of alcohol was then added to the diet. The respiratory quotient fell promptly, and to such an extent as to shew that, while the alcohol was being oxidised, carbohydrate was being saved from oxidation and added to the fuel reserve of the man's body. (2) Fats. All those who have made experiments on the point, agree in finding that alcohol can take the place of part of the fats in a diet. If a fixed diet, sufficient for maintenance, is given, the addition of alcohol to it protects some fat from oxidation, and allows more fat to be added to the body's reserve supply. (3) Proteins. The case of the proteins is peculiar. They contain nitrogen, and a small proportion of sulphur, in addition to carbon, hydrogen, and oxygen, and, as stated above, the animal bod)- can obtain the materials for repairing the wear and tear of its proteins, only from proteins in the food. Neither carbohydrates, nor fats, nor alcohol can replace in the food the protein needed for this purpose. But an ordinary diet contains protein much in excess of this minimum needed for repair, and this excess is oxidised to furnish energj-. The amount of protein which is thus- being burnt can be estimated by determining the sub- stances containing nitrogen which leave the body in the urine. When protein alone is given as a food, the body destroys such a large proportion of it, that a large amount of it must be given ; otherwise, the body loses more nitrogen in the urine than it receives in the food — an indication that the natural wastage of the body's own protein is not being made good. But if fat or carbo- hydrate is given in addition, the proportion of the food protein which is thus burnt is much reduced, so that a larger proportion is available for repairing the waste of the tissues, and a smaller ration of protein suffices. The question which we have now to consider is whether alcohol can also act in this way. And it will be clear that we are not discussing whether alcohol can take the place of protein, for nothing can trulj' do so ; the question is whether alcohol can act like carboh5'drates and fats in economising protein, by reducing to a minimum the amount which the body requires. The general result of experiments made to determine this- point, shews that it can. The method of experiment is to put the subject on a mixed ration, containing known amounts of protein,, carbohydrate and fat. A daily determination is made of the nitrogen lost in the urine on this standard diet. The diet is so adjusted to the needs of the subject that a reduction of the carbohydrate or fat will lead to the burning of more protein, which will be indicated by the loss of more nitrogen in the urine. But instead of merely reducing carbohydrate, the effect can be tried of sub- stituting for it a chemically equivalent amount of alcohol. For example, the original diet may contain a high pro- portion of sugar; a large part of this can be dropped,, and a corresponding ration of whisky given instead. If 26 this change does not cause the appearance of more nitrogen in the urine, we have evidence that the alcohol is acting like the sugar as an economiser of protein. Most observers who have used this method are agreed that alcohol can act like a carbohydrate in this respect also, the amount of protein burnt in the body remaining just the same after the change from carbo- hydrate to alcohol. Others have found evidence of an increased destruction of protein when the change is made, and have, therefore, maintained that alcohol has not the same power as the carbohydrates have of saving protein. There is good reason to believe, however, that this result was due to the fact that the subjects of these experiments were not accustomed to taking alcohol, and it seems to be clearly established that, in those who are used to it, alcohol in moderate quantity is as efficient as carbo- hydrates or fats in reducing the amount of protein required by the body to maintain itself, by saving the proteins from being used merely as a source of energy. Has alcohol a special action on nutntion ? Reference has already been made to the presence in an efficient diet of traces of so-called "accessory substances," which are not themselves foods, but have a profound influence on the power of the body to nourish itself on the true food-stuffs. It is necessary to discuss whether alcohol has, apart from its food-value in the sense already defined, any such specific stimulant action on nutrition. Medical opinion formerly attributed to alcohol an effect of this kind ; certain wines and beers especially were credited with special value in the " building up of the constitution," and were widely prescribed in cases of malnutrition, such as that due to tubercular infection. This view is no longer widely held by medical men, but appears still to be current as an article of popular belief. 27 There are probably many who regard abstinence from ■alcohol as incompatible with the development and pre- ■servation of a robust and vigorous bodily habit. Accurate •observations lend no support to such a belief. The food- value of alcohol, within limits, is no less than that of a ■corresponding quantity of sugar or lard ; but, on the ■other hand, there is no evidence for its ever being ■greater. If sufficient food is being taken in other forms, the addition of alcohol to the diet may lead to the accumulation of fat, but it will not improve the nutrition. When wine or beer aids recovery after a wasting disease, it does so because the food taken in ordinary forms is not adequate. In such circumstances, the alcohol is of some use in increasing the food-value of the diet ; but its main effect is probably in making the patient more comfortable and contented, and thereby improving his appetite for ordinary foods. This is not a •special action on nutrition, but a drug action, which will be considered in a subsequent chapter. Summary. It will be useful now to summarise the conclusions which seem to be justified by the evidence discussed in this chapter, and to consider their practical bearings. 1. Alcohol is completely and rapidly absorbed from the •stomach and intestine, and distributed by the circulating blood to the different organs of the body. The rate of its absorption is modified by conditions which still need further investigation. 2. A variable but always small proportion of alcohol escapes unchanged in the breath and urine. The rest •disappears completely in about 24 hours after it has been ■swallowed. 3. The concentration of alcohol in the blood rises for a few hours after it has been swallowed, remains nearly .constant for a few hours longer, and then steadily 28 declines. The duration of these phases is modified by conditions affecting either the rate of absorption or the rate of disappearance ; but the evidence as to the nature of these conditions is still very imperfect. There is, how- ever, good evidence of an increased rate of disappearance in subjects who have become habituated to taking alcohol {vide chapter on " Poison Action," p. 85). 4. The disappearance of alcohol from the bod}', apart from the small proportion escaping unchanged, is due to- its being oxidised ( burnt). None of the alcohol is known to be converted into any substance which the body can retain. 5. The energy liberated by the combustion of a mode- rate amount of alcohol can be used by the body to its full value. 6. Alcohol can, within limits, replace an equivalent amount of carbohydrate or fat in a diet, and has a similar effect in economising proteins. 7. The whole food-value of alcohol is due to its use by the body as a fuel. Food-value of alcohol qualified by its drug action. The conclusions thus summarised are concerned with the purely scientific question, whether alcohol acts as a food in the sense here defined. The question whether- a substance is a desirable constituent of a diet, whether its use is advisable, is a much wider one than this, and involves many considerations other than that of the body's ability to use it as a fuel. There are other substances which the body oxidises as completely as it does alcohol, such as citric or acetic acid (the acids- contained in lemon juice and ^•inegar respectively). Nobody would suggest that either of these substances could be taken in such quantities as to make a material contribution to the fuel required by the body. In practice they are not so taken, being used only in small 29 quantities, for their acid taste, and not for their food- value. Their other properties make it impossible to- take them in large quantities. Theoretically they are foods ; practicall)" their use as such is negligible. Similarly, in discussing the evidence for the action of alcohol as a food, it has repeatedly been necessary to use qualifying expressions such as ''within limits," "in mode- rate quantities," etc. These limits are, of course, imposed, by the other effects of alcohol, which will form the subject of the ensuing, chapters. The question, as to how far the theoretical value of alcohol as a food is in practice restricted and counterbalanced by these other properties, must be left till they have been considered. It is- desirable here, however, to emphasise the fact that, for' the normal man, alcohol has no advantage over an ordinary food-substance like sugar, from the point of view of its food-value alone. The question of its food- value cannot, therefore, be allowed much weight in the practical decision of the individual as to whether he should or should not take alcohol. This question must be decided rather by a balancing of the advantageous features of what is here termed its "drug-action" against those which are undesirable, and against the risk of the wholly bad effects included under its action as a "poison." 30 CHAPTER III. Mentai, Effects of Alcohoi,. Effect of environment on symptoms of drunkenness. The aspect of drunkenness which has most impressed, the popular mind is that of boisterous, disordeted and even violent activity of mind and body which not infrequently appears as one phase of the process, of intoxication. But this phase commonh- appears under social conditions which stimulate the emotions ; alcohol undoubtedly diminishes the control of the intellect and the will over the emotions, and it appears not improbable that this passing phase of excitement may be sufficiently accounted for by exciting influences of the environment, the jovial company, the bright lights, the unrestrained talk and song; the general sense of festivity, which are the common setting of the feast. The conclusion indicated by laboratory tests of mental and bodily capacity is borne out by simple observation of one's self or of other subjects, if one takes successive doses of alcohol in the absence of all such exciting influences. The first effect generally noticed is a slight giddiness or " light-headedness ; " this is followed by an increasing heaviness and disinclination for all effort, soon passing into sleepiness; and this in turn, if not counteracted by any excitement coming from within or without, gives place to a heavy sleep which continues for many hours. Reeling of xvetl-bcing induced by alcohol. We may notice at once that even under these condi- tions alcohol produces to some degree that efi"ect which, perhaps more than any other, is the secret of its «harm, its well-nigh universal attraction for the human 31 race, namely, a sense of careless well-being or bodily and mental comfort. In so far as this sense of well- being is of bodily origin, it is no doubt largely due to a flushing of the skin with blood that abolishes all sense of chill ; but it is due also in part to a blunting of the sensibilitj' to the small aches and pains and a thousand hardly distinguishable sense-impressions which, except in those in perfect health, contribute to tip the balance of bodily feeling-tone to the negative or unpleasant side. In so far as this effect is primarily mental, it results from the blunting of those higher mental faculties which lead us to " look before and after and pine for what is not " and harass us with care for the future and a too sensitive self-consciousness for the present. Appearance of excitement not due to any stimnlant action of alcohol. If on the other hand the drinker is subjected to the stimulation that comes from social intercourse he usually passes through a phase which may be justly called one of excitement, the degree of excitement depending upon the temperament of the drinker and the nature and degree of the external stimuli. But careful observa- tion of and reflexion upon the phenomena of this stage shew clearly that they do not require for their explana- tion the assumption that alcohol stimulates the nervous system, whether directly or through the medium of other bodily organs. The drinker's conversation and actions become less restrained ; all his emotional responses are freer and fuller than in his normal state. He laughs and smiles more readily, he grows more easily angry or tender, elated or depressed, scornful or compassionate, according to the appeal of the moment. Perhaps the various emotional states into which fear enters as an element, and which we denote by such names as anxiety, worry. -care, despondency, are exceptions to this rule. This ■group of exceptions is readily explicable on the prin- •ciple that we apply, namely, that alcohol successively weakens and suspends the hierarchy of functions of the brain, and therefore of the mind, in the order from above downwards ; that is to say in the inverse order of their development in the individual and in the race. For the emotional dispositions or capacities are a very ancient racial endowment and have their physiological seats in the basal ganglia, the lowest levels of the great brain, the part which alone is represented in the brains of the lower vertebrates. The higher intellectual faculties on the other hand are the latest acquired and are connected with the anatomically highest and last developed parts of the brain. Intermediate between these come, in the order of development, the sensory and skilled motor functions (and their nerve centres). / Blunting of self-criticism by alcohol. Now, of all the intellectual functions, that of self- criticism is the highest and latest developed, for in it are combined the functions of critical judgment and of self- consciousness, that self-knowledge which is essential to the supreme activity we call volition or the deliberative will. It is the blunting of this critical side of self- awareness by alcohol, and the consequent setting free of the emotions and their instinctive impulses from its habitual control, that give to the convivial drinker the aspect and the reality of a general excitement. In the mature well-developed mind, this interplay of thought and emotion goes on under the checking and moderating influence of self-criticism ; in social intercourse especially, it is constantly checked by the thought of the figure one cuts in the eyes of one's fellow men. In proportion, then, as alcohol hampers this mechanism of self-control, the liberation of intellectual 33 -or emotional effects goes ou at a higher rate. Normally the emotional states of anxiety, care and despondency are maintained by self-consciousness, by the repeated turning of the stream of thought to the self, its difficulties, its embarrassments, the snares and dangers that beset its course on every hand, and are far more frequently imagined and foreseen than actually en- countered. Hence, when imaginative .self - conscious- ness is dimmed, the emotions of this class are pro- portionately less liable to be touched to life, and in the -absence of their restraining influence, the other emotions run riot the more gaily. Weakening of self-control evident in every stage of diunken- ness ; most prominent feature of initial stage. Both introspectively and objectively this lack of self- control is clearly discernible in every stage of alcoholic intoxication. It is' commonly counteracted in part by the subject who becomes aware of it by means of a deliberately increased effort of self-control: but, as the influence of the alcohol increases, this effort ceases to be continuously effective, and the drinker surprises the observer (whether himself or another) by smiling or laughing aloud at some very small joke, or by remarks or other actions which betray the suspension of his habitual self-control. And the weakening of his critical self-awareness is especially revealed by the fact chat such jovial remarks as he now utters seem to him to shine with a lustre hardly perceptible to the normal mind ; hence the tendency, perhaps the most characteristic and constant feature of the first stage of drunkenness, to flippant whimsical utterances, which, like the rest of the subject's behaviour, betray the blunting of his critical self-consciousness and of his sense of personal responsi- bility. The successive stages or phases of intoxication cannot 34 be sharpl}' distinguished, and every case presents its peculiar combination and succession of features, varying with the temperament and disposition and character of the individual and his circumstances of the moment. But three main stages may be broadly distinguished corresponding to the invasion by the narcotic of the three principal levels of cerebral function mentioned above. The first stage, that in which the highest or intellectual brain level is alone distinctly affected, has been described above. Nei'votis functions involved in second stage of drimkenness. The second stage is that in which the functions of the intermediate level, sense - perception and skilled move- ment, are invaded and disturbed. The drinker begins to shew a certain clumsiness of behaviour. If he is self- observant, he notices that he is liable to make ill-adjusted movements ; on setting down his glass it makes a more violent contact with the table than he had intended ; on rising, he maj- stumble against a chair, perhaps upsetting it ; on lighting a cigarette he may break the match which he essays to strike ; in speaking he may slur a word or drop an h. Each such little mishap will at first be quickly rectified, for ^ap{,.^22^ i^^y evoke the power, possessed in some degree by all and to a wonderful degree by many men, of temporarily correcting by an effort of concentration or self-control the paralysing effects of the drug. Emotional instability in second stage of drnnkenness. At this stage also his perceptions are impaired. His field of sense-observation is narrowed ; the several senses work in relative isolation from one another ; the fineness of his ear, of his taste, his touch, his vision is blunted ; he may momentarily see objects doubled, and becomes relatively indifferent to heat and cold, to the flavour of .15 his food and the aroma of his wine, to the glare of the lights, the strains of the music, and the stridency of his own or his neighbour's voice. The impairment of his intellectual functions being further advanced than in the first stage, and the functions of th^ third or lowest cerebral level, that of the emotions and instinctive impulses, being still relatively intact, h'e is apt to give way to clumsy but violent displays of emotion characterised by the exclusive dominance of each primary emotion in turn ; and he passes quickly from anger to affection, from boisterous merriment to tears, from elated boasting to despondency, each unrestrained and unmodified by that blending of other emotions which expresses the reaction of the intellectual faculties upon them. At this stage the drinker is apt to feel that his bodilyi movements occur without his initiation or intention — they escape from him rather than proceed from his will — and, in so far as he remains self-conscious, he leads a double life; his inner self, aTTietached observer with folded hands, watches his bodily actions, not seldom with surprise, consternation or amusement ; he may, e.g: become aware of wearing facial expressions, of making gestures, or of uttering remarks, which he did not intend and cannot wholly repress, but which seem to him to be executed by his members of their own initiative. At this' stage the paralysis of the drinker's higher mental functions reveals itself clearly also in the increasing dependence of all mental and bodily activity upon external stimulation ; he lives in and for the present moment only, and, if he is deprived of the stimulus of social intercourse, he quickly lapses into dreamy somnolence or actual sleep. Third stage nj drunkenness. In the third stage, the intellectual processes of judgment and self-criticism and control are virtually suspended ; 16066 D 36 the functions of sense-perception and skilled movement are grossly impaired, and the emotional tendencies themselves are invaded and weakened, so that only strong appeals to them suffice to evoke any response and, in their absence, the drinker sinks inert and nerveless into a hea\'y sleep, which lasts until the alcohol absorbed by the nervous system has been oxidised or carried away in the blood and consumed by other tissues. Hypothesis that alcohol acts primarily on nerve-cell junc- tions^ or synapses. This succession of events constituting the normal course of alcoholic intoxication as it appears to common observation, can be explained in general terms by an hypothesis as to the direct action of alcohol on the nervous tissue, which is suggested and supported by many physiological facts and analogies ; the hypothesis, namely that alcohol acts primarily and most powerfully not upon the nerve cells or fibres, but upon the junctions between nerve-cells, technically known as synapses. It is now pretty well established that we may properly regard the nervous system as consisting of a vast number of vital units, the nerve- cells, each consisting of a central body and one or more slender threads or fibres ; each cell having no anatomical but only a functional continuity with others. Their relations to one another may be likened to those of a crowd of people, in which each person maintains relations with his fellows and communicates with them only by the touch of hands and feet. There is much evidence to shew that these points of contact are the weak points of the nervous pathways ; the points that give way most readily under strain or shock and under the influence of fatigue and of various paralysing drugs. 