ALBERT R. MANN LIBRARY AT CORNELL UNIVERSITY Cornell University Library BF 371.L8 Memory and the learning process, 3 1924 014 104 610 «| Cornell University B Library The original of tiiis book is in tine Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924014104610 MEMORY AND THE LEARNING PROCESS MEMORY AND THE LEARNING PROCESS By DARWIN OLIVER LYON Late Fellow in Psychology , Columbia University BALTIMORE WARWICK & YORK, Inc. 1917 Copyright. 1917 WARWICK & YORK. Inc. TO D. H. M. IN MEMORY OF BAR HARBOR DAYS COIS'TEKTS. Preface , Introduction . Chapter I. On the Forms of Mental Activity Included Under "Memory" ... 13 CHAPrBR II. The Sub-Divisions of Memory in General, with Special Refer- ence to Their Relations to the Learning Process 32 Chaptee III. The Relation of Length of Material to Time Taken for Learning and the Optimum Distribution of Time 47 Chapter IV. The Relation of Quickness of Learning to Retentiveness 75 Chapter V. The Educational Value of Psychological ; v'l with Special Reference to Economy in Learning ',>'■,•■'• if^or nic Systems. . 155 Appendices '. "^i^^ 172 Plates Pocket in cover PREFACE.* The present work is the outgrowth of experiments on mem- ory that I have been conducting since the year 1906. The first experiments performed were somewhat limited in character, being made solely with the view of ascertaining the relation existing between facility of learning and tenacity of impres- sion. Whenever publishing brief abstracts of the work as it progressed, the title chosen as being the most appropriate was "The Relation of Quickness of Learning to Retentiveness." As the experimental work progressed, the method used became more elaborate, with the result that, though the relationship in question was never lost sight of, the data obtained were so extensive as to make the old title seem almost inappropriate. In a somewhat condensed form the results of these experi- ments, where they refer to the problem in question, are placed in Chapter IV. In a more complete form they constitute No. 34 of the Columbia University Archives of Psychology, Vol. V. In 1908 experiments were started on The Relation of Length of Material to Time Taken for Learning. Two methods, or distributions of time were used and thus there was added an extra problem — a problem that we might call the Optimum Distribution of Time. The results of these experiments have been published in The Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. V, Nos. 1, 2, and 3. In a slightly more condensed form they constitute Chapter III of the present volume. The larger parts of Chapters III and IV are confined to the examination of data obtained by experiment, and can be of but little interest to any but the student in experimental psy- chology. Chapters I and II are also somewhat technical in character, so that, apart from Chapter V, this book can be of but little interest to the ordinary public. *This work was anuounced in 1914, shortly before the war, but its publica- tion delayed until now because of the loss, in Germany, of the original manu- script. 1 2 Memory and the Learning Process Generally speaking, Education has use for Psychology only in so far as the latter may be of assistance in laying down rules for study. Education demands of Psychology that it show us how our various mental activities may best be de- veloped, how to recognize and use our dominant form of imagery, what form of imagery to select under certain con- ditions and depending upon the object in view, how good habits of study may best be acquired, and how labor-saving devices — in so far as the mind is concerned — may best be disclosed and developed. We do not presume to say that this book is an answer to such questions as these. Psychology is as yet unable to formu- late definite rules on problems so complex. We merely pre- tend to discuss the learning process from an educational point of view, and trust, at the same time, that the conclusions we have drawn from the various experimental data may be of some practical use to the teacher. Acknowledgments are due to Professors Cattell, Wood- worth, and Meumann for the numerous suggestions given me in the treatment of the results. Many of the experiments were performed on pathological subjects for purposes of comparison with the normal. In this connection I gratefully acknowledge thanks to the follow- ing for permissions and privileges granted: — Joseph F. Scott, Superintendent of New York State Beformatories and Prisons; Hon. John J. Barry, Commissioner of Correction of the City of New York ; Dr. C. Macfie Campbell of Blooming- dale Hospital, White Plams; Dr. Frederick L. Wells of McLean Hospital, Waverley, Mass.; Dr. August Hoch, Director of the Psychiatric Institute on Wards Island. INTEODUCTION. The ever-increasing appearance of popular articles on topics supposedly relegated to the psychologist testifies prob- ably more than anything elsie to the general interest taken in psychological matters wherever it is thought that any prac- tical advantage may result therefrom. This is especially true of MEMORY, and the interest with which the layman reads articles on this subject is but proof of the fact that it is still felt some royal road to knowledge may still be found — some mysterious method by which a thing once heard shall be for- ever remembered. It might be supposed that in the field of memory — a field so admirably open to experimental research — the psychologist would hold full sway, but that this is far from being the case is evidenced by the large following that the authors of the numerous memory "systems" have been able to acquire from a public that ought to know better. The success that these men have gained is, however, not alone due to the practical advantages that their students hoped may follow the adoption of their master's rules, but also to the fact, that the experimental psychologist himself has thus far been unable to lay down, rules that, to an impatient public, seem of any great importance. It is possible that this in- ability of the psychologist to here give any positive aid is due to the relatively small amount of experimental work that has been done on such problems as memory-improvement, economy in learning, mnemonic systems, etc., but the reason more probably lies in the hard fact that — as will be explained later — memory, as a faculty of the mind, is unimprovable. Though within the past decadie the progress that has been made in Experimental Psychology has completely changed the general aspect of this science, and although an unprece dented amount of attention has been devoted to an experi- mental study of the various educational problems, relatively little has been done with memory, notwithstanding its great importance to psychology, pedagogy, and education in gen- 3 4 Memory and the Learning Process eral. Many psychologists liave devoted the best part of their life to problems less important, but with the exception of Ebbinghaus, Meumann, and perhaps half a dozen others, no one has devoted himself for any lengthy period to the exclu- sive study of retention, recollection, and kindred problems. This is the more surprising when we consider its great