37 Why effects of alcohol are first shewn on higher functions. Further, there is good reason to believe that in the pathways of the lower levels of the brain, those which subserve the functions first developed in the race and in the individual, the points of junction are relatively firm and open to the passage of the nervous current; while those of higher and later developed levels are less solidly organised, and that they therefore offer more resistance to the passage of the nervous current, in proportion as they stand high in the scale of function and late in the order of development. If we accept this view, and if we make the further simple assump- tion that alcohol acts equally upon all such junctions of nerve-cells (or synapses), we have the explanation of the phenomena of drunkenness. For, by the terms of the hypothesis, the alcohol, acting equally upon all cell- junctions in the nervous system to increase their resistance to the passage of the nervous current, will first raise this resistance to the point of impermeability in those junctions in which it is normally highest, that is, in the latest developed paths of highest func- tion ; and it will progressively effect a similar paralysis of other nerve-paths in the descending order of functional dignity and complexity. Action of alcohol purely narcotic. It may be added that a review of the many laborious attempts made in recent years to determine by the methods of the laboratory the effects of alcohol on the mind and nervous system, shews that such observations harmonise well with these general conclusions ; for, although some of the earlier workers on these lines belie;ved they had found evidence of an initial stimulating effect of alcohol, this appeared in all cases to be of but small extent and duration ; and later work throws doubt upon the validity of this interpretation of the evidence 38 and supports the conclusion that the direct effect of alcohol upon the nervous system is, in all stages and upon all parts of the system,' to depress or suspend its func- tions, that it is, in short,^froni first to last a narcotic drug. Varieties of mental symptoms in dninkenness. If we have truly stated the principle according to which alcohol attacks the functions of the brain and of the nervous system generally, it will be seen that mental changes are naturally among the first of all the symptoms of derangement to appear. With small doses of alcohol, they may be the only symptoms which are noticeable ; with larger, they are the earliest of the whole sympto- matic train of changes. It is obvious that though traceable by psychological analysis to one single source, namely the blunting of that intellectual self-criticism and control which the mind normally exerts, the detailed forms which they assume will, under the manifold ■\'arieties of individual circumstance and character, appear disordered items of behaviour . almost Protean in shape and kind. Yet of these, various as they are, certain sub-types appear with such frequency, and have been so accreditably documented in carefully conducted test experiments,^ as to be worth .specific mention here, since they can be regarded as established marks sympto- matic of alcoholic effect. In this respect they ha^e therefore some practical importance. They may be sub- sumed briefly thus : (i) Uncritical self-satisfaction of the subject with his own performances, (2) disregard ' For a possible exception to this statement, in the case of the nerve centres of respiration, see pp. 69-70. ' Kraepelin. Dber die Beeinflussung einfacher psychischer Vorgange dnrch einige Arzneimittel. Jena, . 1893. Rivers. Influence of Alcohol and other Drugs on Fatigue. Ivondon, 1907. Partridge. Studies in the Psychology of Intemperance. New York, 1912. 39 of occurrences and conditions iiormall)- evoking caution of act and word, (3) trespass of rules and conventions previously respected, (4) impaired appreciation of the passage of time, (5). loquacity and (6) an argumentative frame of mind. Early appearance of purely mental effects of alcoliol. These changes of mentality actually observed in labora- tory experiments directed toward' study of alcoholic effect, are largely confirmed by general experience. As laboratory experiments shew, they may occur when the dose and degree of action of the alcohol are quite in- sufficient to cripple manifestly and openly the power to perform routine technical operations, such as adding figures or typewriting by a person accustomed to perform them. To attempt to specify the lower limit of dose required to produce these symptoms is probably of little value. Not only is there varying susceptibility to alcohol from person to person, and not only does, in one and the same person, the susceptibility differ according to circum- stances, digestive and other, under the same dose, but intellectual self-criticism and control are strong in one person, weak in another, and, in the same person, while strong in respect of certain kinds of acts, may be weak in respect of certain others. With large doses these slighter mental effects are part of the train of symptoms passed through as profouhder degrees of intoxication are approached. Disturbance of higher mental functions in conditions falling short of drunkenness. But a point of greater practical importance is that, without signs of intoxication in the full ordinary or in the legal sense of the term, the bearing and individual attitude of mind suffer temporary change as an effect of the drug; and those in contact with the person so affected have for the time being to deal with an altered 40 individual, whose mind lacks temporarily- its normal factor of judgment and conspicuous elements of its self- control. There is hardl}' any need to emphasise here the' obvious fact that the directions which these alterations tend to take commonly, even as instanced merely by the few sub- types of deviation of judgment and behaviour just men- tioned, are likely to be fraught with serious consequences for the due discharge of responsibilities in all walks of practical life. Accuracy, avoidance of accidents, tactful handling of colleagues and subordinates, observance of discipline, punctuality, reticence in matters of confidence, are all obviously jeopardised; and an additional source of friction is brought to complicate the relations between the employer and the employed. 41 CHAPTER I^^ al,cohol and the performance of Muscular Acts. Complexity of muscular acts. The effect produced by alcohol on the performance of muscular work is not so readily determinable as might at first sight appear. The performance of a muscular act even of a simple kind involves a number of processes. The actual contraction of the muscle is only the final step in a series of events. The action of the muscle has invariablj' to be called forth by recurrent nervous action, and this nervous action includes, even in the simplest voluntary act, a linked series of processes with which many parts of the nervous system are concerned. Influence of alcohol on their performance due to action on nervous system, and not on muscles. The muscle being the final executant of the act, a point desirable of elucidation is how far the administration of alcohol influences directly the functioning of the muscles themselves. The muscles differ sufficiently from the organs of the nervous system to make it probable that the influence of alcohol on the two will not be the same, at least not of the same magnitude. The influence of alcohol upon muscles separated from the nervous system has been examined, and it has been found that when administered to them through the blood in doses up to the equivalent of about 70 cubic centimetres in man or nearly 5 oz. of whisky at proof strength,^ alcohol produces no obvious effect upon the contractile power or other functional properties of muscle. 'V. Furth and C. Schwarz, Pfliiger's Archiv, f. d. ges. Physio, logie, Vol. 129, p. 525. 1909. 42 We may infer then that any influence which alcohol, in such doses as are met with in ordinary human consumption of it, exerts upon the performance of muscular acts, must be referred to its effects upon the nerve centres concerned with activating the muscle. Reflex acts and volitional acts. In regard to the nervous processes which find their expression in muscular acts, they are divisible into two main kinds. Those of one kind are termed volitional, because produced at the behest of the will ; of the other, involuntary, because independent of the will. The former, even at their simplest, are complex. The latter, often spoken of as " reflexes," are less so. These latter are for this reason more completely understood and analysed ; the nervous events involved in them are sufficiently known to serve as standards by -vvhich the efiect of alcohol upon certain fundamental processes of nervous activity can be gauged. The relative simplicity of the reflexes has made it possible for medical study to ascertain with exactitude what parts and elements of the nervous system are required for their perfor- mance and to judge from them whether those parts are working well or ill. Changes in them give fairly precise information as to the seat and manner of any improvement or impairment they may shew. They are of importance further since it is by means of the nervous centres which the reflex acts use that volitional nervous acts, initiated in the higher nervous centres, exercise their effect upon their executant muscles. Skilled movements of hand and arm, executed under the mandate of the will, demand for their performance the employing by the brain of those same lower centres which the reflexes of the limb employ and test. The brain centres bring the muscles into action through the lower reflex centres. 43 If for some part of the body, say leg or eye, these latter are deranged, it is not to be expected that the skilled execution, by that part, of acts under the behest of the will can continue to be perfect or normal. Inftuencc of alcohol on simple reflex adion. (i) The knee jerk. A simple reflex act, much used by the physician for testing the healthy working condition of the spinal cord, is the knee-jerk. A light tap is dealt to the front of the knee just below the knee cap, on the tendon of the muscle that straightens the knee. The tap is delivered at a time when the limb is at rest, for instance when the knee is passively resting crossed upon the other knee. This light blow stretches slightly and briefly the muscle, and this slight stretch excites nerve-fibres which pass from the muscle to the nerve-centres in the spinal cord, and so excites these centres. The centres in their turn excite the muscle by means of the motor nerve passing from them to it. This causes the jerk-like movement of the knee. This reaction tests therefore, (/) the nerves passing from the muscle to the spinal centres and from the latter to the muscle (//) the spinal centres themselves ; and (/») the muscle. It gives reliable indications not only of the healthy or disordered condition of these parts but in several respects also of the condition of the brain itself as influencing that of the spinal cord. The indications it yields are the more valuable because it is a reflex which lies beyond the voluntary control of the person examined. It cannot be quickened or slowed, increased or diminished at wil by the person experimented on. Elxperiment shews that a dose of 30 cubic centimetres of alcohol, equivalent to a little over 2 oz. of whisky at proof, or to about i^ pint of beer of average strength {i.e., containing 4 per cent, of absolute alcohol), administered an hour to an hour and a half earlier, lessens the speed and amplitude of the movement of the 44 knee-jerk in healthy persons. As an average obtained from observations on six men, this dose of alcohol reduced the speed of commencement of the responsive movement bj' 9'6 per cent, and diminished the extent of the movement by 48'9 per cent.^ A larger dose, 45 cubic centimetres of alcohol, in the same persons impaired the reaction speed and amplitude still more. {it) The " eye-closing reflex." Another reflex, somewhat less simple, is the "eye- closing reflex." The movement is an involuntary blink which occurs when some local danger imperils the eye, as when grit enters it or a blow threatens it. It commonly occurs also when some startling shock is received by the body, or when a loud sudden unexpected noise is heard. Although it is an involuntary act, repression of it can by practice and training be acquired in regard to circumstances which otherwise regularly evoke it. Thus, as a response to a threatening by a blow or to a sudden loud noise, those who are practised in boxing and in the use of firearms learn to suppress it. The effect of alcohol on this reflex has been examined^ in the same six normal men whose knee-jerk was tested as above. The dose of 30 cubic centimetres alcohol retarded the reflex speed by 5'9 per cent., taking the average from four of the men ; it decreased the eyelid movement's extent by 107 per cent., taking the average measure from five of the men. 45 cubic centimetres of alcohol, equivalent to about 3 oz. of whisky at proof, or to nearly 2 pints of beer, impaired the reflex still more. In two of the men the smaller dose slightly increased the reflex-speed, and in one of these two the larger dose also increased it. The means used in these observations for evoking the eye-blink reflex was the sudden production ' Dodge & Benedict, Psychological Effects of Alcohol, Washington, 1915- ' Dodge & Benedict, op. cit., 1915. 45 of a loud urgent noise. Both of the men in whom the dose of alcohol induced increase in the speed of the reflex were found normally and apart from any dosage with alcohol to exhibit unusually small blink-reflexes. The observers concluded that the normal blink-reflex in these two men was a partially restrained one, some degree of inhibitory control over the reflex having been acquired b^- them. One of them was practised in boxing and one in revolver shooting. The observers concluded that the quickening of the reflex produced in these two cases by the alcohol was due to the alcohol weakening the acquired inhibitory control. Deprcssaul effect of alcohol on simple reflexes. This dose of alcohol, therefore, depresses these simple reflex reactions of the nervous system. Even in regard to the somewhat greater frequency of the pulse-rate which commonly follows the administration of a moderate dose of alcohol, the cause seems to lie in a depressant rather than a stimulant action of the drug. The accelera- tion of pulse appears to be due to depression in degree of the reflex cardio-inhibitory tone which normally restrains the heart-beat.' The depression caused by the alcohol in all these instances indicates a specific lowering of the powers of the lower nervous centres, of a nature re- sembling, though much less in intensity, that produced by chloroform and drugs of that kind, tending towards temporary paralysis. Difficulty of analysing effect of alcohol on volitional acts. Simple reflexes like the above form a suitable starting point for inquiry into any influence which alcohol may exert upon the nervous system in its performance of muscular acts. But the step from such simple reflexes to acts initiated and controlled by the will is a consider- able one into a region of greater complexity. Besides the ' Dodge & Benedict, op. cit., 1915. Vide also Chapter VI, p. 73. 46 lower reflex centres through which the volitional processes must ultimately play, the nerve-centres concerned in calling forth and directing a muscular act at behest of the will are many, some in the highest parts of the brain and others in the lower, and some in the spinal cord itself. It is impossible to track the influence of alcohol step by step through such a maze. But it can be said with certainty that the degree of action of the alcohol will not appear equally in all the centres nor in all the phases of their processes. The detailed nature of the normal inter- action of the various centres, whence the willed muscular act results, is far from being as yet sufficiently known to justify here an attempt to analyse the influence of alcohol in regard to the steps of the process taken piecemeal. Effect of alcohol on efficiency of willed movements. Experiments have however been made which, without attempting to analyse the willed act, and accepting the movement made as index of its success, indicate how its efficiency changes with var5'ing conditions. Three main directions in which the efficiency may varj' are (i) in power, (2) in ability to withstand fatigue, and (3) in nicety of adjustment for the object in view. The power of a muscular act and its ability to withstand fatigue can be examined by the ergograph. This instrument registers the strength and extent of a particular willed movement which can be easily repeated, the instrument continuing its register throughout a series of repetitions of the act. The movement arranged for is purposely kept a very simple one, and therefore little scope is given in the ergograph for examination of nicety of adjustment, or other factors which constitute " skill." Ergographic experiments on the effect of alcohol. The influence of alcohol has been examined ergo- graphically by many observers. Some of the observations 47 appear, however, to be of small value for our purpose ; in some the dose of alcohol is unstated ; in some the form of ergograph was unsatisfactory ; in some too little heed was paid to circumstances other than the giving of alcohol likely to influence the muscular act under the conditions of the experiment. The earlier obser- vers, impressed with the simplicity of the actual movement employed as an index, did not appre- ciate fully the extent to which mental conditions might affect it. Experience with ergographic records has shewn that fleeting states of the mind, greater or lesser concentration of attention, or greater or lessei" interest in the repetition of the movement at one time than at another, may influence quite distinctly a person's performance of a movement even so simple as that chosen for the ergograph. Mental "suggestion" has to be excluded so far as possible i from disturbing the subject's attitude toward the experiment, which should be a neutral attitude. The mere knowing that he has or has not received a dose of alcohol ma}- affect his performance under the test and obscure or confuse any effect produced by the alcohol itself. The researches in which precautions have been taken definitely to safeguard against these various sources of error are still relatively few, and notable among them is the investigation by Rivers. ^ Rivers found that single doses of 5, of lo, and of 20 cubic centimetres of alcohol left no indubitable trace upon the muscular act, as recorded by the ergograph. His experiments were carried out upon two persons. When the dose was increased to 40 cubic centimetres, corresponding to over 2f oz. of whisky, or to about 1 1 pint of beer, an effect was produced on one of these. That person was habituallj- an abstainer from alcohol. The effect produced on his ergograph record Rivers. Influence of Alcohol and other Drugs on Fatigue. London. 1907. 4S T.vas increase in the series of contractions performed, this increase appearing about an hour and a half after the administration of the dose. The increase was small. •On the other person examined the dose of 40 cc. was followed by slight decrease of the ergographic work, but Rivers was not satisfied that in the case of this person the dose produced any clearly indubitable effect. The result stands in general conformity with results obtained previously b}^ Oseretzkowsky and Kraepelin^ who found no obvious evidence of an effect of alcohol on the ergographic record after administration of a single dose of 50 cubic centimetres {i.e., 3^ oz. of whisk>' or a little over 2 pints of beer). In the extensive series of ergographic experiments by Hellsten^, the ergograph employed was of an unusual type, the movement registered being executed with both arms, and therefore not so limited and simple as is generally preferred for ergographic examination. Working with this type of ergograph, Hellsten tested the influence of alcohol upon its records. The subject of experiment was an athlete of 90 kilos (between 14 and 15 stone) weight. Single doses of 25 and of 50 cubic centimetres of alcohol, given in appropriate dilution with water 5-10 minutes before the ergographic record began, produced no clear and unequivocal effect on the record. When the dose was 80 cubic centimetres, corresponding to between 5 and 6 ounces of whisky, or more than 3 pints of beer, there ensued, after a slight and brief- lasting improvement in the record, a marked decrease in the recorded muscular work. When this dose preceded the test by half an hour, the decrease observed amounted to 20 per cent, of the normal performance done without ' Oseretzkowsky n. Kraepelin, in Psychologische Arbeiten, Vol. 3, pt. 4, p. 587. 1901. ' Hellsten. Skandinav. Archiv f. Physiologic. Vol. 16, p. 160. 1904. 49 alcohol. The decrease was 17 per cent, when the dose preceded the test by one hour, and was 1 1 per cent, when it preceded the test by two hours. Animal experiments to test effect of alcohol on work. Comparable in some measure with the above experi- ments are those carried out by Chauveau,i who examined the influence of alcohol upon the output of work by a trained dog turning a treadmill. The dog had a measured and liberal daily ration of raw meat and cane-sugar. When qne- third, namely 84 grammes, of the daily sugar ration was re- placed by 50 cubic centimetres of alcohol the output fell by about 22 per cent. Similar results were obtained whether the alternation of the rations followed weekly or monthly. The dog lost weight on the alcohol substitution ration and maintained its weight on the ration without alcohol. The actual weight of the dog is not stated, but the daily amount of alcohol taken by it in the substituted ration must have been equivalent to not less than 250 cubic centimetres alcohol for a man, or not much short of a pint of whisky. This animal is, however, less susceptible to alcohol than is man! Observations on effect of alcohol on efficiency in hill-climbing. Comparable also to some extent with these observa- tions on the dog are observations by Diirig^ upon man. Diirig observed the effect of alcohol upon the muscular exercise involved in walking to the top of a hill. The ascent and the route taken and the time of day and other conditions of the ascent were kept as far as practicable the same for a number of successive repetitions, with the exception that on some days 30 cubic centimetres alcohol in 150 cubic centimetres of water, i.e., as much alcohol as ' Chauveau Comptes rendus de I'Acad^mie des Sciences. Vol. 132, pp. 65 and no. 1901. 'Diirig. Pfiiiger's Archiv fiir die ges. Physiologic. Vol. 113, p. 314. igo6. so is contained in 2 oz. of whisky or ij pint of beer, was taken in addition to the daily ration. This dose was taken at breakfast just before starting. It was found that although the walker, who was accustomed to moderate use of alcohol, felt in himself no difference between his condition on the alcohol and non-alcohol da}"s, the distance and ascent per minute was on the alcohol da3's less bj' 12-14 per cent, than that on the non-alcohol daj's. This was so, although the expenditure of energy by his body was greater on the alcohol days than on the non-alcohol days. Diirig inclined to attribute this deterioration in the performance of the ascent to impairment of skill with which movements are directed. He says that it was as if the effect of pre-\-ious training in the act were teiiiporarily lost. The experienced climber is reduced by the dose of alcohol towards the level of a beginner at such work and makes an unduly large number of badly directed or ill-judged movements. In short, alcohol in a dose of 30 cc. tended to undo the effect of previous training. The act here was of course much more complex and gave much more scope for skill than the acts examined by the ordinarj" ergograph. Disiitrbin^ effect of alcohol on skilled- movements. Rivers, in an appendix to the account of his ergographic observations, notes that, although the ergographic records shewed, even when the dose of alcohol was 40 cubic centi- metres, slight evidence onlj^ of their being influenced by it, there was from the experiments other evidence to the effect that after that dose " the control of movement did not appear to be so good," and that the execution of movements tended to be slower than usual. His evidence for this is as follows : — " In the normal condition," i.e. of the sub- ject ejcperimented with, "the two minutes allowed him betweeii successive ei'gograms for taking the customar}' readings and for making necessai-y adjustments of the •ergograph were ample for his doing so ; but on the 40 cc. alcohol days the period of 2 minutes was hardly long enough for him to do what was necessar\', although the time it took him seemed to him no longer than usual. This was so striking that the subject was at first inclined to believe that his watch was in error, for it seemed to him that he had been carrying out his usual task at the normal speed." "Several small accidents happened on days on which the •dose of alcohol was 40 cc, and these were probabh' the result of awkwai'dness in adjusting the apparatus. Some of the intervals " (between the actual spells of exercise at the ergograph) ''were occupied" (by the subject) "in drawing lines for tabular purposes or in pasting ergograms in a book, and these operations were found afterwards to have been done roughh* or irregular!