im- portance. What can the psychologist tell us that is more im- portant than how best to remember ! The experimental study of memory for logical trains of thought has received but little attention, while an experimental study of the various systems of mnemonics seems to have received practically no attention at all. No experimental psychologist has as yet told us the most economical method of memorizing a passage of prose — or what method will give the greatest retention. Hosts of articles are found on other aspects of the memory problem, but as yet no one seems to have made these problems the sub- ject of serious research. Probably the main reason that there has been but little ad- vancement in this field is due to the difficulties encountered when one undertakes to investigate this so-called ' ' faculty ' ' of memory. The reasons for this are twofold — that is, not only do the difficulties exist as a matter of fact, but they are in- creased by the erroneous conceptions held concerning this men- tal function. Memory is not a separate and distinct faculty of the mind, as is generally considered to be the case, but a gross, unanalyzable term, having no claim to represent an elementary function of mental life. Having been taught to consider memory as a natural, specific, and distinct property of the mind by which it acts uniformly and machine-like in a specific way, experimenters naturally supposed they could investigate it in much the same way as they could reaction time, sensation, rhythm, etc. This view is still the prevalent one, even among the more educated classes, and the average layman still looks upon memory as an elementary, separate, and distinct function of mental life. The error is an enormous one. To see this it is necessary to get rid of the wrong con- notations that the word "memory" has acquired, and to use instead the term intellect. Memory properly interpreted is co-extensive with intellect, and in one sense of the word is part Introduction 5 and parcel of it. In reality — and as we shall endeavor to ex- plain in the next_chapter — there are many hinds of memories — as many kinds as there are senses and combinations of senses. To the sense of sight an orange is a spherical yel- lowish body, but this is, generally speaking, one of its least important qualities. Sight alone gives but a- poor idea of an orange. We have a better conception of one when we reniem- ber its taste, its odor, its consistency, how it sounds when we cut it, how it feels when we handle it, and whether it grows on a tree or in the ground. All these impressions, and many others, are necessary for a full and distinct conception of what an orange really is, and, generally speaking, therefore, we may say that the more complete our knowledge of an orange, the more perfect will be our memory image. In like manner with all conceptions. Take, for example, the law that the distance traversed by a freely falling body dur- ing any number of seconds is equal to 16.08 feet multiplied by the square of the number of seconds. Strictly speaking, to remember it we must know it ; and to know it we must under- stand it. Now, for a complete understanding there must exist in the mind conceptions of time, space, and matter, conceptions of what "distance traversed" and "freely falling" really mean; and there must exist a conception of what the square of a number means. In short, every- thing that is involved in this phenomenon or "law" must have its internal representative in the mind. Why do we go so into detail on this matter? Merely because, as we shall later en- deavor to show, MEMOEY is bascd on imagery, and imageey, to a certain extent, on our ' ' conception of. ' ' In much the same way as it is difficult to express the degree of perfection of an individual's imagery, so it is difficult to express the degree of his retentive capacity. We might even go so far as to say that it is impossible to say if a certain in- dividual has or has not a ' ' good memory. ' ' Too many factors are involved. One may have a very good auditory memory^ but a poor visual jm&^-er-^ce versa. He may, for example,^ stand high in the various memory tests as given in the psy- chological laboratory and yet find it exceedingly difficult to recall the names of people after introduction. It frequently 6 Memory and the Learning Process happens in performing memory tests on a class of students that he who stands first, and who, according to the tests, has the ' ' best memory, ' ' stands low in his class and is considered by his teachers as "stupid" and of "poor memory." The fact of the matter is that the "memory tests" ordinarily employed in the psychological laboratories are narrow in the extreme and "test" but a narrow portion of this so-called "faculty." The retentive capacity is far from being the only factor worthy of consideration in school life. Interest, voli- tion, power and rapidity of association, quickness of percep- tion, acuteness of the senses — these and many others must be taken into consideration. "We are much too accustomed to look upon memory as a unit, and think of it merely as a faculty of the mind that per- mits us to retain and reproduce the various impressions (sensations and ideas) received. But psychology, psychiatry, physiology, and anatomy have shown us that such a concep- tion is both too broad and too loose. The memory is an ensemble of operations of which each one has its proper sphere. These possess an independence to such an extent that any one of them may be strengthened — or weakened, and even disappear completely — without causing the slightest change in the others. Another factor that greatly complicates the determination of any certain individual's retentive capacity is the problem as to what extent we should consider his faculty for learning mechanical habits. It is certain that the process by which we memorize a "logical," "meaningful," or "connected" train of thought is far different from that employed in the ''learn- ing" of a mechanical habit, e. g., typewriting, or telegraphy — and it is probable that the process, both from a psychological as well as a physiological point of view, is also somewhat dif- ferent from that employed in the memorizing of a set of digits or nonsense syllables. The complexity of the problem may be seen when we consider such experiments as those per- formed by Bair^ on the typewriter — where the keys were cov- ered with different colors — and the writing performed by ^Bair. Psychol. Rev., Monograph Suppl. No. XIX. 1902. Introduction 7 watching a "copy" on which certain colors corresponded to certain letters. After the appropriate associations had been formed and "memorized" the colors on the keys were inter- changed, so that a different reaction was called for to each of the former ' ' color stimuli. " In a case of this sort it is diffi- cult to say just how far the "memory," as we ordinarily use the term, enters. It would be interesting to determine the cor- relation between efficiency for work of this type and that of memorizing prose. Interest is one of the main factors to be considered in mem- ory; it is the "mother of attention," and attention is the ' ' mothe