}- on the 40 cc. alcohol days." " Within half an hour of the taking of the 40 cc. alcohol there came on," in this subject, " a subjective feeling of lassitude and disinclination for activity either of body or mind." " It was doubtful how far the state of lassitude was preceded by one of exhilaration, but, if the latter occurred, it was certainly of ver}^ brief duration." " During the state of lassitude there was decided irrita- bility ; and a fellow worker states that he was able to recognise clearly the days on which the 40 cc. dose had been taken by the general demeanour of the subject — partly from his lassitude, partly from his very obvious irritability." Effect of alcohol on adjustment of eye- movements. Rivers, therefore, although he found no clear evidence of influence of alcohol upon the performance of the muscular act examined by his ergograph until the single dose had reached 40 cubic centimetres and sometimes not then, con- cluded that that dose did in one of his two subjects impair ]6o66 E the execution of some skilled niovenieiits incidental tO' those experiments but not recorded b>- the ergograph. This- stands in harmony with observations b\- Guilleryi on the influence of alcohol on certain movements of the eyes. (Guillery tested the ability of the eyes to follow clearly, an object brought nearer and. nearer to the person observing it, that is to keep both eyes focussed on it as it approached. This requires a convergent movement of the eyeballs, the convergence increasing in degree as the object is brought closer. At a certain closeness further convergence be- comes impossible and fixation is lost, the object no longer appearing single. He found that the degree of conver- gence possible for the person was not affected, either in the direction of improvement or impairment, b)' a dose of 20 cubic centimetres alcohol, equivalent to nearly I5 oz. of whisky, or to over f of a pint of beer, but that by a dose of 40 cubic centimetres it was very distinctly impaired. The impairment took the form of weakening and of slowing of the movement. The impairment was first detectible about 20 minutes after the taking of the dose ; normal speed and ' power were regained about 40 minutes later. With 60 cubic centimetres alcohol the impairment was greater, came on about 10-15 niinutes after the dose and lasted for an hour and a half. The opposite movement of " divergence " was impaired even more, and other eye movements similarly examined were found also to be impaired. Guillery re- marked that under no grade of dose of alcohol and at no time under any dose were the eyeball movements found to be strengthened or rendered quicker. Similarly, Dodge and Benedict found that a dose of 45 cubic centimetres of alcohol measurably impaired the -speedpf starting the movement of turning of theeyes toward a fresh object : in short, the speed of directing the gaze. The whole movement is one of considerable complexity ' Guillery. Pfluger's .\rchiv fiir die ges. Pliysiologie. Vol. 79. v.- 597' 1899- 53 of nervous adjustment. The twelve muscles of the eyeballs have all of them to act appropriately together, that j^is to say, some have to be made to contract more than they were contracting, others have to be relaxed, from contraction, and these changes have to be made in each muscle with harmonious rate and degree. To CKecute the movement the nerve centres must not only have perfect control of the necessary motor powers but must be aware of the posture the eyeballs start from and of the direction toward which they have to be moved. These latter requisites demand the alertness of sensory nerve-centres, information from which is a factor in the guidance and alertness of the motor centres themselves. The act is, of course, one of extremely frequent execu- tion throughout the waking day, and from an early period of infant life onward. It is indeed in many respects a reflex act; and it is under many circumstances impossible, and under still more very difficult, of repression by the will ; it tends to occur " in spite of ourselves." A form, however, in which we meet its use in a highly practised technical act is that trained movement learned in reading which enables the eyes to follow the words across a page and then return correctly to the first word in the line immediately below. Like all other acts of the kind a slight delay attends its starting, a delay which though it is not long is yet considerably longer than the delay attending such simpler reflexes as the knee-jerk. The influence of alcohol on this delay has been tested and on the same persons as those forming the subjects of the experiments on the simpler reflexes. A' dose of 45 cubic centimetres alcohol was found an hour and a half after it had been taken to increase the delay (average of all six men) by 15 per cent. The accuracy of the movement, that is, the degree of truth with which the eyeballs when moved hit the required direction, was not tested. 54 Effect of alcohol on speed of to ami fro movement of linger. The same observers designed an experiment testing the performance of an act relatively simple in character yet one which is not naturally regularly practised, namely a rapid to and fro movement of a finger, the person being told to move the finger thus as rapidly as possible. The index of success taken was the speed with whjch the movement could be alternated, the rate at which the to and fro movements could be made to follow each other. \Vhile requiring the action of relatively lower nervous centres the exercise demands therefore in addition some effort of the higher centres in controlling and urging the former. It involves activity of the will and, though not to a high degree, demands that kind of effort which is necessary for acquiring facility in a novel manoeuvre which habituation has not as yet made easy. It is there- fore to this extent a test of skill, skill taking effect in speed. It tests, though very simply, just that type of nervous process which is involved in the first steps of learning to use a new tool or attaining dexterity in a new manual process. The effect of alcohol as thus tested was examined in the same .six men employed for the " turning of the gaze " test. The dose of 45 cubic centimetres of alcohol taken an hour and a half previously reduced the rapidity with which the movement could be performed by 8"8 per cent, {average of the six persons). Conclusions. It will be seen that experiments suitable for yielding inferences for the present purpose are not numerous ; and that, if for that i-eason alone, caution is necessarj- in making deductions from them. Yet, they agree in indicating that a single dose of less than 40 cubic centi- metres of alcohol, or as much as would be taken in about, 2| oz. of whisky at proof, or in ij pint of beer, in an adult accustomed to moderate use of alcohol, exerts little or no appreciable influence on the performance by him of a muscular act of simple character not demanding precision. For acts requiring skill the inference from the experiments, so far as they go, seems, however, to be that their performance tends to be temporarily impaired after a dose of alcohol of even less than 40 cubic centi- metres, e.g. 30 cubic centimetres; especially in diminished speed and nicety of the required act's performance. It seems therefore permissible to suppose that the greater the precisional delicacy and alertness demanded in a muscular act and the greater its degree of difficulty, e.g. by reason of novelty to the performer, the more liable will that act be to shew impairment under the influence of alcohol, and, within limits, the smaller will be the dose of alcohol which may impair the act. Reliable evidence that alcohol improves, in normal circumstances, the efficient performance of any muscular act, unskilled or skilled, seems at present to be altogether lacking. 56 CHAPTER V. Action of Alcohol on the Digestion. Factors in digestion ivhich ina\ he influenced by alcohol. The process of digestion is ver>' complex and it is convenient to distinguish the following factors each of ■which might be modified by alcohol. (i) The secretion, or outpouring, of the digestive juices, saliva, gastric juice,- pancreatic juice and bile, which may be altered either, directly, by some action upon the glands which form the juices, or indirectly, through nerves which convey impulses leading to alterations of quality or quantity in the juices poured out. (2) The churning movements of the digestive organs, particularly the stomach, movements which normally aid ■digestion but, when abnormal in kind or degree, hinder it and often cause pain. (3) The actual digestion or chemical transformation of such food-stulFs as undergo it and their subsequent absorption after they have been more or less profoundl)- changed by the action of the digestive juices. Effect iif alcohol on secretion of digestive juices. Beginning with (i) we may first inquire in general terms whether alcohol does, or does not, cause a flow of •digestive juices, and if so, how. So far as the saliva is concerned, there is no doubt that alcohol taken into the mouth does, like anj- other sapid fluid, or indeed, like the act of chewing inert substances such as india-rubber, cause a flow of saliva. This action is not of much importance. So far as the gastric juice is concerned, the question is more important and the answer less simple. Thus, alcohol might cause a flow indirectly, by acting upon nerves of taste or smell ; any agreeable taste in the 57 •mouth is well known to do this; but we cannot say that this is peculiar to alcohol, and the onlj- direct observation (upon a woman who had an artificial opening in her giillet and another in her stomach) did not afford anj- decisive information. When alcohol reaches the stomach, it certainly arouses a considerable secretion of a juice, partly, perhaps, by directly irritating the wall of the stomach but largely through a specific action on the cells which manufacture the digestive juices. This has been proved by man\' •experiments on animals and also by observations upon men who, in consequence of disease or injurj', had had artificial openings made into their stomachs through which alcohol could be introduced, often without the patients knowing what had been given. In animals, a part of the stomach separated from the main stomach :secreted actively when alcohol was given by the mouth. The gastric juice which is secreted owing to the action of alcohol is not, however, of a normal kind. It contains the usual amount of h5'^drochloric acid but very little pepsin — the ferment which is concerned in the digestion ■of albuminous substances by the stomach. In fact, all the pepsin which the juice contains has been merely washed out of the cells; no fresh supply is formed in Tesponse to the action of alcohol. It accordingly follows that this increased amount of gastric juice is of little or no value in the process of digestion, although there is no reason to suppose that it is in any way injurious. So far as the amount of secretion is concerned, the increase, in dogs, is greatest when small doses of alcohol, producing in the stomach a concentration not higher than lo per cent., are given. As the amount and its concentration increase, the rate of secretion falls off and there is a tendency to form much slimy substance (mucus). The greater the amount and the concentration, the greater the formation of mucus. When the alcohol was 5« repeatedly given, even in dilute form, the iuucu> secretion became more marked and the increase of total juice smaller. It is said that the [xvicrcntic secretion is also increased by alcohol, given by the stomach or by injection through the anus into the large bowel. This, together with the alleged increase in the secretion of bile,. may in part be due to some action on the stomach, for an increase of gastric secretion often leads to an increase in the activit}' of the pancreas. Action of alcohol oil inovciiiciits of stomach. Passing to (2), the churning movements of the stomach are a natural part of the digestive process — they aid' digestion. They aid it, not only by promoting digestion in the stomach itself, but also b_\- duh' passing on partially digested food from the stomach into the intestine, for the completion of its digestion there. It is commonly stated that alcohol promotes the movements of the stomach, but the experimental methods by which it has been sought to establish this conclusion were not altogether satisfactory. Chittenden', for instance, did not find that the sta>' of food in the stomach of a dog was materialh- altered by the presence of alcohol. Carlson-, who investigated the movements of the stomach in a man with an artificial opening (fistula) into that organ, found that diluted alcohol, brandy and various wines introduced directly through the fi.stula, arrested the rhythmical movements and the bracing of the muscular coats (tonus) which are associated with hunger, but saw no increase of movement.. 50-100 cubic centimetres (2-3 ounces) of 10 per cent., alcohol were enough to do away with the hunger con- tractions for two hours, and 200 cubic centimetres (half a' ' Chittenden, in Physiological Aspects of Liquor Problem, Vol. I., p. 294 ; Boston, 1903, ^ Carlson, .\nierican Journal of Physiology. Vol. 32, p. 252, 1913.. 59 pint) of beer had this effect for from half an hour to an "hour. So far indeed as the evidence goes, it would seem that there is no reason to believe that gastric movement is increased by alcohol while some forms of contraction are arrested. This lessened movement of the stomach, a so-called carminative action, is produced b}' many other volatile substances, and maj' explain the relief of discomfort and colic-like pains which svich substances, including alcohol,, may afford. Eifcct of alcohol on activity of digestive ferments. Finally, under (3), we take first the chemical process of digestion. It has already been said that the gastric ferment pepsin is not increased in amount hy alcohol, but its activity might be. This is a matter which has engaged the attention of manj- investigators and their general results are concordant. The possible action in this way can be measured by ascertaining the amountof change which gastric juice is able to effect in food with which it is mixed in test tubes containing different small amounts of alcohol. It j has been shown that while 1-2 per cent, of pure alcohol has little effect upon the activity of the juice, in the presence of from 5-10 per cent., digestion is slightly retarded, while a concentration of more than 10 per cent. ver>- definitely diminishes the rate of digestion. If the gastric juice used contains but little ferment, the retardation induced by even 10 per cent, alcohol is considerable. (Such a concentration can rarely if ever be present in the stomach of a living man for more than' a few seconds.) Ordinary forms of spirits act on gastric digestion merely in proportion to the amount of alcohol they contain, but wines, especially red wines, and sherry, retard digestion much more than can be explained by the alcohol they contain, evidently owing to the activity of some other 6o constituents. The same is true of the beers, although these in small quantities have no pronounced effect. The above general statement applies equallj- to the- salivarj- ferment, viz., little or no action when the proportion is low, retardation when the alcohol is stronger than lo per cent. Here, again, wines and beers have a retarding effect out of proportion to their content of alcohol, an additional, and more powerful, factor here being the acidity of some wines. Although some authorities have belie^•ed that the action of alcohol upon pancreatic digestion was less un- favourable than upon gastric digestion, one even stating that the digestion of fats is accelerated by it, there is no , doubt that some of the ferments themselves when examined in test tubes, are more sensitive to the action of alcohol than are those of the stomach. The digestion of albuminous substances may be delayed by so little as 2-3 per cent, concentrations. Spirits are here more deleterious than can be explained by their alcohol, while the acid reactions of wines and beers are unfavourable to the digestion. In practice it is probable that alcohol reaches the intes- tine in such comparatively small quantities and such dilute .solution, while it is also so quickly absorbed there, that its action on pancreatic digestion is of small importance. To sum up these results, we may say that moderate quantities of alcohol, .particularly when mixed with other foods, have no serious effect, good or bad, upon the chemical processes of gastric or pancreatic digestion, and this is true both of persons who are moderate users of alcohol and of total abstainers. Psychic effect of alcohol in irlaiion to digestion. It has been suggested that the action of alcohol upon the central nervous system, the brain and spinal cord, may help digestion by distracting the subject's attention 6i from worries and promoting a more cheerful outlook upon life, because of the interaction of the nervous system with other organs of the body. Whether this is or is not the case cannot be determined by laborator>' experiment. Jt is a matter of common knowledge that many people «at, they say, with more appetite, if alcohol is drunk with or before the meal, but there is no reason to think that the actual transformation of food into substances fit to be absorbed is increased, although one may derive more pleasure from a meal at which alcohol is taken. Alcohol and the absorption of food. With respect to absorption itself, that is the passage of the digested food-stufis into the blood, while there is no doubt that some drugs, for instance chloral and strychnine, are readily absorbed when given in alcohol, more so than when taken in water, and there is some reason to think that small quantities of alcohol in the 'intestine promote the absorption of food-stufFs, there is no reliable evidence leading to the conclusion that moderate doses have an}'^ serious effect, one way or the other, upon the process of absorption of food. The possibility that alcohol may promote the absorption of harmful bodies which would not otherwise make their way into the blood, is considered elsewhere.^ Excessive doses of alcohol are liable to cause vomiting, partly through local irritant action in the stomach but mainly indirectly through disturbances in the nervous mechanisms. The whole subject of this section can be epitomised in the following terms. Conclusions. Moderate doses have never been shown to affect appre- ciably the digestive organs apart from their taste and their tendency to increase the secretion of fluid and mucus from the walls of the stomach. ' See p. 95. 62 On the other haud, there is a consensus of opinion that some of the alcoholic beverages may be more deleterious than others ; wines which do not notice- ably interfere with the digestion of one man may cause trouble to another.^ The alcohol present in these wines cannot be regarded as responsible, for the effects are not observed when the same quantity and concentration of alcohol is taken in another form. The solid non-volatile bodies in the wines have usually been blamed for the dyspepsia sometimes associated with the use of them, and possibly with justice. But it is not possible to dismiss the volatile bouquet substances from consideration, as similar differences are observed in the use of different forms of spirits where the non- volatile substances are present in but very small quantities and appear to be harmless. In these spirits, the higher alcohols (fusel oil) and the ethers are popularly credited with the action on the stomach, and there is some ground for believing them more disturbing to the digestion than pure alcohol. The changes induced by these bodies are, however, quite unknown, and may be different in character from those due to alcohol itself. ' Roberts. Collected Contributions on Digestion and Diet, 1891. Chittenden, in Physiological Aspects of Iviquor Problem, Vol. I, 1903. CHAPTER VI. Action of Alcohol on the Respiration and on THE Circulation of the Blood. This chapter is concerned with some of the minor effects which follow the use of alcohol in moderate doses : effects so small that they are easih' overlooked altogether, and have no real importance in the ordinary use of alcohol by normal persons. In the literature devoted to the action of alcohol these effects have received an amount of attention which seems ■out of proportion to their real significance.' There are two reasons for the importance which has been attributed to them. In the first place it must be realised that a large part of the reputation which alcohol has enjoyed as a medicine, is attributable to the belief that it stimulates a feeble heart or failing respiration. With medical men, until comparatively recent years, alcohol was the favourite " respiratory and cardiac stimulant," and, indeed, practi- -cally the only one available. It is still the most popular domestic remedy for the temporary failure of the heart, and secondarily of the respiration, which is known as " faint- ing." It is clearly of importance, therefore, to know whether alcohol really possesses the stimulating action on these vital functions, with which it has been generally ■credited. A second reason for the amount of investigation which has been devoted to these effects, is that the discussion of their nature formed part of a wider controversy, concern- ing the action of alcohol on the body generally, and ' For a general review of the literature, except for recent years, see Abel, in "Physiological Aspects of the Liquor Problem," Vol. II, Boston, 1903. 64 especiallj- on the brain and nervous system. In the chapter dealing with the mental action, we have seen that one school of opinion has maintained that moderate doses of alcohol have a truly stimulant action, while another school has regarded the appearance of increased mental activity as misleading, and has supported the view that alcohol, even in the smallest dose having any perceptible effect, is always depressant or paralysing in its action. The effects on the respiration and the pul^e are such as can easily be measured and recorded, and this partisans of the one theory were accordingly eager to demonstrate that alcohol had a genuine stimulant action in these cases, however small it might be ; while their opponents showed a corresponding anxiety to prove , that any appearance of such a stimulant action could be explained in other ways. Sub-Chapter I. The Action of Alcohol on Respiration. Normal working of respiratory mechanism. I The movements of respiration, by which air is rhythmi- cally drawn into and expelled from the lungs, show wide; variations both of depth and rhythm, even under normal conditions of active life. These movements are initiateti by the activity of a nervous centre, called the " respira' tory centre,' which is situated in the hindmost part o^ the brain, just above the level where it becomes continuoiis with the spinal cord. The activity of the centre is nor- mally called forth by the presence of carbon dioxide in the blood carried to it by the circulation. If the blood-is deprived of carbon dioxide completelj-, the breathing stops altogether. If the quantity of carbon dioxide in the blood becomes abnormally great, the respiration becomes rapid and laboured ; as when we find ourselves ," out of 65 breath" after runiiiiig up a flight of stairs. The action of the centre is also modified by the activity of other parts of the brain and nervous system. The respiration is to a large extent controllable by the will, and is affected by every emotion ; we " gasp with astonishment,'' " hold the breath with anxiety," or " pant with eagerness." It is also subject to " reflex " effects from the stimulation of sensory nerves ; a sudden pain, a dash of cold water on the skin, makes us gasp or pant for breath. Indirect effects of alcoJiol on respimforv actwitx. Since the rhythm of breathing is normally subject to such wide variations, it is clear that a small effect . of a drug will be difficult to detect with certaintj', unless special precautions are taken. Under ordinary conditions a dose of alcohol will produce, in nian\- persons, a condition of excitement, talkativeness, and restless muscular activity. The fact that this is accom- panied by quicker and deeper breathing cannot be taken to represent an action of alcohol on respiration ; for a similar access of respiratory activity can be seen in persons of excitable temperament if, without having taken alcohol, they become engaged in animated discus- sion and gesticulate freely. Earlier nbsoi'aiions : difficulties in their interpretation. Many competent observers formerly stated, however, that the taking of a moderate dose of alcohol, by a subject kept in repose and carefully protected from disturbing influences, was followed by a small but definite increase of respiratory activity. All forms of increase in activity were recorded : a quicker rate, with deeper, unaltered, or shallower individual respirations, or even a deepening of each breath without alteration of the rate. The uett result was, however, in general an increase in the volume of air breathed in a given time. One observer 66 stated that the effect was more pronounced with a wiiie •of fine bouquet than with pure alcohol, and noted its special prominence in subjects suffering from fatigue. A long and vigorous discussion arose as to the nature of this effect. It would be unprofitable to follow this into all its detail, and it will sufiice to indicate the main lines of the argument. Those who believed that alcohol never stimulates the brain, and who were there- fore unwilling to admit that it would stimulate the respirator}' centre, attributed the effect to an irritant action on the mucous membrane of the stomach. The similar effects seen in experiments on animals, when the alcohol was inhaled as vapour, or injected under the skin, were similarly attributed to irritation of the lining of the wind-pipe and the lungs, or the nerves in the subcutaneous tissue. Other irritant substances, such as mustard,^were said to cause a similar increase of respiratory movement, when they were introduced into the stomach.^ On the other hand, the adherents of the " stimulant " theory of the action of alcohol, claimed the effect on respiration as supporting their view. They pointed out that the effect was apparently not diminished by diluting the alcohol freeh- with water before it was swallowed ; whereas the irritant action of strong spirit would be greatly reduced by such dilution.^ They laid much stress also on the fact that the injection of suitably diluted alcohol directly into the vein of an animal was followed by an increase of respiratory activity ; and under such conditions, there was no possibility of local irritative action, affecting the respiratory centre indirectly.'' ' Jaquet — Archives Internationales of 35 cubic centi- metres and another of 60 cubic centimetres of alcohol. The quantities of whisky at proof containing these amounts of alcohol would be 2| ounces and rather less than 4J ounces respectively. Only in one case was an increase in the excitability of the respiratory centre detected, namely, when the larger dose was given to the person who was not used to taking alcohol. Proportion of carbon dioxide in " alveolar air" as a measure of the excitability of the respiratory centre. Another method of measuring the excitabilitj- of the respiratory centre has been used in more recent experi- ments. The activity of the centre so adjusts itself to the need of the body to get rid of carbon dioxide, that the proportion of carbon dioxide in the blood ' tioewy — Pfliiger's Arcliiv. fur die gesammte Physiologie, Vol. 47, p. 601. 1890. 16066 F 68 filling the arteries is kept practically constant. The gases held by this blood are in equilibrium with the air in the deepest recesses of the lungs — the so-called "alveolar air." Samples of this air can easily be obtained, and analysed, and it has been found that the proportion of carbon dioxide in it remains very constant in the same individual under similar conditions. When the rate at which carbon dioxide is being formed in the body increases, the respiratory centre is stirred to greater activity, and the more active breathing quickly readjusts the proportion of carbon dioxide in the alveolar air to its normal level. Now it will be clear that, if the normally constant pro- portion of carbon dioxide undergoes a change and takes up a new level, this must indicate a change in the excita- bility of the respiratory centre. If more carbon dioxide accumulates in the air in the lungs, we know that the excitability of the centre has become less; for othenvise the tendency to such accumulation would have excited the centre to greater activity, and the more vigorous breathing would have swept carbon dioxide out of the lungs more rapidly, so as to maintain the normal propor- tion. If, on the contrary, the proportion of carbon dioxide in the alveolar air becomes less, we know that this indicates an increase in the excitability of the centre; for otherwise the tendency to diminution of the amount of carbon dioxide would have resulted in smaller activity of the centre, and the slower and shallower breathing would have allowed the normal proportion of carbon dioxide to remain. More active breathing may, there- fore, be brought about either by an increase in the excitability of the respiratory centre to the normal pro- portion of carbon dioxide, or by a more rapid production of carbon dioxide by the body. In the former case, a decrease in the proportion of carbon dioxide in the alveolar air would be detected, in the latter it would not 69 ■change perceptibly. The two influences may work in opposition — e.g., an increase in the excitability of the centre may be accompanied by diminished production of carbon dioxide; and in such a case there may be no change, or even a decrease of respiratory activity, although the excitability of the centre has risen. Experiments to determine the effect of alcohol on the proportion of carbon dioxide in the ''^ alveolar air. ^'' It will be clear that the interpretation of an apparent effect of alcohol on breathing is a much more complicated matter than the earlier investigators supposed. Quite recently the changes in all the difl^erent factors have been carefully investigated in a series of men.' Seven subjects were chosen for the experiments, whose ex- perience of alcohol included all variations from habitual abstinence to daily use. In each test two doses were used, 30 cubic centimetres and 45 cubic centimetres of pure alcohol, suitably diluted with a flavouring mixture ; the equivalents in alcohol of whisky at proof would be rather more than 2 ounces and 3 ounces respectively. The doses were large enough to produce distinct physiological effects — feelings of comfortable warmth and drowsiness, and in some cases talkativeness and mental excitement ; on the other hand, they were not enough to cause definite inebriation. The patients were kept care- fully at rest, and under constant observation, and each alcohol experiment was controlled by one iij which the sub- ject took the same flavouring mixture without the alcohol. In a certain proportion of the experiments there was an indication of a slight' increase in the excitability of the respiratory centre, the proportion of carbon dioxide in the alveolar air undergoing a small reduction after alcohol had been taken. This effect, however, was counterbalanced by a diminished production of carbon ' Higgins — Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Thera- peutics, Vol. 9, p. 441. 1917. 70 dioxide by the body. Tlie nett result on the breathing was that the volume of air breathed per minute was either unchanged or slightly diminished. Conclusions regarding the action of alcohol on respiration. There need be no hesitation in accepting these experi- ments, made by the most recent methods, as giving a true account of the action, or want of action of alcohol itself on the respiration. The fact that a definite, though small increase, in the volume of air breathed, was so consistently observed by the earlier workers to follow the taking of alcohol, may be explained in one of two ways. Pother (i) their precautions against an increase of muscular activity following the taking of alcohol were imperfect, so that the effect they obser\'ed was in part the indirect result of more rapid formation of carbon dioxide; or (2) the wines, brandy, etc., used in some of the earlier experiments contained, among their ethereal bouquet- substances, some which had more action on the respira- torj'' centre than alcohol. Only further experiment, by modern methods, could decide between these possibilities. For our present purpose it is sufficient to have reached the conclusion that alcohol, in moderate doses, has no effect on the respiration of practical importance, either for its use by normal persons, or for its medicinal use. The onh' important effect of alcohol on respiration is the paralysis of the respirator}- centre bj' large doses, which, as another chapter shows,^ is the cause of death in poisoning.^ > Chapter VIII, p. 86. '' Since this section was sent to the printers an interesting note has been published by Hooker (Journ. of Pharmacology and Exp. Therapeutics, vol. X., 1917, p. 121), who, by an ingenious method of artificial perfusion, has tested the direct action of alcohol on the respiratory centre, isolated from the general circulation. His results confirm the stimulant action of low concentrations of alcohol on the centre ; but the conditions are so abnormal, that no deduction can be made as to the importance of the action under normal conditions, and no revision seems necessary of our conclusions as to the trivial nature of the effect on respiration of alcohol as taken b%' man. 71 Sub-Chapter II. Action of Alcohol in the Circulation of the Blood. In what ways a drug max influence the Circulation. A drug can influence the circulation of the blood by causing either a change in the rate or strength of the heart's beating, or a change in the state of contraction of the blood-vessels, especially of the smaller arteries. The heart is a muscular pump, which at each beat drives a spurt of blood into the great arteries, causing a wave of pressure, the pulse-wave, to travel down them. Where the large arteries divide into small ones, and these again open into the net-work of capillary blood-vessels, the flow of blood encounters a pronounced resistance. As a result of this resistance, and of the elasticity of the larger arteries, a head of pressure is built up in these, by which the blood is driven on through the capillaries into the veins in a steady stream. The maintenance of this ■"arterial blood-pressure," which is one of the essential factors of an efiicient circulation, depends on the rate and volume of the heart-beat, and on the state of contraction of the small arteries. The arterial pressure rises when the heart-beats follow one another more rapidly, or when the volume of blood expelled at each beat increases ; and it rises also when the walls of the small arteries contract, and increase the resistance to the flow of blood out of the arteries. If the heart-beat becomes slower or weaker, or if a general relaxation of the small arteries occurs, the arterial blood-pressure falls. But the arteries of the whole body need not, and do not usually contract or relax together. The arteries in the skin, for example, may contract, while those of internal organs relax ; and the result may be that, while the pressure in the large arteries is unchanged, a larger proportion of the blood goes to the internal organs, and less to the skin than before. 72 In discussing the effect of alcohol on the circulation of the blood, the main points on which information are required are, therefore, its effect on the rate and strength of the heart-beat or the pulse, and its effect on the state of contraction of the smaller arteries ; and we further have to inquire whether, as the result of the effects on the heart and arteries, any change occurs in the pressure of the blood in the larger arteries. Effect of alcohol on the superficial blood-vessels. There are only two features of the action of moderate doses of alcohol on the circulation concerning which there is anything like a general agreement. All who have studied the matter are agpreed as to the general relaxation of the small arteries carrying blood to the skin. This causes a slight general flushing of the surface and accounts for the comfortable feeling of bodily warmth which the taking of alcohol produces. It is more important for its effect on the body temperature, which is dealt with in the following chapter, than for its effect on the general circulation of the blood. But it will be clear, from what has been said above, that such a dilatation of the skin vessels will be attended by some fall of the arterial blood - pressure, unless it is compensated either by a constriction of arteries carrying blood to internal organs, or by an increase in the activity of the heart. Effect of alcohol on the piUse-rate. The majority of observers are further agreed in stating that the swallowing of a moderate dose of alcohol is followed by an increase in the rapidity of the heart-beat, which lasts for about half an hour. There are some who have failed to detect this action, and others even who observed that the pulse became slower after alcohol was taken. The rate of the pulse is not a matter concerning which there can be a difference of opinion, and it can only 73 be concluded that different subjects react differently to alcohol in this respect, while dose and dilution have probably some infltience on the effect. There seems to be no doubt that a slight quickening of the pulse commonly occurs. In the most recent series of experiments on the effect of alcohol on respiration, described in the foregoing section, records of the pulse-rate were taken. These showed a small acceleration, following the taking of alcohol, in about half the experiments. In the others no change was detected.^ Quickening of the pulse iindcr alcohol is probably brought about indirectly. There is very good reason, however, for believing that this effect, weak and inconstant as it is, is not due to a direct action of alcohol on the heart. It has been seen to occur when alcohol of sufficient concentration (50 per cent.) was simply taken into the mouth and rejected without being swallowed. In this case, the quickening of the pulse only lasted for a few minutes, whereas, if the dose was swallowed, the quickening lasted for half an hour or so.^ It seems reasonable to attribute at least a part of the result to the continuance in the stomach of the irritating action of alcohol on mucous membranes, which must be the immediate cause of the effect seen when alcohol is held in the mouth. The heart-beat, though automatic, is under the constant controlling influence of the nervous system. Under normal conditions, the heart is held in check by impulses from an " inhibitory centre," so that the rate is less than that at which the heart would beat if freed from such control. This restraint can be weakened by various causes. The quickening of the heart in response to the irritation of mucous membranes by alcohol is probably an ' Higgius — loc. cit. ^ Dixon— Journal of Physiology, Vol. 35, p. 346. 1907. 74 example of a reflex weakening of this control. A quick- ening of the heart, largely due to a similar action, regularly accompanies muscular activity ; and it is not unlikely that the tendency to restless movement, which follows the taking of alcohol, also plays its part as an indirect cause of the quickened pulse. That the effect is, in the main, an indirect one, has been shown by experi- ments, conducted by a number of observers, on the action of alcohol on the heart isolated from the body. Experiment to test the action of alcohol on the isolated heart. The heart of a recently killed animal, if blood or a suit- able nutritive fluid is made to flow through its blood- vessels, can be kept beating for hours or days outside the body. Under such conditions its action is uncom- plicated by changes in the rest of the circulator3' system, and is free from all nervous control. Alcohol in accurately measured proportion can be added to the fluid perfusing the heart, and the direct and uncom- plicated effect of such addition on the beat of the heart can thus be studied. Most of those who have made such experiments have failed to detect any quickening or strengthening of the heart-beat under the influence of alcohol of any con- centration. ^ Such observers found that, in concentrations up to about o'l per cent., alcohol was without effect. Others have observed some strengthening of the beat as the result of such low concentrations of alcohol. - This, however, appears to have been due to the fact that the heart, during the long perfusion, had used up much of its reserve supply of food-material, needed for producing the energy of its beat, and that, under ' See Backman — Skandinavisches Archiv fur Physiologic, Vol. i8 (1906), p. 323, who gives full references to earlier work on these lines. ' Dixon — loc. cit. 75 such conditions, it was able to use alcohol as a fuel, just as it can use glucose (grape-sugar) added to the perfusion fluid. So that there was no evidence that alcohol, in these concentrations, had a specific stimu- lating action on the activity of the heart; on the other hand, it was harmless. Direct effect on the heart only obsemed with large doses, and then of depressant character. All are agreed that in greater concentrations alcohol is directly harmful to the heart. The investigators differ as to the concentrations at which the harmful effect Tjegins to be manifest. Those who used a pure saline fluid for perfusion through the vessels of the heart observed a depressant action as soon as the proportion of alcohol rose above ci per cent. On the other hand, in experiments in which blood was perfused, even -5 per cent, of alcohol was found by one observer to have no definitely harmful action. ^ It will be seen later (Chapter VIII) that when, in man, the concentration- of alcohol in the circulating blood rises above ci per cent., the condition of the subject becomes one of definite inebriation, while at a concentration of o'5 per cent., we are approaching the fatal limit. There seems no good reason, therefore, for ascribing to direct stimulation of the heart any favourable effect which a small dose of alcohol may appear to produce, or to direct depression of the heart, a large share in the danger to life produced by poisonous doses. The effect in either case, whether of quickening or depression, must be mainly the indirect result of the other actions of alcohol. Effect of alcohol on arterial blood-pressure. The effects of alcohol on the arterial blood-pressure are similarly small and indefinite. Some observers have ' Loeb — ^Archiv fur experimentelle Pathologic und Pharma- kologie, Vol. 52, p. 451. 1906. 76 described a relativelj' small rise of the arterial pressure, as the result of injecting small doses of alcohol directly into the circulation. ^ Others have failed to detect any definite effect, or have observed a small decline of arterial pressure ; while one found that the pressure fell if alcohol was injected into a vein or under the skin, but rose to a small extent, for a few minutes, if the same dose of alcohol was swallowed. - Conchtstons. The action of alcohol on the heart and circulation has theoretical interest for the pharmacologist, and has accordingly been the subject of much investigation and discussion. But no scientific ground has been discovered for any claim made on behalf of alcohol to practical value as a direct stimulant of the heart in cases of threatened failure of the beat. When it appears to pro- mote recovery from fainting, it probably acts simply by virtue of its irritant action on the mucous membrane of the mouth and throat. The fact that the beneficial effect appears almost immediately, and long before any signi- ficant amount of alcohol can have been absorbed and carried to the heart, is evidence for this local and indirect nature of the action. Its use in these circumstances is, therefore, comparable with that of smelling salts, or the irritating fumes of burnt feathers, traditionally employed for the same purpose. When, in conditions of more pro- tracted weakness of the heart, the administration of alcohol has a beneficial effect, this must be attributed mainly to its mildly narcotic and sedative action. ' Dixon — Loc. cit. Kochmann — Archives Internal, de Pliarmacody nu . \'ol. 13, p. 329. 1904. Bachem — Archives Internal, de Pharmacodyn. Vol. 14, p. 437. 1.905- Brooks — Journal of the American Medical Association. Vol. 30^ p. 373. 1910. 77 relieving the centres, which modifj' the action of the heart, from the disturbing influence of pain and anxiety. The promotion of a patient's comfort, the relief of mental strain, may be an essential element in the treatment of disease, and an important factor in recovery. It does not, however, justify the description of alcohol as a " stimu- lant" of the heart. In this direction, as in others, the popular idea that it is a " stimulant " proves on examina- tion to be scientifically untenable. 