r jofjiem.ojy. ' ' Many a boy who can remember the winning team in the baseball games for five years back, with the names of the various players, the umpire included, may stand at the foot of his class in school. ' ' Many a woman of a generally feeble memory can remember every dress she has owned since she was ten years old. ' ' - T" In connection with interest we should mention general un- j derstanding and intelligence. Other things being equal, the j. more a man knows concerning a certain subject, the better '' will be his memory for facts connected with that subject. "" Thus, for example, the physician remembers a diagnosis ; the merchant, prices ; the minister, sermons, and the lawyer, ver- dicts. /jWith this in mind the remarkable memory of certain ' scieimstsand philosophers is more easily understood, tn his autobiography Herbert Spencer says he could never under- stand how it was that an ordinary card player could remem- ber the various hands dealt at whist. Yet he himself remem- bered each and every fact that in any way related to his theory of evolution. Here the various facts he read or heard that in any way tended to verify his theory "clung" to him as grapes to their stem, and each addition did but seemingly make his inemory stronger. / As before said, there can be no such thing as a general ( improvement of the memory as a unitary faculty. Our desul- \ tory memories are given to us once for all. We have not a I faculty of memory that helps us as much for one subject as ^E. L. Thorndike. The Elements of Psychology. I'age 2.59. 8 Memory and the Learning Process another, but a "faculty," if we must use the word, that is of unequal eflficiency for different subjects. "No amount of cul- ture," says James, "would seem capable of modifying a man's general retentiveness. " Wherever a seeming increase (in the power of memorizing has been obtained it is generally due to an improvement in one's method of work, for every individual has a certain scheme of remembering according to his habitual occupation, and this scheme he may change or improve. The broadest statement possible on this matter is that given by Lloyd Morgan:^ "Eetentiveness," says he, "is in fact to a large extent a psycho-physiological datum ; some- , thing given in the brain-structure and mental character of each individual ; something which we can no more alter than we can alter the size of our heads, or, to take what is perhaps a closer analogy, "of the size of our muscles. By careful use and training we may develop our muscles within the limits assigned to them by nature. So, too, by careful exercise we may perhaps develop our retentiveness within the limits as- , signed to it by nature. ' ' ^""^trange to say, however, there are few questions in psy- chology on which the layman is so willing to give an opinion as upon methods of improving the memory, — and right here we have as good an example as may be given for the entrance of experimental psychology on such questions. This science has already overthrown many of the layman's pet theories, and the further the experimental psychologist carries his work, the more doubtful he becomes concerning many of the gener- ally-accepted beliefs. "There are current in psychology nu- merous well-appearing theories which, when looked into, are found not to rest on experimental observation, but on a few superficial statements of fact, eked out by a vast amount of logical construction. All such are properly subject to suspi- cion, and the more beautiful and self-consistent the logical construction, the more suspicious they are, because they are so much the less likely to owe their acceptance to agreement with facL/iThe experimental psychologist holds that we shall never jknow much about the mind until we take the trouble to find it ^Llotd Morgan. Introduction to Comparative Psychology. Page 107. Introduction 9 out, and that the trouble will consist in controlling the condi- tions under which observations are made and in using suffi- cien tly fine methods of ' ' observation. ' ' * Only by a careful scien- tific investigation can we determine the validity of the current views held concerning the mind's action. The popular belief is that "mental fatigue" follows very quickly upon protracted "mental labor." It has even been suggested that this "men- tal fatigue" is nature's safeguard to protect the muscles. This belief probably had its origin in the fact so commonly observed that people as a rule soon grow tired of mental work.^ The results of experiments, however, would seem to^ show that there is no such thing as "mental fatigue," as the word is ordinarily used. Memory tests have been continued without a break for five solid hours, showing, not a steady de- crease, but a steady improvement. Thus experimental re- search has destroyed many of these popular ideas, and in a few fields has this been more true than in that of memory. Take, for example, the rules laid down by "Prof. Loisette" in his "Psychological Memory; or, the Instantaneous Art of Never Forgetting," a memory system that some 20 years ago was in vogue throughout the United States. The brazen effrontery and insolent presumption of this "System" is the only psychological explanation of the success it attained. Notwithstanding that psychologists have shown the folly of trusting one's mental culture to systems so artificial, these have continued to advertise and thrive. The ridiculous pre- tensions and absurd claims made .by "Loisette" called forth numerous publications attempting to "expose" his system, but for the most part they were unefficacious. The only re- deemable feature of the "Loisette" system was that by advo- cating the use of certain definite methods for the formation of relationships it taught its followers to pay careful attention to their associations and thus weave the facts they Avished to remember with something already known. The perception of relationship is the desideratum. What- ever cultivation of memory that tends to the arrest of the power of rational thinking is to be absolutely avoided. Apart *R. S. WooDWOETH. Psychiatry and Experimental Psychology. Proceedings of the American JMedico-Psycliological Association, 1906. Page 128. 10 Memory and the Learning Process from mere names, numbers, and words in general, the mind in attempting to retain facts should seek inherent and essential relations, particularly those of cause and effect, reason for and subordination (individual — species). • Generally speak- ing, therefore, we hold that the various memory "systems" and schemes of mnemonics so in vogue at present are to be condemned without reservation. Experimental psychology has proven that the best way to remember a thing is to hnotv it. The only improvement that \ can be made in the memory is to alter one's habitual methods \t recording and recollecting. By this we mean that the most rational method of improving the memory is to seek to knoiv and understand the things we wish to retain. Go about the matter in a logical way, think over the subject carefully, and classify and tabulate the various facts as they come to mind. Endeavor to file the various facts in an orderly arrangement, and make this habitual if possible. This is the method used by the scientist, and in the end it is the only method that is of any use for logical trains of thought. Though, as already stated, it is probable that one's native capacity of retention is unchangeable, this does not mean that one may not improve one's methods of study. Experi- ments have been made that prove the superiority of the ■'whole" method as opposed to the "piecemeal" method;" that prove the superiority of short study periods as com- pared with long ones," and that prove the superiority (in re- tentiveness) of " stringing -out " the study periods instead of finishing the work in one sitting.' Psychologists are now quite unanimous in saying that one's native retentiveness is unchangeable, and that no amount of training can modify it. It is a physiological quality, and as it is given us at birth, so it remains. It may fluctuate with health and change with age, but this is all that can be said of it. Education can alter it but slightly, if at all. To be born, therefore, with a high retentive capacity is one of the "Lottie Steffens— Zeltsclirift. Vol. XXII. 1900. [For definition of these methods see Chapt. IV.] "D. Starch. Periods of Work in Learning. Journal of Ed. Psych Vol III Pase 209. 'D. O. Xyon. Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. V, Jan., 1914. Introduction 11. greatest of blessings, for retention is the sine qua non of human mental activity. It is probably the most important power of the mind so far as education and culture are con- cerned. Without it all advancement would be impossible, since it is only through the storing up of experiences, i. e., "memories," that mental progress is made possible. Other things being equal, the man with the best stock of ideas can reason the best. His life will be fuller and more complete, and he will be able to perceive relations and formulate laws impossible for the man whose native retentiveness is poor. One might even say that a fair degree of retentive power is necessary to a proper development of the emotions. Take, for example, the most typical of the altruistic emotions, sym- pathy. To be a good sympathizer one must have had expe- rience ; he must be a good observer ; he must be a good thinker ; he must possess a good imagination, and he must have a good memory. The last is really the basis of the othei- four. To be a good observer one must be on the alert to perceive the cause of grief. He must be able to detect pride, fear, shyness, and embarrassment, and to appreciate these he must be able to recall how he felt at times like these and the occurrences that caused them. The more vivid a person's imagination, the easier he can put himself in his friend's place. Now, as is well known, imagination must have at its basis a stock of ideas and images to start with, and this "stock" is part of memory. Taking everything into consideration, we think we are justified in saying that one of the greatest boons that can be granted a man is a high retentive capacity. CHAPTER I. ON THE FORMS OP MENTAL ACTIVITY INCLUDED UNDER "MEMORY." The cUvisibility of memory. — Testimony of anatomy, physiology, and psy- chology. Aphasia. Training along any special line does not necessarily improve kindred actions. — Mental improvements not transferable, "External" and "internal" forms of memory activity. Memory in its relation to Asso- ciation and Reasoning. "Literal" learning and "logical" learning. — The asso- ciation of words V. the association of ideas. Types of Imagery or Forms of "Presentation" : — visual, auditory, olfactory, gustatory, tactile, motor. Verbal memory. Professional and occupational memories. Effects of education, environment, sex, race, etc. There are as many ways of comprehending as there are forms of imagery. Visual imagery a form of "external sensitivity," acoustic and motor of "internal sensitivity." Importance of the subject to Pedagogy. An idea (in the sense of complete mental image) of an object is both more complex and more variable than we usually imagine — complex because images of the various senses enter into its make-up, variable because no two images of an object are ever the same, but vary with the time, place, and circum- stances. The image that arises when we shut our eyes and endeavor to recall a person or object depends, as we shall later explain, upon the individual's type or form of imagery — this, in turn, depending upon his sex, race, education, environ- ment, age, etc. It is the same with words, spoken or heard.' The word heard may recall not only a souvenir of the sensations felt when pronounced, but a souvenir of the movements made when writing it, as well as a visual image of the word, written or printed. The question is one of interest not alone to the psychologist, but to the physiologist and anatomist as well. For if it be that there exist in the cerebrum certain areas or fields corresponding to the various senses, it must follow that when one of these "areas" is cut out or its functioning sup- pressed, peculiar or irregular phenomena will occur. To the 'Here again neither is simple, but either form recalls, to a greater or less extent, the other. 14 Memory and the Learning Process physician and physiologist such phenomena are well known and are frequently brought about experimentally. It is pos- sible by a certain operation on the cerebrum of a dog to so isolate the optic area that, although it in itself may function, all "association" with other "areas" is prevented. Such a dog is psychically blind. He sees, but no longer comprehends what he sees. Through accident or disease similar condi- tions may occur in man, and the physician encounters them frequently in the various forms of aphasia. Sensory aphasia appears clinically in three distinct forms: (1) simple loss of memory for words; (2) word-deafness, or inability to under- stand spoken words ; (3) ivord-hlindness, or inability to under- stand written or printed words. Various forms of moTor APHASIA also exist. In the so-called motor or atoxic aphasia there is a loss of speech owing to an inability to execute the various movements of the mouth necessary to speech. The patient cannot be said to have forgotten the word, for he can write it, but he is unable to pronounce it, notwithstanding that there is no paralysis whatsoever of the mouth or tongue muscles. The cause of all this is a lesion in a certain part of Broca's convolution, though to the ordinary observer the patient appears merely to have forgotten how to pronounce the word. There are other forms of motor aphasia— these de- pending on the site of the lesion. In agraphia there is an inability to express thoughts in writing. In alexia there is inability to express written language in words. We have gone somewhat more into detail on aphasia and kindred matters than may seem necessary in a book of this sort. It is our object, however, in this chapter to show that the memory is not a single, separate, and distinct "faculty" of the mind, but a gross, unanalizable term having no claim to represent the functioning of any one portion of the brain. The various aphasias and kindred phenomena brought about