78 CHAPTER VII. The Influencf of Alcohol on the Body Temperature. Distinction between surface tempciatitre and deep temperature. In speaking of body temperature we must carefully distinguish between the surface temperature, i.e., the temperature of the surface of the skin, and the deep temperature, which is that of the underlying parts and the internal organs. The surface temperature is in general lower than the deep temperature, but the most important difference between the two is that, whereas the surface temperature varies widely, not only at different times but on different areas of the skin at the same time, the deep temperature is, in health, remarkably constant. If one hand is plunged into hot water (at 45 degrees Centigrade, i.e., 113 degrees Fahrenheit, for example) and the other into melting snow, the surface temperature of the former rises higher than the deep temperature, while that of the latter falls far below it. If the temperature on the surface of the body be taken immediately on rising from bed on a winter's morning, and again immediateh* after a cold bath, it will be found to have fallen through a long range. Through all these changes, howe^-er, the deep temperature will have remained practically unaltered, as may be verified by taking the temperature under the tongue, with the lips closed, before and after the cold bath. This maintenance of the deep temperature at a practi- cally constant le\el, with but small fluctuations from its normal average of 37 degrees Centigrade ^ (or 98*6 degrees ' Normally the temperature curve exhibits a regular diurnal cycle, reaching a maximum between 4 p.m. and 8 p.m. and a minimum between 2 a.m. and 6 a.m. The extreme range is between i and 2 degrees Fahrenheit, and the cycle is related to ■variations in the activity of metabolism. 79 Fahrenheit) is an essential condition of health ; for it is only at this temperature that the chemical and physical changes of the living tissues proceed at their normal rate. This constancy of the deep temperature gives to the clinical thermometer its value as an instrument for detecting the onset of illness, and for measuring its progress. Hoiv the deep temperature is maintained at its normal level in health. We have seen that the chemical changes of the bodj', \'iewed as a whole, are in effect a combustion, and are accompanied by the evolution of heat. This constant production of heat renders the body warmer than the air and most of its surroundings. Heat is, therefore, always being generated in the body, and always being lost at its surface to the air. The maintenance of the deep temperature at its normal level involves a continual adjustment of these two processes, so that the amount of heat lost at the surface just balances the amount that is evolved within. The rate at which heat is produced in the body is always varying, with changes in the acti^-ity of the muscles and of the glandular organs. The rate at which heat is lost from the body is likewise subject to continual variations, with changes in the temperature and moisture of the air, in the degree to which the skin is exposed to it, and in the rate at which winds and currents keep the cool air moving over the surface of the body. The heat-regulating centre. The adjustment of processes by which heat is produced and lost by the body so that an accurate balance is maintained, is under the control of a nervous centre in the brain, the unconscious action of which suffices for moderate changes of the external or internal conditionsr We may confine our attention to the manner in which the 8o ■ body protects itself against a/w// of the deep temperature, since this is of the greatest importance in connection with the action of alcohol. Auiomaiic adjustment of body tetnperature in exposure to cold. When the body, by exposure to cold, is threatened with a loss of heat at a rate exceeding that of its production, the first means of readjustment is a change in the distribution of the circulating blood as between the skin, where heat is lost, and the internal organs, where it is evolved. The nerves of the skin, excited by the surface cooling", ca:ll into action the nerve centre regulating the body temperature ; this, through the nerve-centre which controls the contraction of the small arteries, causes a narrowing of the channels through which blood is conveyed to the skin. Less of the blood streams through the cold skin, and more through the heat-producing organs in the interior. The result is a further fall of temperature of the skin ; but the all-important deep temperature is maintained. If the exposure to cold is more severe or prolonged, the narrowing of the blood vessels in the skin is inadequate to protect the body against excessive loss of heat, and the deep temperature begins to fall. This fall also affects the heat-regulating centre, through which the heat-producing organs, such as the muscles, are stimulated to activity, so that a more rapid evolution of heat takes place. We become conscious of the uncontrollable rhythmic contraction of the muscles and call it " shivering." How conscious effort instinctively assists automatic mechanism. So far we have dealt with the automatic, involuntary mechanism for maintaining the deep temperature of the body at a constant level. In the normally alert and 8i sensitive person this automatic regulation is assisted by conscious effort. Before the stage of shivering is reached we become conscious of a definite, unpleasant sensation, which we describe as " feeling cold." This is due to the fall in the temperature of the skin, accelerated, as we have seen, by the diversion of the blood to the interior of the body. Of a fall in the deep temperature we are not directly conscious, and are made aware of it only through the reaction of shivering. The feeling of discomfort and misery thus engendered is a naturalwarning of impending injury by cold, a signal that the deep temperature is beginning to fall, and that the balance bfetween production and loss of heat can no longer be upheld by the unconscious reflex mechanism. The desire to reinforce the automatic adjustment by conscious effort becomes urgent. We find ourselves impelled to accelerate the production of heat by vigorous muscular activity and to restrict still further the rate of its loss from the surface by putting on more clothes. If the excessive surface cooling, which is the cause of discomfort, is confined to a small part, such as a hand, we may endeavour to alleviate it by beating and chafing the part affected, and thus restoring in it a freer circulation of blood. Alcohol causes loss of heat through flushing of surface. Turning now to the action of alcohol, it will be re- membered that, in the previous chapter (p. 72), mention was made of a general flushing of the skin surface, a widening of the small arteries carrying blood to the skin, as one of its effects on the circulation of the blood. We saw, indeed, that this was the only one of its effects on the circulation concerning which there was no doubt or difference of opinion. In considering the bearing of this effect on the regulation of the body temperature, we must note, in the first place, that such flushing of the skin has, as a necessary result, 82 an accelerated loss of heat from the surface of the body. The question arises whether this is balanced by an increase in the production of heat by the internal organs. There is no evidence to indicate that alcohol directly affects the rate at which heat is formed in the body. Indirectl}', indeed, it may increase the rate of heat production, by weakening the control of the brain, and thereby favouring a restless muscular activity. In a resting subject it does not increase the production of heat, and the result of its effect on the circulation through the skin is a fall of the deep temperature. Alcohol hlttnts the ivarning sensation of cold. So far as the unconscious reaction to external cold is concerned, therefore, the effect of alcohol is to weaken the action of the heat-regulating centre, so that the protective constriction of the vessels carrying blood to the skin is relaxed. What of the conscious instinctive reaction ? The flushing of the skin relieves the un- pleasant sensation of " feeling cold," and replaces it by a comfortable glow of warmth. We have seen that under conditions of exposure to cold the unpleasant sensation is a valuable warning, which assists the maintenance of the deep temperature by creating an impulse towards muscular exertion, so that the production of heat is accelerated. This warning sensation alcohol blunts or annuls, and thereby tends to preclude the instinctive protective action, which the warning should call forth. It impairs or annuls, therefore, the conscious instinctive reaction, as well as the automatic, reflex mechanism of adjustment. From the point of view of maintaining the deep temperature, the influence of alcohol is evidently wholly bad. Its popular reputation as a means of " warding-off cold " is due to the relief which it affords from the warning discomfort. 83 Practical conclusions regarding use of alcohol by persons exposed to cold. If the practical question is asked, therefore, whether a person who feels cold can take alcohol with advantage, the answer must depend on a balance between discomfort and danger. Whereas it maj' well be considered legitimate to incur a trivial danger in order to avoid a considerable discomfort, even serious discomfort is not worthy of consideration if the alternative is danger to life itself. It may be safely stated that, under the conditions of ordinary life indoors, or in the open when the tempera- ture of the air is mild, the effect of a moderate dose of alcohol on the body temperature has no importance. In circumstances of brief exposure to cold, with the body as a whole well clad, the discomfort due to a chilling of the face and extremities maj' be mitigated bj' the use of alcohol without likelihood of producing a dangerous fall of internal temperature. The mental depression which accompanies the feeling of being " miserably cold," the discomfort of numbed hands, can be alleviated by a small dose of alcohol ; and the relief, though short-lived, reacts favourably for the timebeing on the complexion of the mind. The cheerfulness and sense of energy, which thus replace dejection and apathy, will not improbabh' awaken a desire for muscular activity ; so that the temporary and, in itself, illusory relief, provided by alcohol, may pave the way to the more genuine, natural and permanent remedy. Such uses of slcohol, for the relief of the feeling of cold, are suited only to a very brief emergency, created by exposure to a degree of cold which is merely unpleasant. The taking of alcohol during, or as a preliminarj^ to, pro- longed or severe exposure to cold, is on every ground to be condemned. Any sensation of warmth which it pro- duces, under such conditions, is an illusion fraught witB danger ; its effect is to render the subject indifferent to a peril, which is in fact seriously increased by its use. 16066 G 84 When, on the otheir hand, the exposure to cold is at an end, and the victim has been placed under conditions promoting warmth, the use of alcohol may have real value. If, for example, the surface of the bod} has become chilled and the deep temperature has been lowered by prolonged immersion in cold water, when once the patient has been wrapped in hot blankets and surrounded with hot bottles, the administration of alcohol can assist the restorative measures by promoting the return of blood to the superficial tissues, to ^vhich the external heat is applied. «.s CHAPTER VIII. Poison Action of Alcohol. Drug action and poison action differ onlv in degree. In the case of alcohol, as in the case of other drugs, it is impossible to draw any hard and fast line between drug action and poison action. Drug action, when, with increase of dose, it becomes excessive, passes into poison action ; but there is no absolute criterion of excess, and an action which under one set of circumstances and from one point of view may be regarded as excessive and harmful, may under other circumstances come within the denomination of a drug action. For instance, a person who drinks such a quantity of ether that he becomes unconscious would certainly be considered to be suffering from poisoning, and yet we do not ordinarily speak or think of poisoning in the case of a patient under an anaesthetic, though his condition may represent a more intense and dangerous action of the same drug. In reference to alcohol, then, it will be sufficient if, without trying to be too precise, we take it that a drinker really begins to suffer from acute poisoning as soon as he shows such immediate effects of the drug as interfere with his normal capacity for taking care of himself. The symptoms of ordinary drunkenness have been fully •described in Chapter III, where it was pointed out that they result from a progressive impairment of the functional activity of the nervous centres in the order from the highest to the lowest, that is to say, beginning with the centres of the brain that have to do with complex mental processes, such as those involved in critical judgment and self-control, and subsequently, 86 with deepening intoxication, affecting lower levels untit finally a state of stupor is reached. In this final stage of intoxication, when the drinker, in common parlance, is " dead drunk " or " paralytic drunk," the nervous centres which control such vital activities as the movements of the respirator}- muscles are more or less interfered with, as shown, for instance, by the snoring or " stertorous " breathing which accompanies the unconsciousness of drunkenness. Ordinarily, this, condition passes gradually into a state of deep sleep, after which the patient returns to the normal. In some cases, however, when the quantity of alcohol taken has been verj' large, these vital centres are so gravely affected that the drunkard may die from failure of respiration. Svmptonis due altogether tfi action on central nervous svstem.- All the symptoms of drunkenness, then, are due- essentially to the action of alcohol on the brain and the rest of the nervous system. Even when it kills by acute poisoning it kills through its action on that system, and not by directly injuring any other bodih' organ. This, indeed, we are already prepared to find, for, as we saw when dealing with the action of alcohol on the other functions of the body, it is ov\y when very large doses of the drug are taken that it can produce an immediate effect of anj- significance on these functions : thus, for instance, to^ depress the action of the heart to an appreciable degree by directly influencing that organ, we saw (Chapter VI, p. 75) that alcohol must be present in the blood in a. proportion of about "5 per cent. — a proportion which would imply an original dose, for a man of 10 stone weight, of nearly twelve ounces of absolute alcohol, or well over a pint of whisky at proof strength. With such a dose, of course, in an ordinarj- individual the direct, effect on other organs would be negligible in presence of 87 the symptoms of profound and dangerous intoxication ■of the central nervous system. Relation of symptoms tn amount of alcohol in blood. The next point which we have to consider is how ■drunkenness is related to the amount of alcohol taken, and to the particular sort or sorts of alcoholic beverage ■drunk. We shall deal first with the question of dose. Alcohol gets to the central nervous system by passing from the blood into the fluid — the cerebro- spinal fluid, to give it its technical name — which bathes the brain and the spinal cord as they lie within their lining membranes in the skull and the spine ; and the amount of the drug which enters this cerebro-spinal fluid is strictly pro- portional to the amount contained in the blood. From -experiments on animals and from observations on man, it has been found that the onset and the intensity of the symptoms of intoxication are roughh' dependent on the •quantitj^ of alcohol present in the circulation. Thus in experiments on dogs and horses it was ascertained that the animals began to be slightly affected when the proportion of alcohol in the blood reached the level ■of "12 per cent., that with higher proportions the symptoms became more marked, and that profound stupor frequently ending in death, ensued when the alcohol content rose to 72 per cent.^ Similarly in cases ■of drunkenness in man, the blood has been found to contain, in one observation '153 per cent, of alcohol, and in another instance, when the intoxication was more pronounced, "227 per cent.^ ; and it is generally accepted that with a blood content of over "6 per cent., there is a •considerable likelihood of death.' ' Quoted in Kobert, Lehrbuch der Intoxikationen, Vol. 2, p. 986. Stuttgart, 1906. - Schweisheimer, Deutsches Arcliiv f. Klin. Medizin, Vol. 109, p. 271. 1913. 'As qualifying this statement, see under "Tolerance," p. 91. These figures, however, do not convey much meaning until we have translated them into terms of doses as drunk. This we can easilj" do if we refer back to what was said in Chapter II concerning the absorption of alcohol into the blood and its subsequent fate in the body. It was explained in that chapter (p. 20) that alcohol passes rapidly from the stomach and bowel into the circulation, and that, owing to the slowness with which it is burned or excreted, the amount present in the blood soon reaches a maximum level, bearing a pretty constant relation to the dose originally drunk. So- that, knowing the quantity of absolute alcohol taken and the body weight of the drinker, we can at once give an approximate estimate of the maximum proportion of the drug which will be found in the circulation ; and conversely, we can say what amount of alcohol must be administered to give any particular percentage in the blood. Thus, taking the figures which we have quoted, the proportion of '15 per cent, which was found in the blood in the less pronounced case of intoxication, would correspond to an original dose of i'5 cubic centi- metres of absolute alcohol for each kilogramme of body weight, and this amount, expressed in English measure, would be roughly equivalent, in the case of a man weighing xo stone, to a total dose of 3^ ounces of absolute alcohol, that is to say, nearly a gill and a half of whisky at proof, or rather more than 4 pints of beer of average strength (i.e. containing 4 per cenL absolute alcohol). Lctlial dose of alcohol. Again, if we accept '6 per cent, as being approximately the alcohol content of the blood in dangerous poisoning, we shall put the fatal dose of the drug, for a man of the weight given above, at about 14 ounces of absolute alcohol, or nearly a pint and a half of proof spirit. The quantity of beer which would contain this amount of alcohol would be over two gallons, so that it is easy to understand that fatal cases of acute alcoholic poisoning are not likely to occur from the drinking of a beverage of this dilution. Effect of diltitioii on intoxicating action of alcohol. So far we have been considering the factor of dilution, in its relation to drunkenness, simply as affecting the quantity of alcohol which the drinker consumes, and this, no doubt, is its most important aspect. It has been suggested, indeed, that it may have a wider influence, and that the inebriating action of a given dose of the drug may differ to some extent according as it is taken in a weak or strong solution, but the available evidence gives little support to this view. Any such influence would probably be due largely, if not entirely, to the effect of dilution on the rate at which alcohol is absorbed into the blood, and on the degree — in any case, as we have seen, a ver\' limited degree — to which it is eliminated unchanged from the body. But the most recent experiments^ go to shew that dilu- tion does not appreciably affect the rate of absorption, and though it is true that a free action of the kidneys, which would be promoted by the taking of a large bulk of fluid, may lead to a slightly increased excretion of unchanged alcohol in the urine, this increase is hardly sufficient to have anj' real effect on the concentration of the dug in, the blood. The question, however, is certainly one to which further research might usefully be directed.^ Inebriating action of different alcoholic beverages. And a similar remark may be made with reference to the supposed influence of the various bodies which are asso- ciated with ethylic alcohol in wines and beers and spirits. 'Widmark. Skandinav. Archiv f . Physiologie. Vol.33 p. 85. I916. ^ See footnote to Preface, p. vi. go We are practically quite ignorant as to whether any of these other substances modify the inebriating action of alcohol, either by accelerating or retarding absorp- tion, or in other ways. We do not know, for instance, whether, as is popularly supposed, the drunkenness due to beer differs in type from that due to whisky when doses of equivalent alcoholic strength are given in both cases, and, if there be any difference, we do not know on what it may depend. It has been asserted that the alcohol of beer may be absorbed more slowly owing to the amount of gummy, or " colloid " matters contained in that liquor, but such experimental evidence as exists does not support this view. Mixing of liquors. Similarly we do not know whether there is any foundation in fact for the popular belief that ''mixing liquors," and, more particularly, the combination of spirits with beer, is more likely to cause drunkenness than the taking of an equivalent amount of alcohol in one or other of these drinks alone. Another point on which we need more exact knowledge is with regard to the inebriating effect of the impurities met with in some of the alcoholic beverages, and especially in badly recti- fied spirits. Practical conclusions regarding advantages of the more dilute alcoholic beverages. As our practical conclusion, then, from the evidence at present available, we may say that an)' form of alcoholic liquor can cause drunkenness, if such a quantity of it is taken, at once or within , a short time, as will lead to the presence of the drug in the blood above a certain pro- portion, which in the case of the average healthy adult, may be put provisionally at from '15 to '2 per cent. From the point of view of the prevention of drunkenness, the superiority of the more dilute beverages, such as the lighter beers and natural wines, is therefore mainly due 9i to the fact that the bulk of the fluid makes it difficult for the drinker to consume a very large dose of alcohol with- in a moderate period. This, as we saw when referring to the question of fatal intoxication, is an extremely important superiority. Differences in susceptibility to alcohol. In the preceding remarks we have had in view the aver- age normal drinker — the person who is neither specially susceptible nor specially resistant to the inebriating action of alcohol. But it is notorious that departures from this average standard are very frequent : some people are so constituted that they are affected by quite small quantities of the drug, while others can take large doses without showing any signs of intoxication. Tolerance. Moreover, as a result of continued use, some degree of tolerance can be acquired, so that the habitual drinker is often able to consume, without becoming obviously intoxicated, quantities of liquor which would cause well- marked drunkenness in a person unaccustomed to the drug. Thus, instances have been known where steady drinkers have been in the habit of taking alcohol in daily doses exceeding lo cubic centimetres per kilogramme of body weight, that is to say, for a man of lo stone weight, no less than 23 ounces of absolute alcohol, or well over two pints of whisky at proof, more than enough to kill a neophyte. We know very little as yet about the bodily changes that accompany the development of tolerance. One factor may be the increased capacity of the tissues to burn alcohol, for it has been shewn that when the system has become accustomed to the drug, it can oxidise it much more rapidly, so that after a given dose, the amount present in the blood does not rise to the same level in an habitual drinker, as it would reach in the 92 case of a person who had not acquired tolerance. That this, however, can only be a contributory element is evident from the fact, proved by experiment, that a tolerant animal may show few or no signs of intoxi- cation even when its blood contains enough alcohol to cause well-marked symptoms in an unaccustomed animal.