experimentally on animals that our so-called "memory" is composed of a great number of primary memories, which are distinct one from the other and which may be lost separately. In like manner with the improvement of any of these various memories. If the brain functioned as a whole, we might as- sume that training along any special line would improve all The Forms of Mental Activity 15 kindred or allied actions, but we know that such improvement does not take place. Were it that the brain functioned in sec- tions or compartments corresponding to "memory,"' "judg- ment," "discrimination" or other of the so-called "faculties" or "phrenological organs" we might expect that the training of any special performance within the scope of a faculty would benefit alike all other performances included within that fac- ulty. "But since the evidence points to a highly detailed locali- zation of cerebral functions, and since the neural mechanism employed in any performance cannot be wholly identical with that required for slightly different performances, though it may be partly the same, training in one performance would not be expected to improve another, except in so far as the neural mechanisms involved were in part identical, i. e., em- ployed the same cells, fibers, and synapses. As applied to psychology, this would mean that, in order for a transference of skill to occur from one performance to another, there should be between the two, not simply likeness in the abstract, but some concrete part-performance in common, as there is between fighting and boxing, or between saying 'boot' or 'book.' In general, since the neural process in any reaction undoubtedly has more detail than appears either to intro- spection or to objective observation, it will not always be pos- sible to point out the common features of two reactions ; but that there should be features in common if any transference of training is possible from one to the other seems necessary from the physiological point of view. ' ' " Working with simple mental tests — such, for example, as the cancellation of 3's and 5's in a list of mixed digits, Poffen- berger* sums up his conclusions on this subject as follows : " ( 1 ) Where there are no identical bonds between stimulus and response in the two processes, the influence of one process upon another will be zero, i. e., there will be neither transfer nor interference. (2) Where there are identical elements in the two processes, or where a given process involves one or more bonds previously formed, there will be a positive trans- "Ladd & WooDwoRTH. Elements of Physiological Psychology. Page 5(i6. *"The Influence of Improvement in One Simple Mental Process Upon Other Related Processes." Psch. Bull., 1915, Vol. XII, page 65. 16 Memory and the Learning Process fer effect. (3) Where one test necessitates the breaking of previously-formed bonds and the formation of new ones there will be a negative effect or an interference." When by introspection we examine our stream of thought or analyze our various sensations and sense impressions we discover that there are various forms of mental activity in- cluded in that which we are accustomed to call memory. There is hardly an activity of the mind that is not in some way related to it or that does not come more or less under its head. It matters not if it be the learning of a poem or a rule in grammar, the latest song or the face of a new acquaintance, how to solve a puzzle or play a new game — each and all smack of "memory," and it is only because they possess certain fac- tors in common that we can group them under this head. It behooves us, therefore, to make clear these various forms of memory activity before discussing memory in the narrower sense of the word. This done, we shall then discuss the proc- ess by which we learn, the various forms and types of memory, and the various forms of imagery — with an attempt at classi- fication. For the present we may roughly divide all memory activi- ties into two classes — (1) those of "external" origin, and in which one or more of the so-called "senses" comes directly into play; (2) those of "inner" origin, and in which associa- tion and reasoning form a prominent part. 1. Memory activities of "external" nature, or, roughly speaking, observation. — Here we include the activity of 'the senses, and, generally speaking, all sense perceptions. This form of memory activity is witnessed when, by observation, we have "impressed" upon us the objects presented to the senses. The impressions are mostly visual, but any sense per- ception may enter. As already stated, this form of mental activity generally passes under the name "observation," and comes especially into consideration where space is entailed, as, for example, in the study of a geometrical figure ; or with time, as in the learning of a melody. The Forms of Mental Activity 17 2. Memory activities of "internal" nature or origin. — These are more difficult of definition. We here include the processes of association and reasoning. Memorizing by asso- ciation is what we ordinarily term "learning" or "learning by heart," and consists largely of an association of words. The really active factor in this form of memory activity is not the mere perception of the senses (single or repeated), but the repetition and reiteration of the various ideas, images and as- sociations between them. The process is largely one of fixa- tion of impressions, and takes place in all true memorizing, especially in the verbal form known as "learning by heart." When, for example, we are committing to memory a passage of prose or poetry, each word and phrase that we perceive (be it by eye or ear) signifies something to us, and it is these ' ' significations ' ' — ^with the associations formed — that give the mind material to work on. Another form of memory activity that we must classify under this head is that which takes place when we wish to get the significance (meaning) of an article, conversation, lecture or description without learning it word for word. This proc- ess calls in the reasoning faculty, so that here association of ideas comes more into play than association of words. The perception (be it visual or auditory) of the separate words plays a much smaller part here than in the "learning by heart" just spoken of. Nearly all that the average adult learns is acquired in this manner. While reading the average man does not pay any particular attention to the individual words, their order or number. He endeavors merely to get the significance of the various sentences and paragraphs and associate their meaning with facts he already knows, impres- sions he has already received, emotions he has already felt, etc. The idea (meaning) is what is retained — not the words — though these may be reproduced because of vivid visual images, or by sheer accident. Thus it is that an educated man can retain in one reading what might take an ignorant man or a child many readings. The extraordinary performances of the logical memory in men of science is easily explainable when we consider that, while reading a book or listening to a lecture, all such minor details as the meaning of each and 18 Memory and the Learning Process every word, the construction of the sentences, arrangement of words, etc., are easily put aside and the memory reserved for the nucleus of the argument. Another reason is that, though a true repetition or re-reading of the lecture may not take place, the result is nauch the same as though there were, for the scientist has so large a stock of ideas on his par- ticular subject and is so continually occupied with them — reflecting, correcting, refreshing, etc. — that he may be said to be continually reviewing his material. Between learning (memorizing) by the association of words and learning by the association of ideas there is considerable difference. For purposes of discussion we shall term the former literal learning; the latter, logical learning. Literal learning calls into play one or more of the senses. Imagery of some form is invariably a factor. Logical learning, on the other hand, does not necessarily employ the senses ; when it does, the employment is seldom direct, but secondary, in the form of imagery. The attentive faculty is more of a factor with literal learning than with logical learning, and is em- ployed in a more direct manner. We frequently hear a difference of opinion concerning the memory power of a certain individual — one speaks of him as having "an extremely good memory"; another, as a man of "relatively poor retentive capacity." Upon examination this difference of opinion wUl generally be found to be' due to the fact that where the one refers to his "literal" memory, the other refers to his "logical." "We can all cite cases of men whose powers of literal learning are remarkable, but whose "logical" memory is weak, and cases of an opposite character are equally common. It should not for a moment be imagined that these two forms of memory are separate and distinct — the one sharply marked off from the other. On the contrary, each, to a certain extent, depends on the other. Association is a factor in both; in "literal" learning, however, it is more of a motor-auditory type, whereas with "logical" learning reason and logical associations predominate. Memory, in the sense of native ability to retain impressions, sounds, words, etc., or "brute" memory, as it is sometimes called, is admit- tedly stronger in children than adults. The logical memory. The Forms of Mental Activity 19 on the other hand, is weaker. This latter is due not merely to the fact that the adult has a, larger stock of ideas and can therefore reason better, but because the attentive faculty is stronger in adults than in children. Generally speaking, we may say that facts memorized by literal learning do not "last" for so long a period as those memorized by logical learning. The speed of light, cause of the tides, distances of the planets, etc., are quickly forgotten by the student who uses his "literal" memory — ^based largely on motor or auditory images. The astronomer, using his log- ical memory, never forgets them. This difference in length of retention is due to several things, but mainly to the fact that the intention or object of memorising is different in the two cases. The same astronomer who uses his logical memory on the matters cited above would probably use his literal memory were he to attempt to recall a set of nonsense syllables just read to him, or were he to desire to retain in his mind a ques- tion asked him while he sought the answer. For in both of these cases there is no desire or intention on the part of the individual to retain the matter memorized for any length of time. In one sense of the word literal memorizing is a contin- ual refreshing of the primary impression — a continual revival or restoring to life of something once partly or wholly mem- orized. Thus it is that the work is so much more intensive and demands greater concentration than logical memorizing. TY^ES OP IMAGERY, METHODS OF LBABNING, AND POEMS OF PRESENTATION. Anyone who has performed memory experiments on a con- siderable number of individuals, such, for example, as the presenting of a passage of prose to a class of students with directions to learn it by heart, is struck by the great individ- ual differences — differences in method of study, time of learn- ing, degree of application, amount retained, fidelity of reten- tion, duration of retention, etc. All these are open to expe- rimental investigation, and have already been dealt with to a greater or less extent by the experimental psychologist. To him all such problems are of interest, even though certain 20 Memory and the Learning Process of them are of relatively little educational value. To the pedagogue and educational psychologist the problem of chief importance is the relation of the time taken for learning to amount retained, or, to be more specific: 1. The relation of time taken for learning to amount di- rectly retained." 2. The relation of time taken for learning to duration (permanence of retention) of the material retained.^' 3. The relation of either of these to the metJiod used in the memorizing." No. 1 has been investigated by Ebbinghaus, Meumann, and others. The matter is somewhat without the scope of this work, though we shall touch upon the subject later on. No. 2 has as yet received but little attention from the experimental psychologist. In a work devoted wholly to this subject the author discusses it in detail under the title ' ' The Eelation of Quickness of Learning to Eetentiveness. ' ' " The problems that fall under No. 3 are considered in the present treatise, chiefly in Chapter IV. In investigating any of these problems it will be found that they fall into two grand divisions — (1) the slow learners, (2) the quick learners. "With each of these two subdivisions are again necessary — (a) the good retainers, (b) the poor retainers. Other things being equal, just why one individual should have better native retentiveness than another is not easy of answer. The problem, however, of why he is able to memorize more quickly — to learn more quickly — than another individual is more susceptible of experimental investigation. Many factors, however, must be considered, e. g., method of study, material used, rhythm, attention, association, mne- monics, etc. "This is not a question entailing individual differences. The matter is exactly as stated— i.e., What Is the relation between time and amount? — e.g., If it takes 5 minutes to learn 20 words, how long will it take to learn 40? "'By this we mean the relation of quickness of learning to retentiveness— i.e., Do those who learn the quickest remember the longest? ^he question here is twofold— (1.) What method of memorizing is the shortest— i.e., what method gives the greatest amount [of material directly retained]. (2.) What method gives the longest duration [time]. "D. O. Lyon. Archives of Psychology, (Columbia University). Vol. V, No. 34. The Forms of Mental Activity 21 In order to discuss intelligently the various types of learn- ers {Lerntypen) it is necessary we first consider the types of imagery (Vorstellungstypen), or, rather, to be more specific, the various methods of learning depending upon the method of imagery or mental ' ' presentation. " It is the custom of the present-day psychology to divide and classify memory into several classes or types, these depending upon the dominant sense or dominant form of imagery employed in the mental process in question. On an anatomical-physiological basis such a classification might be expressed somewhat as follows : 1. Visual. 