^ It is obvious, .therefore, that when tolerance has become established, either the access of alcohol to the nervous centres is in some way impeded, or, as is more likely, the nerve cells react less readily to its presence; but we do not know anything of the conditions on which this change depends. Tolerance not acquired by all cells. In the case of some other narcotics, as, for instance, morphine, it has been proved that while certain nerve cells acquire tolerance after long use of the drug, others remain as susceptible as at the beginning. This has not been shown to occur in alcohol tolerance, in which it is probable that all nerve cells lose their reaction to the drug in nearly equal measure. But it is certain that, while the nerve Cells of the habitual drinker become tolerant to the presence of alcohol, other body cells are less able to accommodate themselves to it, with the result that tfaey are very liable to be injuriously affected, if the develop- ment of tolerance in the nervous system leads, as it naturally tends to lead, to the taking of larger doses of the drug. That is why drinkers who have, as they would say, strong heads for liquor and who can con^tihie a great deal Of alcohol without becoming drunk, are very likely to suffer in the long run from some of those bodih- disorders we shall presentlj- have to consider in dealing with the subject of chronic alcoholism. And, on the other hand, people who are specialh" susceptible to the intoxicating action of alcohol are to a great extent ' Pringsheim. Biochemisclie Zeitschrift, Vol. 12, pts. i & 1908. 93 protected from excess by that very f^ct. Just because they are so easily made drunk, they escape the more serious evils of chronic poisoning. Hence, from the point of view of general health, the acquisition of toler- ance really implies the removal of a protective mechanism — it allows the drinker to use the drug in doses which he could not have taken if his brain cells had retained their normal power of reacting to it. Special susceptibility to alcohol ; pathological drtuikenness. A further point which should be noted before we leave this question of drunkenness is that under certain con- ditions, as, for instance, after an injur}' to the head or, after an attack of mental disease, a special susceptibility to alcohol may be developed in persons whose reaction to the drug had previously been normal. And this change may also come about in the later stages of chroniq poisoning, especially after an attack of delirium tremens, so that the habitual drunkard, from being more resistant to the influence of alcohol, may become very much less resistant. It is a fact of some importance that this special susceptibility to alcohol, and notably the suscepti: bility which results from injury, or appears in an advanced stage of chronic alcholism, is shown not merely in the readiness with which small doses give rise to mental symptoms, but is also very often manifested in the character of these symptoms, Which may differ more or less widely from the symptoms of ordinary drunketiness. Very usually, for instance, there is in these conditions of " pathological drunkenness," as it is called, a special degree of in^ohement of the highest nei'vous centres— thfe parts of the brain whose activity is related to our conscious life — so that prolonged phases of mental confusion with excitement and impulsiveness are more likely to occut than in the intoxication of normal individuals. For this reason the pathological drunkard, especiallj- when his 94 morbid predisposition is due to chronic alcoholism, is very apt to commit acts of a seriously criminal character. Chronic Alcnholistn. Having said this much about acute poisoning by alcohol, we now pass to consider the question of chronic intoxica- tion. As was pointed out in the introductory section, there may be no relation between the acute symptoms set up by a single excessive dose of a drug, and the chronic disorders that result from slower poisoning by repeated and usually smaller doses of the same drug ; and this is conspicuously the case with alcohol. The symptoms of drunkenness are due to the immediate action of alcohol in the body, an action, the effect of which, as we have seen, is mainly apparent in disturbance of the functions of the brain ; when the drug is eliminated from the system, all trace of this action disappears and the patient returns to his normal state. It is quite otherwise in chronic alcoholism. Here we have to do with changes in the drinker's tissues which may be of a permanent character, and which at all events persist long after the alcohol has been entirely removed from the system ; and these changes are not limited to the nervous centres, but may affect in one way or another most of the organs of the body. Chronic alcoholism due to a generalised devitalising action : its associatiott with other deleterious influences. It would be beyond the scope of this book to give any detailed account of the disorders which are collec- tively described under the term chronic alcoholism. Briefly, it maj- be said that they represent the results of a general devitalising action on the tissues, and that they vary from a relatively moderate impairment of the normal state of the organs to gross morbid changes which ultimately cause death. Of course, the habitual drunkard is exposed during his life to many other 95 deleterious influences besides that of alcoholic excess ; and several of these influences resemble alcohol more or less closely in their mode of action ; so that it may be a matter of considerable difficulty to assess the due im- portance of the alcoholic factor in the causation of the diseased conditions which are found in specially frequent association with intemperance. This fact has not always been adequately recognised in the past, with the result that many morbid effects have been set down to the account of alcoholism which are really due, wholly or in far greater measure, to other causes. The responsibilit}- of syphilis in particular has often escaped recognition in this way, and diseases, such, for instance, as general paralysis of the insane, which we now know to be invariably dependent on syphi- litic infection, were long regarded as capable of being brought about by alcoholic excess. Such con- fusion can occur the more readily because, as pointed out in the recent Report of the Royal Commission on Venereal Diseases, the intemperate drinker, by reason of his lessened power of self-control, is specially prone to expose himself to the risk of contracting these diseases, and, owing to his lowered vitality, is liable to suffer from them in aggravated form. Moreover, the waj- in which alcohol acts in chronic intoxication is complex : its injurious effects are only partially the direct result of the drug entering the system ; they are largely brought about through it.'; detrimental action on the lining membrane of the stomach and bowel, causing chronic catarrh and failure in the action of the digestive juices, and so promoting the absorption from the alimentary canal of poisonous matters (digestive and microbial toxins, as they are termed), which, in health}- conditions of the mucous membrane would be formed in lesser amount, if at all, and would certainly not be allowed to pass so freely into 96 the system. It is, indeed, a question whether many of the morbid changes which occur in other organs in the course of chronic alcoholism may not really be secondary to these local disorders in the gastro-intestinal tract ; for it has been found in some recent experimental inquiries that when alcohol was administered to guinea-pigs, not by the mouth but by inhalation, no visible alterations were produced in the animal's tissues, even when the intoxication was kept up for periods as long as three years — that is to say, during half the average life of an animal of this species — and although its effects were thought to be shown by the frequent occurrence of abnormalities of development in the oiffspring.^ Experimental alcoholism. When these considerations are borne in mind, it will be readily understood that it is very difiRcult to define the limits of chronic alcoholism, and to distinguish its effects from those of the other morbid influences which ma}- be mixed up with it. Efforts have been made to overcome this difiSculty by having recourse to the experimental method ; and numerous investigations have been made to determine whether the continued administration of the drug to rabbits, guinea-pigs and other animals will produce in their tissues morbid changes similar to those found in chronic alcoholism in man. There are, it is true, obvious limitations to this method : its conditions are necessarily very artificial, and moreover, the differences between man and other animals, especially in regard to the development of the brain — the organ on which alcohol exercises its most important action — are enormous, ' so that it is only with considerable reserve that con- clusions drawn from animal experiment can be applied to human beings. Despite such drawbacks, however, these researches have given certain positive results of ' Stockard. American Naturalist, Vol. 47. 1913. Proc. Society of Bxper. Biology and Medicine, Vol. 11. 1914. 97 unquestionable value, particularly with regard to the effect of chronic poisoning on the liver and on the reproductive glands. The bearing of the facts will be best brought out if they are considered in relation with the data derived from the observation of alcoholic diseases in man. Individual variations in stisccptibilitv to chronic intoxication. The first important point to be noted is that the results of laboratory experiment are in full agreement with medical experience in proving the existence of wide individual variations in susceptibility to the injurious effects of alcohol. Just as instances are occasionally met with where persons have drunk what would be generally regarded as dangerously excessive quantities of alcohol for years, and have yet shown no signs of being the worse in health, so likewise in experimental ,1,'esearches it has been found that some animals do not appear to suffer fi'om daily intoxication over long periods of time, while others of the same species succumb after a few weeks. Individuals, whether experimental animals or men, differ also very widely in the susceptibilit}- of the several tissues and organs to the action of the poison ; in one subject, the liver may be specially attacked, in another the nervous system, and so on. Mental disorders in chronic alcoholism , relation af alcoholism to insanitv. Some affection of mind is usually present in chronic alcoholism, but it is commonly moderate in degree, and may be shewn only in weakened will-power and failure of memory. More intense but transitory disorders occur in delirium tremens^ and in an allied condition of mental confusion associated with wide-spread nerve-in- flammation. And in relatively rare instances, alcoholism seems to be responsible for a chronic insanity with persistent delusions, usually of persecution and jealousy. 98 In general, however, the part which alcoholic excess plays in the causation of the ordinary forms of mental disease is of secondary importance : it has been shewn that when the two facts are associated, intemperance is more usually a symptom of insanity than its cause.^ This is conspicuoush- seen in certain cases of periodical (or manic-depressive) insanity, in which, or in one phase of which, the most obtrusive symptom of mental disorder is an intense and overpowering impulse to alcoholic excess. In these relatively infrequent cases, to which the name dipsomania is sometimes given, the patient drinks heavily for a period varying in different cases but usually pretty constant for the same case, and then, when the attack ceases or passes into another phase, he returns to sobriety — a fact, it may be noted incidentally, which goes to shew that alcohol has no very strong habit- forming influence. Morbid cliangcs in nci-vous system in chronic alcoholism. The morbid changes in the nervous system which are found in chronic alcoholism are very variable in character and degree : most commonly there is some thickening of the membranes, with wasting of the nervous tissues, notably in those parts of the brain which are supposed to be specially related to the higher mental activities. These appearances, however, are not distinctive oi alcoholism, nor is it possible to see in them an explana- tion of the nervous disorders of chronic intoxication any more than we can discover, with our present modes of examination, any morbid changes in the brain tissues of the insane which can account for the symptoms of insanitj-. As bearing on this point, it may be noted that the diseased conditions seen in the brain of patients dead of delirium tremens are quite similar to those just referred to as occurring in cases of alcoholism without ' Mott. Archives of Neurology, Vol. 3, p. 424. 1907. 99 such active disorder, and, as in these cases, are neither constant nor very definite. It is evident, therefore, that in chronic intoxication the nervous tissues may be damaged in ways which, while profoundly affecting their intimate constitution and their healthy functioning, are not revealed in any visible structural change. We have already had an intimation of this fact in the development of tolerance, which is also probably dependent on some obscure alteration in the vital activity of the nerve cells. In the affection of the nerves, especially the nerves of the limbs, which occurs occasionally as a result of chronic alcoholic poisoning, more particularly amongst women, and which is technically known as alcoholic peripheral neuritis^ degenerative changes are found in the nerve- fibres, and also not infrequently in the related cells of the spinal cord. In animals, after prolonged intoxication with alcohol, some observers have found alterations in the nervous system, affecting both the nervous, tissues proper and the smaller blood-vessels ; but the meaning of these changes and their dependence on the chronic poisoning are uncertain. Disorders of the digestive system in chronic alcoholism. It is well known that a frequent cause of death in the subjects of chronic alcoholism is cirrhosis of the liver with dropsy. An organ is said to undergo cirrhosis when the fibrous connective tissue is increased and there is an associated degeneration of the cells upon which the functions of the organ depend. Chronic inflammation of the stomach {gastritis) is met with in heavy drinkers, especially in those who indulge in large quantities of spirits. It precedes and is, in a great measure, responsible for the disease of the liver. The chronic irritation and subsequent inflammation of the mucous lining of the stomach sets up degenerative changes in the cells which secrete the gastric juice ; and 16066 H lOO the hydrochloric acid, essential for digestion and the prevention of bacterial fermentation may be completely absent. The blood circulating in the blood vessels of the stomach and intestines is returned into the general circulation via the portal vein of the liver, so that the blood in this organ contains poisons absorbed from the alimentary canal to a greater degree than other organs, e.g. the kidneys. The continuous circulation of these poisons in the liver gives rise to prolonged irritation of the connective tissue supporting the portal vein, its branches and remote ramifications throughout the organ. A chronic inflammation is thus brought about in this tissue, accompanied by a degeneration of the intrinsic cells of the liver, vsrhich at farst is enlarged. This enlarge- ment in many cases persists until fatal dropsy sets in, or the more complete conversion of the chronic inflamma- tory tissue into fibrous tissue may eventually cause such a shrinkage of the organ, that it becomes much smaller than natural, and its surface is so irregular and nodular as to be termed " hobnailed." In experiments on animals a catarrhal inflammation of the mucous membrane has been set up, similar to that which is seen in the stomach of the habitual drunkard. And some recent researches have shewn that changes in the liver corresponding to those of cirrhosis in man, can also be produced in animals hy long-continued intoxication with alcohol.^ Other bodily disorders m chronic intoxication. It is also generally recognized that chronic alcoholism is an important cause of disease in other organs, that it contributes, for instance, to the development of Bright's Kyrle and Schopper. Virchow's Archiv f. patholog. Anatomie, Vol. 215, pp. 309 and 321. 1914. lOI disease of the kidneys, that it helps to bring about the degeneration of the blood-vessels which leads to apoplexy, and that it tends to promote fatty degenera- tion of the heart, as well as being a very important cause of fatty deposit on the surface of that organ. All these diseased conditions, however, are met with also very often in persons who never take alcohol : they may be brought about by many deleterious agencies, operating singly or in combination, and alcoholism is merely one of these agencies. Alcohol and resistance to infective disease. The relation of alcohol to infective disease is of a somewhat similar kind : the chronic poisoning, by devitalising the tissues, lowers the defences of the body against microbial invasion ; consequently, specific germs, such as those which cause pneumonia and tuberculosis, as well as the ordinary microbes of septic inflammation and blood-poisoning, find a suitable soil. A slight general depressing influence — a chill or a local injury — which would have no harmful effect upon a healthy individual, even if micro-organisms were present, because the vital reaction of the living tissue would prevent a general infection, may be most dan- gerous to a chronic alcoholic. It has also been suggested that resistance to infective disease may be prejudicially affected by alcohol when taken in even moderate and occasional doses ; but the experimental researches by which it has been sought to establish this view have not given any clear results. On the other hand there is no evidence for the popular belief which attributes to such doses of alcohol a pro- tective value in cases of exposure to infection. Action of alcohol on the reproductive cells. In the foregoing remarks we have been considering alcoholism merely as affecting the individual : we have I02 now to refer to the wider influence which it may exert on the stock through its action on the reproductive cells. The earlier inquiries regarding the reality and extent of this influence were mainly directed to determining, from statistical material, whether conditions of defective development are or are not specially frequent in the ofi'spring of alcoholic parents. Evidence of this sort, however, even if its accuracy were unquestionable and its indications positive and clear — and this can rarely be said of any of the statistical data used in this controversy — would obviously be open to serious criticism, inasmuch as any excessive incidence of defect in the children of habitual drunkards might be due, not to the influence of parental alcoholism, but to the presence of a syphilitic taint or to an original faultiness in the stock. On this account it is preferable to approach the question from another point of view, and to rely mainly on such data as are obtainable from the s}'stematic examination of the genital glands in alcoholic subjects, and from breeding experiments on animals. Researches in both these directions ha\e been recently carried out, and have given results which, as far as they go, are definite and concordant. The state of the reproductive glands in habitual drunk- ards has been investigated by two independent observers,^ who have shewn that a condition of wasting of the testicles with absence or scanty production of the fertilising cells or spermatozoa, is to be found in the majority of male alcoholics d)ing in the prime of life, while in the female. alterations of a similar character may be discovered in ' Bertholet. L'lnfluence de I'alcoolisnie chronique sur les. organes de rhomtiie, et sur les glandes reprodiictrices eu particu- lier. Ivausanne, 1913. Also comiiiunications by the same author to the luteruatioual Congresses on Alcoholism at I/Ondon (1909), and at The Hague (1911). Weichselbaum. Verhandl. der deutsch. pathol. Gesellschaft, Jena, 1910. X03 the ovaries. Further, it has been shewn by experiments •on rabbits that, by the continued administration of the drug, morbid changes can be produced in the genital glands of these animals of the same type as those which have been observed in chronic alcoholism in human beings. Effect of parental alcoholism, on offspring. The fact that these glands are so frequently and so markedly affected in conditions of chronic intoxication gives some indirect support to the view that parental alcoholism may react injuriously on the vitality and normal development of the offspring. And recent re- search has furnished very important direct evidence to the same effect. It has been shewn by Stockard,i in a series of experiments in which alcohol was administered to guinea-pigs by inhalation, that the offspring of the alcoholised animals, in comparison with the young of control animals of the same stock, were conspicuously inferior in strength and vitality, and in many instances presented gross abnormalities of organisation. These bad effects were transmitted through several generations, and were, indeed, more pronounced in the later genera- tions, than in the immediate offspring of the alco- holised subjects. An important point brought out by Stockard is that the injurious influence of the intoxica- tion may be manifest in the offspring even when the genital glands appear to be quite healthy, thus shewing that, although there are no visible structural changes in the germ cells, they have been modified to an extent suf&cient to cause them to give rise to defective embryos ■or weakened individuals who die shortly after birth. These observations and experiments would thus appear to indicate that parental alcoholism may have a seriously ' Stockard, loc. at. Stockard and Papanicolaou, American Naturalist, Vol. 50, 1916. 