2. Auditory. 3. Olfactory. 4. Gustatory. 5. Tactile. 6. Motor. It will be observed that the first five correspond to the class- ical "five senses." The sixth corresponds to what we might term the muscular, kinetic or kinesthetic sense. In the strict sense of the word, there is no such sense, motor imagery being more or less a combination. It is not to be understood that to classify one's imagery is a simple matter, that each and every individual can be thrown at ease into one of the above divisions, and that these divisions are clear-cut and distinct. On the contrary, it is frequently a difficult matter to say just what one 's predominate form of imagery is. When we speak of an individual as being of the "auditory type" we mean that in the ensemble of memories retained by this man auditory imagery is either predominant or more clear and alive than in the average individual. In a considerable number of people two or more of these forms of imagery may be so high and so nearly equal that they may be said to predominate at the same time. In fact, this is usually the case with our great sculptors, artists, and musicians — our virtuosos. Excellence in but one form of imagery, be it ever so high, is seldom^* sufficient to make one a master among "Exceptions occur only In such cases as those of some "lightning calculators," chess-players, etc. — ^here a certain form of imagery — usually visual — may be greatly in excess of'all others. 22 Memory and the Learning Process men. To be great a sculptor must have not only his visual memory developed to an extraordinary degree, but his motor (kinesthenic) as well ; without it he will lack execution. That which characterizes a person's type of memory — that makes him, for example, of the visual type rather than the auditory — is that he has the habit of preferring visual imagery in his various mental operations. It does not follow that such an individual has no auditory imagery as though he lived in a world of sight. For the nature of one's imagery varies with the time and the circumstances — or, to be more exact, ac- cording to the sense with which he is paying attention. Thus it is that a person of the visual type may, if he desires, evoke auditory images — auditory images that are perfectly clear and intense, and that differ in no way from those of the aver- age individual. It must not be imagined for a moment that there are six, and only six, forms, or types of memory. Various groups and combinations of these types exist, to say nothing of the various subdivisions of each type. For example, there exist at least three forms of visual memory : 1. Color — and brightness. 2. Shape — and size. 3. Place — and movement. Some authorities hold that only the first form is truly visual, the two others being called the false visuals, because in these it is not the retina with its associated cortical centers that plays the principal part, but the various muscles of the eye which (by their various contractions and relaxations) permit us to form, an idea of the object's shape, size, and place. As a matter of fact, there are many more than three sub- divisions of visual memory, and the three forms given above are but general in the extreme. We now know that one can push further the above classification and detail subdivisions to each of the three forms. With No. 1, color — for example, we may speak of shades, hues, depths, etc. ; effects and degrees of light and shadow; colors that with some individuals give a sensation of sound or of smell, and others that cause the pulse to quicken or give rise to certain emotions. The visual The Forms of Mental Activity 2H imagery in such a case is obviously more complex than in the normal individual. To go into a detailed discussion of the remaining types and sub-types of memory and imagery would be outside the scope of such a book as this. The six forms we have mentioned are sufficient for our present purposes. The classification made is a physiological one, and the six forms are thus more or less ideal. The chapter would be incomplete, however, and certain of the experiments later to be described not well understood, were we not to devote a few words to a form of memory which, for want of a better term, we shall call the verbal memory. Psychologically, the chief thing that differentiates man from the lower animals is his ability to express himself in words. A natural concomitant of this ability to express his thoughts by words {language) is that of thinking and reasoning in words. The larger part of the memory images as recalled by an adult are in the form, of ivords, and we may say that the development of memory is always more or less concomitant with the development of language. The constant employment of verbal imagery in the mental operations of civilized man has become so habitual and so firmly rooted that we may all now be said to possess what we may call an inner language — a language that accompanies (or rather is part and parcel of) our various acts, judgments and feelings. These at the time of their occurrence are felt and expressed in words to such a degree that the image retained is more or less a verbal one. Thus it is that we may be said to store away and carry about with us a large part of our various experiences done up in "verbal packages," so to speak. Thus it is that when we revive an experience or sensation we do not reproduce the sensation itself (though this may occur) but only a story of it — a story in words. Introspection will prove to anyone that whenever he attempts to reason, especially if in a care- ful and methodical manner, a voice within him arises that formulates his reasoning in words. It matters not what the mental action may be — be it the observation of an object, a decision, a judgment or a sensation — in each and every case 24 Memory and the Learning Process verbal imagery enters in. It has been experimentally proven that every time one hears a word or sound the mouth, and especially the tip of the tongue, make a movement — a ' ' sketch- like" movement, as though there were a desire to reproduce it. It is a frequent occurrence to hear one who attempts to recall a certain word say, "I can almost remember it, for I have it on the end of my tongue. ' ' There are people who do a large part of their thinking in a loud voice, and who appear to talk with themselves whenever they think. Their "inner" language becomes "outer," and they say aloud that which the ordinary individual says to himself. We may feel — expe- rience a sensation — ^without words, but as soon as one dwells on the sensation and attempts to reason about it, state its cause or give its effects, words arise. Theoretically, it is possible to feel a sensation, experience an emotion, see an object, or even take a decision hy sensation alone and without the employment of verbal