104 detrimental influence on the stock ; and if the results are confirmed by further investigation, it will be reasonable to conclude that this is probably one of the most important modes in which intemperance threatens the health and well-being of the community. But, in view of the extreme importance of this conclusion, it is clearly desirable to suspend judgment until the work has been controlled by other inquirers. Causation of chronic alcoholism. After this brief review of the disorders of chronic alcoholism, the next question to be considered is, under what conditions do these disorders come about? What mode and degree of excess are liable to lead to chronic poisoning ? In endeavouring to answer this question we have not at our disposal any direct evidence comparable to what we had when discussing drunken- ness ; and we must, therefore, be content, at all events for the moment, with such indirect indications as can be gathered from a general survey- of the facts. Continuity of action and excess of dose nccessaty factors. The main conclusion that we arrive at in this way is, that there are two essential factors in the causation of chronic alcoholism : first, the drug must be taken in sufiicient quantity to exercise an injurious action on the tissues, and second, that action must be more or less continuous. Excess of dose without continuity of action does not give rise to persistent tissue changes ; as we have already seen, an isolated bout of drunkenness does not leave any lasting after-effects. Again, the regular use of alcoholic beverages does not induce chronic poisoning so long as only moderate doses are drunk ; this is a matter of common experience, and we can see evidence of it on a large scale in the absence, or extreme rarity, of chronic alcoholism in communities where, as I05 in the non-industrial populations of Southern France, Spain, and Italy, alcohol is part of the ordinary dietary, but is drunk in moderation and in the form of natural wines of low strength. Conditions which promote chronic alcoholism. In the absence of exact data, we cannot safely go beyond these general propositions, and we shall not, therefore, attempt to make any estimate as to how much alcohol there must be in the blood, and how long its action must be kept up, to exercise a detrimental effect on the vitality of the tissues. For practical purposes it will be sufficient if we endeavour to distinguish the conditions which cause alcoholic beverages to be taken in such excess, with regard to amount and frequency of dose, as appears from actual experience to involve more or less injury to the drinker's health. In a general view, we may summarise the more important of these con- ditions as follows. (i) Use of the stronger alcoholic beverages. — When liquors of high alcoholic strength are drunk, there is an ante- cedent probabilitj' that an excessive dose of alcohol may be more readily taken. The man who drinks a light beer must consume some pints of fluid before he gets as much alcohol into his blood as the spirit drinker would absorb from a glass of neat whisky at proof. And that, no doubt, is one reason why drunkenness appears to be relatively more frequent in spirit drinkers than in beer drinkers, though the difference is probably less pro- nounced than is commonly supposed. In a recent inquiry directed to this point, it was ascertained that amongst 1,505 persons (1,032 males and 473 females) charged with drunkenness, in I,ondon and 14 other large cities in England during a period of three weeks, intoxication io6 was attributed to beer alone in 45*4 per cent, of the cases occurring in men, to spirits alone in 42^0 per cent., and to beer and spirits together in io"8 per cent., whilst amongst women the proportions were, beer alone, 37'6 per cent., spirits alone, 49*0 per cent., and beer and spirits together, 8"6 per cent. Inasmuch as the amount of alcohol taken in the form of spirits in the whole United Kingdom is only one-third to one-quarter that which is consumed in beer, it is legitimate to conclude from these figures, drawn from the predo- minantly beer-drinking part of the country South of the Tweed, that there is in fact a greater likelihood of immediate excess when the stronger alcoholic bever- ages are used. The statistical information with regard to the relative importance of the several sorts of liquor in the causation of chronic alcoholism is very scanty, but, as far as it goes, it points even more clearly in the same direction, and is thus confirmatory of the general medical belief that spirit drinking is specially liable to induce alcoholic disease. Apart from the greater risk of excess of dose when highly alcoholic beverages are drunk, the use of these more concentrated liquors tends, as we have already seen, to promote chronic poisoning, on account of the damage done to the lining membrance of the stomach by the direct action of alcohol in strong solution. (ii) Frequent repetition nf dose. — It takes some hours before even a moderate dose of alcohol is completely eliminated from the system ; so that if a fresh quantity of liquor is drunk during this period when the blood still contains a significant percentage of the original dose, the alcohol level in the circulation will rise again. And if further doses continvie to be taken at similar intervals, the action of the drug in the body will be kept up persistently. This is what happens, for instance, in the case of industrial workers who have frequent recourse to I07 alcohol during the hours of labour, as, e.g.^ dock labourers, butchers, etc. : owing to this frequent repetition of doses, which, taken individually, may be quite moderate, but which by their addition maintain a considerable con- centration of the drug in the blood, drinkers of this class, industrial drinkers as we may term them for brevity's sake, are peculiarly apt to suffer from the chronic effects of alcohol. In this respect, they contrast very strikingly with men engaged in such occupations as coal-mining. The miner's task involves muscular effort which in its character and degree might appear a priori to be equally favourable to the use of alcoholic beverages as an element in the working diet. The conditions, however, under which the work is carried on are such as preclude altogether the possibility of taking alcohol during the hours of labour ; and amongst coal-miners, accordingly, industrial drinking is practically unknown, and alcoholic disease is exceptionally infrequent. Thus, if we refer to the statistics of occupational mortality published by the Registrar-General (vide appendix 3), we see that, while in the case of dock labourers the "comparative mortality figure " for alcoholism is more than three times that for the general male population, the corresponding figure for coal-miners is less than half the same standard, and is, in fact, one of the lowest on the list. There is no reason to suppose that this contrast is due to any difference in general sobriety between the two groups in question — the incidence of drunkenness is very similar upon mining districts and upon seaports — so that it seems more reasonable to attribute it mainly to the prevalence of continuous or quasi-continuous drinking amongst dock labourers, and to the absence of that type of drinking amongst the coal-miners. The inference to be drawn from these facts is, there- fore, that, in order to prevent the development of chronic intoxication, it is essential that alcohol should io8 not be taken in such frequentl}' repeated doses as will keep up a constant excessive action in the system- — that is to say, that intervals should elapse between the times when alcoholic liquors are drunk, such that the average concentration of the drug in the blood remains well below the point at which there is any risk of injury to the tissues. (iii) Dtinking zvithou food. — Food, when taken with alcohol, dilutes it, and so decreases the irritant action of strong alcoholic beverages upon the lining membrane of the stomach ; the lack of this protective influence is probably an important factor in the greater suscep- tibility of the industrial drinker, who commonly takes much of his alcohol on an empty stomach, par- ticularly the early morning dram, or " livener," with which it is customary in some districts to start the day's work, (iv) Sedentary Occupation. — It is popularly supposed that intemperate persons of sedentary habits are more liable to suffer from the effects of alcoholic excess than drinkers engaged in active outdoor accupations. And to a certain, though very limited extent, this view is supported by what we know of the conditions which may modify the oxidation and elimination of the drug. Thus we have seen that vigorous muscular exertion causes alcohol to be burned rather more quickly in the body, and also increases the proportion which passes out unchanged in the breath. The effect produced in either of these ways is not, however, so considerable that it can to an important degree protect the drinker from the injurious results of the habitual use of the drug in excessive and frequently repeated doses ; and, in fact, as shewn by the statistics of occupational mortality already referred to, the death-rate from presumably alco- holic disease is specially high in several of these industries log which involve hard muscular labour in the open air. Thus the theoretical advantage which this active sort of work confers on the drinker, in accelerating the removal of alcohol from his system, is in practice much more than counterbalanced by the tendency, which such work creates, to the continued and excessive use of the drug as a supposed aid to labour. And from this point of view the risk of chronic intoxication is in some sort enhanced by what would appear prima facie to be a desirable quality in alcohol, viz., its food value. As we have already seen, it can be used to furnish as much as one-fifth of the total amount of the energy required by the body, so that the industrial drinker, over and above the relief from the sensations of fatigue which the drug gives him, obtains in his alcoholic liquor a supply of fuel food which enables him to reduce substantially his consumption of other forms of nutriment. The fact, therefore, that alcohol has a high fuel value, tends in an important measure to promote its habitual use as a food for very laborious work in substitution for other foods, and as, unlike these other foods, it is not stored in inert form in the body, but remains unaltered in the tissues, it can exercise for some time a deleterious influence on their vitality. Habit-forming influence of alcohol. To complete this examination of the factors which contribute to the development of chronic intoxication, it is necessary to say something of habit in relation to alcohol. A number of drugs, especially drugs of narcotic action, when they have been taken repeatedly for a sufficient length of time, tend to create in the subject a need or "craving" for their continued use; such drugs are described as habit-forming, and the condition existing when the need or " craving " has become established, is called the drug-habit. It is to^be noted that a drug- habit. no in this special sense of the term, means a good deal more than the habit of taking a particular drug : thus, for example, a person who suffers from some chronic or frequently recurring disorder which is relieved by a specific medicine, will be likely to use that medicine regularly, but he will not be considered, on that account alone, to have contracted a drug-habit ; his need of the drug is due to the persistence of the cause which Jed him to resort to its use in the first instance, it has not been created by the action of the drug on his system. Neither can we properly bring into the category of drug-habits, in the narrower meaning in which the term is here employed, those instances where the con- tinued use of a drug has given rise to a sense of need, solely through that general characteristic of our nervous organisation in accordance with which we tend to repeat actions that we have become accustomed to perfoini. It is true that this influence does enter to some extent into the formation of true drug-habits, as it necessarily must in the case of all behaviour which has become ■customary and agreeable ; but it is obviously a factor of different nature from the specific disposition which is ■created by the continued action of the drug, and which gives to the drug craving its distinctive character. Factors which may contribute to the formation of the alcohol habit. In the development of the alcohol habit, all these factors play their parts. The habitual drinker takes alcohol primarily for the sake of the agreeable effects produced through the nervous system, because he finds that it relieves the sensations of fatigue, and because he believes that it gives him increased energy for his work ; and the return or persistence of these motives un- doubtedly contributes to the regular use of alcohol. Similarly, some persons are said to " fly to drink" when Ill they are worried or in pain. In these instances alcohol is taken with the definite object of obtaining relief from some disability. In other instances the chief factor in drinking seems to be an attitude of mind which is comparable with that induced by the continued repetition of any other act which becomes habitual in the ordinary sense. Such is the case of the drunkard who cannot pass his usual public-house or meet his ordinary boon com- panions without having a glass ; his conduct may be largely an instance of habit, and his difficulty in breaking it off may be scarcely greater than that of the loafer in deserting his usual street corner. But while these factors undoubtedly play an important part in the development of habitual drinking, there can be no question that the continued abuse of alcohol leads in many instances to changes in the mental and bodily functions which create a need or craving for alcohol. The depression which is present during the recovery from one drinking bout may be relieved by further indulgence and thus lead to the continued use of the drug. The most striking examples of craving are seen in secret drinkers, in whom the abuse of alcohol is somewhat similar to that of morphine in morphine-takers. Tolerance and abstinence symptoms in driig--habits. In the case of some habit-forming drugs, the drug gradually loses its power and no longer induces the action which followed its first administration. The best example of this is offered by morphine, the most active ingredient of opium. The morphine taker, or morphino- maniac, through the habitual use of the drug, becomes less and less susceptible to its immediate eff'ects, that is to say, he acquires tolerance with regard to it, and, by reason of this tolerance, he tends constantly to in- crease his regular dose. Further, when he is deprived of his usual allowance, he experiences acute discomfort, and, if the drug is suddenly and completely cut oft, he sometimes presents symptoms of serious collapse which may even end fatally if morphine is not administered to him. Tolerance and abstinence symptoms in drinkers. The development of tolerance, then, and liabilit}' to abstinence symptoms are often the accompaniments of the true drug-habit. The degree, however, to which they may be present varies very widely in the case of difierent drugs. How does the matter stand with regard to alcohol ? As we have already seen, the continued use of alcohol produces tolerance, though never to the extent that may be reached in the morphine habit. And alcohol also may induce such a condition in the drinker that its complete and sudden withdrawal will give rise to a feeling of urgent need, or " craving," for the drug, which is usually regarded as, and probably is, at least in part, a genuine abstinence symptom. Though very lurid ac- counts of this craving are sometimes given by neurotic drinkers, it would appear that ordinarily the feeling, in so far as it really is such a symptom, is vers- much weaker than the craving which habitual users of other drugs experience as a result of abstinence. Thus, drunkards in prisons and other institutions where liquor is not obtainable, complain much less of discomfort from the lack of alcohol than from that due to the prohibition of smoking. Delirium Tremens not due to abstinence. It has also been suggested that abstinence symptoms of a more definite and graver sort may occur in alcoholic subjects, and, more particularly, that the abrupt cutting off of the drug in the case of a heavy drinker, may precipitate an attack of delirium tremens. This has been disputed, however, by many authorities, and several arguments have been brought against it. Thus, delirium ^^3 tremens is of very rare occurrence in institutions where, as in the majority of inebriate retreats, it is the regular practice to stop all alcoholic liquor immediately on the patient's admission. Further, when the disease has developed, its course is not affected by the administration of alcohol, whereas the abstinence symptoms resulting from the suppression of other diug-habits, as, e.g., the morphine habit, are relieved at once when a dose of the drug is given. Finally, recent investigation has shewn that in many cases of delirium tremens, the fluid which bathes the brain and spinal cord — the cerebro-spinal fluid — contains appreciable amounts of alcohol, so that evidently its presence in the nervous system does not prevent the onset of an attack^. Relatively slight habit-forming tendency of alcohol. Alcohol thus diff'ers from such habit-forming drugs as opium and morphine in the fact that its sudden with- drawal causes less severe symptoms, and that no high degree of tolerance is acquired by its prolonged use. But these are diff'erences in degree rather than in kind. And it is to be added that our experience of morphine- takers in this country is limited to severe cases com- parable to the confirmed drunkard. In each case the habit of abuse may in part arise from the persistence or recurrence of the conditions and circumstances which originally led to recourse to the drug. But the habit of excess once formed tends to become stronger through the enfeeblement of the will which results from the continued action on the central nervous system and lessens the power to resist the invitation of the narcotic. ' Demole. I/'Eiicephale, January, 1914. "4 CHAPTER IX. Alcohol and Longevity. Does " moderate " drinking affect the average duration ot life ? Thus far we have examined the subject in the light of direct experimental, pathological or clinical researches, and our task has not differed in kind from that of any writers on physiological or medical questions. But a discussion of the properties of alcohol is more or less intimately related to that of various sociological and economic problems the elements of which can rarely be subjected to the process of isolation and intensive study which is the essence of experimental science. Hence arises the appeal to other kinds of evidence. In popular discussions a good deal of use has, for instance, been made of statistics. A proportion of such statistics refers to matters which do not come within the scope of our inquiry, such for instance as alcohol and crime ; but something remains which we are called upon to examine. To see how this happens, we may recur to a point made in our introduction, viz., the existence of loosely defined or undefined notions respecting the value of alcohol as an habitual beverage. Many people think that a daily glass of beer, wine or spirits "does them good" ; others are not less confident that the use of such substances does harm. Some of the implications of these statements have already been noticed, but one remains for individual attention. This is the belief that "moderate" drinking shortens (or lengthens) life on the average. Every reader of these pages must have seen a para- graph describing an interview with some aged man (by preference a centenarian) where the person interviewed attributes his length of days to (a) total abstinence from alcohol or tobacco, or (b) absolute regularity in the 115 daily consumption of — say — one pint of beer and one pipe of tobacco. . Again, beyond this anecdotal substitute for scientific evidence, one finds statements describing the heavy mortality suffered by, let us say, publicans and inn servants ; the inference, whether expressed or implied, being that they have shortened their lives by the abuse of alcohol; or, on the other hand, we see quoted the low death rates of the clergy, it again being suggested that the large proportion of total abstainers from alcohol to be found in this class is responsible for the advantage enjoyed. The wide currenc)' of such statements and implications makes it incumbent upon us to inquire whether there really exists any evidence upon which scientific conclusions can be founded with reference to the relation between a habit of consuming moderate quantities of alcohol and longevity. Nature of available evidence. The extreme difiiculty of the subject can be best displayed by considering the kind of data that v/ould need to be provided for a solution on the experimental lines of the work used as a basis for the earlier chapters of this book. In effect we should require to have at our disposal a number of men or women of like ages, similar ancestry, occupations and civil condition. We should divide them at random into two groups, the members of one group would receive a fixed quantity of alcohol daily, and the fates of all would be recorded from the time of beginning the experiment until everyone was dead. Naturally no such experiment has been carried out, and we are obliged to fall back upon such partial substitutes as the ordinary chances of society have placed at our disposal. We shall not weary the reader with a description of various admittedly imperfect or quite unsuccessful attempts that have been made to find such i6o66 I ii6 substitutes, but pass at once to the efforts which may at kast be regarded as partially successful. Among siich, certain statistics of insurance companies deserve mention. As everybody knows, nearly all insurance companies take special pains to learn the state of health and habits of persons whose proposals are entertained, while there are some which classify separately policy holders who are or are not total abstainers from alcohol. An important, example is the United Kihgdom Temperance and General Provident Institution, and its experience has been often quoted in the present connection. The scientific papers dealing with this special case are cited in the footnote and must be examined by a reader who wishes to master the somewhat technical problems which can here only be sketched in outline.^ Experience of the United Kingdom Temperance and General Provident Institution. This office accepts, at the same premium, abstainers from alcohol and non-abstainers, but, after acceptance, the records of the two classes are kept apart, bonuses being declared separately for each class in accordance with its mortality experience. This is to say, that no immediate inducement is held out to entrants who are abstainers ; if they do derive any advantage in the long run, it will be because their average longevity is greater, so that their section of the office earns larger bonuses. Now among males insured by this company (whole life policies) it was found that 26-4 per cent, fewer deaths occurred among the abstainers than would have taken place had they died at the rate observed in the class of non-abstainers. This great difference does not mean that the non -abstainers were bad lives ; as a matter of fact, ' Moore, Journ. Institute of Actuaries, XXXVIII, 213. Lidstone, ibid. p. 273, 1904. Andrae, Berichte d. sten. Inter- nationale!] Kongresses f. Versicherungs-Wissenschaft, p. 491, 1906. ir/ their mortality was almost the same as experienced by the clients of the 23 British Offices whose pooled experience over the years 1 863-1 893 formed the basis of a widely used table of mortality for insurance purposes (that is, a table shewing the chance of a person dying at various ages). To use a statistical average which is, possibly, more vivid than a mere comparison of per- centages of deaths, we may take the expectation of life at age 30, that is, the average number of years lived by persons after the thirtieth birthday. According to the experience of the total abstainers, this is, on the average, a little more than 39 years, while for the non-abstainers it is some 35 years less ; indeed only one life office, viz., the Clergy Mutual, has recorded an expectation of life superior to that of the Total Abstainers of the United Kingdom Temperance and General Office. Thus far we have considered only males having ordinary whole life policies. The experience of the office with women was not so extensive and the difference between the two classes, abstainers and non-abstainers, although in favour of the former, was not significantly so ; they had only 3 per. cent, fewer deaths than their fellows (all these comparisons naturallj- allow for differences of age). The data for Endowment Policies (the popular modern form of insurance in which the sum assured is paj'able at a •certain age or at death, whichever conies first) shewed a great advantage among the abstainers (i8'2 per cent, fewer deaths than expected) but not so great an ad- vantage as seen in the whole life policies. Difficulties in interpretation of the experience of the United Kingdom Tempei'ance Institution. There is evidently no reasonable doubt that the abstainers insured in this office do, on the average, live longer than the non-abstainers, but it would not be scientific to attribute the result solely to the fact of ii8 abstinence from alcohol without further analysis. In- surance statistics have to be interpreted with great care, owing to the influence of selection and collateral social conditions. Thus, it has been pointed out that the number of policies in force in the abstainers' section of this office increased by 29 per cent, between 1882 and 1902, while the number of non-abstainers' policies diminished in the same time by 23 per cent. The con- sequence was that the experience of the abstainers was necessarily somewhat more favourable than that of the non-abstainers from an assurance standpoint, for two reasons : (i) It contained a larger proportion of recentlj' selected lives, i.e. persons who had been more recently medically examined ; (2) these persons lived during a period of lower general mortality, viz., after rather than before 1882. It is of course obvious that a man aged 35 is more likely to die within a year if he had insured when 25 than if he had been accepted by the insurance doctor when 34 years old, since he has had time to fall a victim to a disease which the doctor will detect at his examination, but could not predict several years earlier. Again, we know that the expectation of life for all classes has improved with time, in consequence of new medical discoveries which benefit all, and of our modern higher standards of public and personal hygiene. It is not possible to measure the effect of this difference, but it is proper to state that when the statistics were limited to the period ending in 1882 (the whole data published relate to the years from 1848 to 1901), the abstainers still had an advantage, although for that period, the above mentioned factors should rather tell against them, because during the first eight years of its existence the office only accepted abstainers, so that there would have been proportionally more new policies among the nou-abstainers. Another criticism from the insurance standpoint is 119 directed to two peculiarities in the experience of this oflSce. The first is that the ratio of death rates, of males to death rate of females (abstainers) is altogether unlike that found in other offices, the female mortality, being proportionally, much too high. The second is that the mortality experience of the abstainers holding endow- ment policies was not appreciably better than that of the abstainers holding whole life policies. Both these facts have suggested to a well-known continental actuary (Andrae) that the abstainers were, at the time they insured, " select " lives.. Thus, the proportion subsisting between male and female rates of mortality must be chiefly due to the differences of liability to certain diseases and accidents dependent upon sex itself and to secondary sexual consequences, such as occupation, general habits, etc. It is not easy to see why this •proportion should be changed if both men and women abstain from alcohol. Again, all other offices have found that persons who take endowment policies die at lower rates, on the average, than do those who take life- long policies. The usual explanation given is that self selection is important. Anyone who thinks either because many of his relations died young, or perhaps because of some private habits or intentions which do not come within the scope of an insurance company's inquiries, that he will not live long, is not likely to pay the higher premium required for an endowment policy, so that the endowment policy holders are twice selected. Naturally we cannot think that abstainers have less powers of self selection than non-abstainers, so, in the opinion of the critic, we must conclude that the whole life policy holders were already a select class when they insured, and that their lower rate of mortality may not have had anything to do with their lifelong abstinence from alcohol. It would be exceeding the limits we have assigned to 120 this statement were we to discuss the theoretical argu- ments in support or in rebuttal of this criticism. It does not seem practicable to apply any objective test which could give a decisive answer, and therefore we cannot regard the experience of the office as conclusive. Possibility that " non- abstaining " group may include some heavy drinkers. There are two other criticisms the force of which can- not be fully estimated. There is no doubt at all that excessive indulgence in alcohol does very greatly shorten life. But in any large group of persons who are not total abstainers there must needs be found, in the long run, a proportion, perhaps a very small proportion, but still some, giving way to circumstances and becoming seriously intemperate. Such persons will, on the average, die below the normal age, and their unfavour- able mortality experience must go to swell the balance of deaths in the general section. It is, of course, impossible to measure the significance of this fact, but in the present state of knowledge, we cannot neglect it without begging the question which we really wish to answer, viz., whether moderate drinking shortens life or not ? Risks of venereal disease and accident in non-abstainers. Also we must bear in mind the conclusion of the Royal Commission on Venereal Diseases, that the abuse of alcohol pla)s an important role in the spread of venereal disease. A single lapse due to the loss of self-control consequent upon convivial excess may lead to infection with disease which shortens life greatly, and such cases would also help to explain the difference between life prospects of abstainers and moderate drinkers. A like remark applies to various forms of violent death, as in street accidents. 121 Mortality of persons engaged in the liquor trade. We have now examined the evidence available as to the mortality experience of total abstainers compared with ordinary life office experience, and must say a few words, of those whose employment brings them into close contact with alcohol. It is, however, unnecessary for us to devote much space to this, because most readers will be acquainted with the figures of the Registrar- General, often quoted in the press, shewing the heavy mortality experienced in the liquor trades.^ According to the published statistics of a large insurance company (the Prudential) the expectation of life of insured persons belonging to the liquor trades is at age 30 as much as 15 years less than that of the abstainers noticed above, and, indeed, many years short of the average for all males as computed in the Registrar-General's life tables for England and Wales.^ The data of the Actuarial Society of America' also shew that persons employed in liquor saloons die at very high rates. It was found that proprietors, managers and superintendents of liquor establishments who did not attend the bar, experienced a mortality which was 135 per cent, of the standard rate ; when they did attend the bar, the per- centage rose to 178 per cent., while the death rate from cirrhosis of the liver was sixfold the normal rate. These figures, of course, confirm the view, which per- haps hardly needed statistical confirmation, that excessive opportunities for indulgence, on the principle "how oft the sight of means to do ill deeds makes ill deeds done," will be attended on the average with special risks to life and health. But the statistics do not tell ' See Appendix 3. * McDonald, Berichte d. 5 ten. Intern. Kong. f. Versicherungs- Wissenschaft, p. 517. 1906. ' Medico-Actuarial Investigation of Association of Life Assur- ance Medical Directors and the Actuarial Society of America. 3 Vols. New York, 1912-13. 122 US much more ; we cannot infer from them that a publican who drinks moderately will die sooner than another man of the same age ; indeed, even if we could infer this, we should still have to consider whether the unhygienic surroundings, poor accommoda- tion and excessive hours of labour of many liquor estab- lishments rather than the drinking of alcohol might not chiefly contribute to the result Sickness and mortality of abstainers and non-abstainers ^" certain Friendly Societies. We next turn to the statistical experience of certain large Friendly Societies, reported upon nearly 30 years ago by Mr. F. G. P. Neison.i Comparing the Rechabites {a society of total abstainers) with the Oddfellows and Foresters, a difference very similar to that found in the experience of the United Kingdom Temperance and General Provident Institution is seen. The Rech- abites had only 69 per cent, of the deaths calculated on the basis of the Foresters' experience, and their expectation of life at age 30 was more than 4 years longer than that of the Foresters. This comparison, however, probably overstates the advantage, as the Rechabites' statistics cover the period 1878-1888, while the Foresters' data were for 1871-5 and the Odd- fellows' 1866-70. With regard to sickness, the aver- age weeks of sickness claim per member were higher among the Rechabites up to age groups 40-50, but lower for ages 50-60 and 60-70 than in the non- abstaining societies. The average numbers of weeks of sickness, not per member, but per member sick, were about the same among Foresters and Rech- abites, and the actuary concluded that the higher ' F. G. P. Neison. — The Rates of Mortality and Sickness accord- ing to the experience for the ten years 1878-87 of the Independent Order of Rechabites (Salford Unity) Friendly Society. Lon- don, 1889, :general rates at earlier ages among the Rechabites were due to less perfect medical examination at ■entrance, his reason being that new entrants shewed a much higher percentage of sickness claims than members of the same age but longer standing in the society. This, which is contrary to the usual experience of Friendly Societies, pointed, in his opinion, to less ■careful selection of entrants. As a whole these results concord with the life office experience detailed above. The obvious criticisms to which they are subject are (i) the Rechabites were a very much smaller society than either of the non-abstaining societies ; both the Oddfellows and the Foresters' sta- tistics covered in each case more than ten times as man)- years of life and more than fifteen times as many deaths, as the Rechabites, while the comparison does not relate to the same calendar years. (2) Both occupational and geographical distribution may be important factors of mortality, especially in industrial populations, and neither could be analysed. Conclusions. The observations we have offered and the illustrations -quoted indicate the necessity of being cautious in draw- ing deductions in respect of the relation obtaining between the consumption of alcoholic beverages and longevity. We think, however, that it is legitimate to •conclude : — (i.) That the death-rate is lower and the expectation of life longer in the section of an Insurance Society admitting only total abstainers from alcohol than in the section admitting those who are not abstainers from alcohol. (ii.) That the statistics of mortality are more favour- able for abstainers than are the returns for other Societies making no such distinctions, and 124 which issue policies chiefly to persons of a more prosperous social class.^ (iii.) That Friendly Society data are concordant with those of the I^ife Insurance Offices in thisrespect. (iv.) That the official statistics issued by the Registrar - General indicate an exceptionally heavy mortality among persons occupied in the liquor trade. It should be added that it does not follow from these conclusions that a consistently moderate use of alcoholic beverages shortens life, since we cannot sort out from the data brought into comparison those instances in which the habitual abuse of alcohol produces disease or deaths or in which accidental abuse has a like consequence, nor can we exclude many other social factors operating for or against longevity. Our duty is merely to assess the value of the evidence dispassionately. Anyone is entitled to argue as follows : Since it is admittedly proved that in a comparison extending over many years and embracing different social classes, non-abstainers do not live as long on the average as abstainers, therefore, for the practical conduct of life a person desiring to live more than the average term of years will do well to become a total abstainer. This is quite possibly a sound rule of conduct, but what the impartial student has to keep in mind is that the con- nection between the postulated "cause," taking or not- taking alcohol, and the supposed " effect," an average longer or shorter life, has not been displayed with the clearness and freedom from disturbing complex social or personal factors, in addition to the consumption of alcohol, which would entitle it to rank as a scientifically established conclusion. ' The validity of the argument has been impugned on technical grounds, the practical importance of which cannot be determined.. 125 CHAPTER X. Conclusions. We have now completed our survey of the present state of scientific knowledge regarding alcohol, considered in its several aspects as a food, a drug and a poison. It remains to sum up in this final chapter the main facts which we have ascertained, and to draw from them such inferences as have a practical bearing on the use of alcoholic beverages under the conditions of everyday life. Main action of alcohol confined to nervous system. The first conclusion to which this review of the evidence leads us is, that, apart from the results of its continued excessive use, the main effects of alcohol that have any real significance are due to its action on the nervous system. As we have seen in the chapters deal- ing with these several points, the result of scientific research concerning the action of alcohol on the respi- ration, the circulation, the digestion, the muscular system, is to shew that, so far as direct action is concerned, alcohol, when administered in moderate doses, in dilute form and at sufficient intervals, has no efiect of anj- serious and practical account. Action of alcohol narcotic and not stimulant. -- A further conclusion of capital importance which emerges with equal clearness is that the action of alcohol on the nervous system is essentially sedative, and — with the possible exception of its direct influence on the respiratory centre — is not truly stimulant. The popular belief in the stimulatiug properties of alcohol, as regards nervous and other functions, seems to be of purely sub- jective origin and illusory; it is in the main, if not wholly, an effect of the narcotic influence of the drug, for, as we 126 have seen, it dulls the drinker's perception of unpleasant •conditions in himself and his surroundings, and may make him feel better, more efficient and stronger than he really is. Effects which are apparently of a different character — ■effects such as the increased loquacity and freer gesticu- lation produced by alcohol — which are usually interpreted by the drinker, if not by those about him, as evidences of stimulation, are really narcotic effects ; they result from the removal of the control exercised by the higher nervous centres, a control which enables the individual to weigh his words and acts, and renders him self-critical and solicitous as to the impression he is making on other people. The removal of this control and the decrease of critical self-consciousness are, in fact, the most constant and characteristic effects of alcohol, whether in the moder- ate dose which diminishes restraint and imparts courage to the diffident speaker or in the excessive dose which gives rise to the verbose diffuseness of drunkenness. We may see a further illustration of the true nature of the drug action of alcohol in its seemingly contradictory effects on persons of different temperament ■^hen com- peting in games of skill in which temperament is an important factor. The cool and self -controlled player will generally find that even a small or moderate amount of alcohol will impair the accuracy of his play, while, on the other hand, it may improve the performance of the excessively nervous player by decreasing his over-anxiety and self-consciousness. Objective effects of drug on nervous system. In reality, so far as the effects of the drug on the nervous system are objectively perceptible, they are always manifested in a lowering of functional activity. We have seen that this effect is appreciable in the case of the simplest reflex actions, but that it acquires increasing 127 prominence with increasing complexit}- of the response chosen for the test; so that a dose which produces no obvious change in the performance of a simple act, involving no skill or exact adjustment of movement, will definitely impair the precision of an act demanding a greater delicacy of control and co-ordination. When nmxotic action may be useful. The conclusion indicated by our review of the scientific evidence, that alcohol is essentially a narcotic, does not imply an adverse judgment as to its value in all circum- stances. There is nothing intrinsically good about a stimulant, and nothing intrinsically bad about a narcotic. Each has its proper utility under given conditions. But these conditions are diflferent for each, and on that account it is important that there should be no confusion as to whether a drug belongs to one category or the other. When stimulation of nervous function is really needed — when the individual has to meet an emergency which calls for the exercise of his highest powers of perception and judgment — alcohol is not merely useless, it is certainly and unequivocally detrimental. On the other hand, there are emergencies where, though the individual may also imagine that he needs to be braced up nervously, he would in fact be assisted far more by a relaxation than by an increase of tension, and here the sedative action of alcohol, so far as the immediate effect is concerned, may be advantageous. The value widely attributed to the rum ration, under the conditions of acute discomfort, cold and nervous strain inseparable from trench warfare, may be explained in this way. It is in this way too that alcohol helps the timid speaker to overcome his difiSdence, and to free himself from the inhibitions that prevent him from finding his words. Similarly, an exhausting and worrying day may leave a condition of tension and irritability which 128 interferes with appetite and digestion ; and in such a con- ., action, when beneficial ... ... ... ... ... 127 Nervous system in chronic alcoholism 98 Nervous system, action of alcohol mainly on 12.") Nutrition, no special action of alcohol on 26 Occupation, in relation to use of alcohol 108 Occupational groups, mortality from alcoholism in Appendix 3 Oseretzkowsky 48 Oxidation, definition of 7 „ of alcohol in body 21 Pancreatic secretion ... ... ... ... ... ... 58 „ ferment 60- Poison action, defined 15 „ „ of alcohol ..'. 85 Poisoning, acute and chronic 15, 85 Precautions in use of alcoholic beverages 132 Proof spirit, definition of Appendix 1 Proteins ... 9 „ sparing of, by alcohol 24 Pulse, effect of alcohol on 72 ,, reflex quickening of 73 Refiexacts ... 42 „ „ effect of alcohol on 43- Reproductive cells, action of alcohol on 101 Respiration 64 Respiratory activity, indirect effect of alcohol on 65 „ centre 64 „ „ alleged stimulation of, by alcohol 67 ,, ., paralysis of, in alcoholic poisoning ... 70, 86 Kespiratory Quotient Rivers Rum ration in Army Saccharin, excreted unchanged Saliva Self-criticism, blunted by alcohol Shivering Spirit-drinking, in relation to chronic alcoholism „ „ „ drunkenness ... Stimulant, alcohol not a Stockard Stomach, catarrh of, in drunkards Susceptibility to alcohol, differences in „ ,, „ in animals ,, „ morbid , Synapses, probable action of alcohol on Temperature of body how maintained effect of alcohol on regulation of ... Tolerance of alcohol „ „ not acquired by all body cells Venereal disease, in relation to alcohol Volitional acts, complexity of ... Well-being, feeling of, induced by alcohol ... Will, enfeeblement of, in alcoholics Wines, effect of, on digestive ferments „ strength of Work, alcohol unsuitable as food for „ skilled, effect of alcohol on Page 22 47, 50, 51 127 18 56 ... ... 33 .• ..• 80 106 ... 105 31 37, 77, 125 103 99 91 97 93 36 78 79 81 80 91 92 95 46 30 113 59 ... Appendix 4 ... 129 126 129 Printed under the authority of His Majesty's Stationery OfBce by Darling & Son, Ltd., Bacon Street, E. 2. LONDON : Published by His Majesty's Stationery OHicc. and to be purchased through anj' Boolcseller or directly from H.M. Stationery Oflice, London, S.W. Price ^s. ijd. net. (i6o66i-,) \Vt. 1S802— 68S. 3000. 2,18. D&S (J 0_ W'f^mm