imagery; but the latter nearly always enters in more or less. An examination of this verbal image reveals the fact that in its most habitual form it con- sists of a mental repetition of a word once pronounced — an auditory image of a word or group of words. It may be simple or complex, sharp cut or ill-defined, lasting or evanes- cent — but it is always there. Each and every word, provided that it is understood, car- ries with it a great number of reminiscences, so that in a cer- tain sense we may look upon every word as a compacted memory. Verbal imagery cannot be called a native form of imagery, it being both a derivative and combination of other forms. It has arisen only in the process of man's mental evolution, and is highest where culture and civilization are highest. In children and the anthropoid apes it occurs only in' a low and most undeveolped form, and can truly be said to exist only where verbal language exists. As to its origin, we might say that it is probably based on motor and auditory imagery, visual imagery surplanting the latter in individuals of the visual type. The Forms of Mental Activity 25 Space does not permit a detailed discussion of the remain- ing forms of imagery/' Those that we have considered are, for a work on memory, those of chief importance. Tactile IMAGERY is undoubtedly the most primordial of all, and is a dominant factor in the mental life of all animals. In man the organs of feeling by which tactile imagery is formed may be said to reside chiefly in the fingers, lips, and tip of tongue. Olfactoey imagery. This form of imagery is used in our thinking more often than we are apt to imagine, and in some individuals is developed to an extremely high degree. In the case of the French novelist, Zola, it was developed to such an extraordinary extent that we are justified in calling him an individual of the olfactory type. For Zola, every object had its proper and characteristic odor. It was the same for every person, city, street, or what not ; — provided he knew ihem inti- mately enough, each one had its proper odor. His olfactory memory extended even to the times and seasons. "L'automne, par exemple, lui parait caracteristique avec son oduer de champignons et de fueilles nouillees." " Gustatory imagery. But few people have this form of imagery sufficiently intense and developed to such a degree as to entitle our calling them of the gustatory type." Gustatory '"As imagery is based upon the senses, it behooves us to emphasize the fact that these latter are by no means limited to the "classical five." Besides the peripherally-initiated feelings of external origin (such as the auditory, visual, etc.), we have the peripherally-initiated feelings of internal origin (pain, mus- cular effort, hunger, nausea, etc.) Besides all these we have the emotions or centrally-initiated feelings. "Toulouse. Emile Zola une enquSte medico-psychologique. Paris; 1896. Page 206. "I know of but one case so developed as to warrant the term. This gentle- man (J. R. H.) employs gustatory imagery to a large extent in his various mental operations. This is especially noticable in his recollections of people and places — which are frequently connected in his mind with some article of diet. For example, recalling some of his old friends, he said, "Yes, Phyfe always had good roast-beef, and Mr. Wing fine lima beans ; Gordon had good roast-potatoes, and Mrs. GofC fine cod-fish cakes and corn-bread." In like manner, speaking of milk at the hotels, he once said, "The Ritz in London has very good milk ; the Ritz in New York is good, but not quite so. good ; Delmonicos has poor milk. The best glass of milk I even drank was up in the country at s. The Murray Hill Hotel has good ice-water, omlets, and Welsh-rarebits; they have not changed their Welsh-rarebits or roast-beef in the last thirty years. I remember that the last time I dined there [six months ago] the bread was not so good as usual." His gustatory memory goes back to boy-hood, "I remember," said he, "when a little boy, of a certain dish of 26 Memory and the Learning Process images are seldom pure, and usually include a greater or smaller amount of olfactory imagery, — due undoubtedly to the close connection that exists between the two senses. As to its relation to tactile imagery, we have only to remember that the taste of an. article depends partly upon how it feels to the tongue. The terminal filaments of certain branches of the nerves of taste have been found to end in the area of general sensibility, situated in the parietal lobe. Besides the six primary forms of memory, with their va- rious subdivisions, there are various combinations and com- plex forms that are difficult of classification. We have already spoken of the memory of ideas and concepts, showing its rela- tion to the verbal memory ; but what shall we say for the mem- ory of feelings and emotional states? And. how, apart from being able to say that auditory or visual imagery may enter in, shall we classify the memory of meaning of abstract terms'? We have tried, but we have been unable to form a satisfactory classification. One of the best and simplest might be stated as follows : 1. Memory of the six senses, including motor mem- ory. (Here also are included memory for space, and, to a certain extent, that for time.) 2. Memory of the emotion and states of feeling. .3. Memory for ideas and concepts (such memory be- ing based on memory for names and abstract word meanings). Neither the memory of space (extent) or of time (duration) have received from the experimental psychologist the attention they deserve. Cattell and FuUerton^^ in an extensive study of memory for lifted weights found that the memory image would appear to last for a period of about nine seconds, "after which the observer does not so much compare the sensations as decide on the approximate intensity of each sensation separately and compare the decisions." roast-potatoes I eat at . / can taste them now. I remember that Barbara Dunigan made the best currant-pie and cooked the best dish of string-beans." He lias proven to me that he is able to reproduce a real gustatory image — vivid and well defined. The subject is an extremely well educated man. Apart from the gustatory part, an examination of his memory shows nothing re- markable. To learn the 20 words (v. p. 172) took him 13 minutes. The Forms of Mental Activity 27 The results and conclusions drawn from the various experi- ments that have been performed along these lines do not always agree. Jastrow" in working on memory for space found that neither the visual nor the tactful variety suffered as much with the passage of time as we might expect. Both forms appeared almost as faultless after a lapse of several days as after a few minutes. Other experimenters, however, notably Landau-" and Weber-\ found a more or less regular decrease. As to the memory for time intervals, here also there is some difference of opinion. Paneth" found that the memory image (for time) was as sharp and clear-cut after an interval of five minutes as after an interval of but one. HoUingworth^^, from a larger and more carefully-planned set of experiments, con- cludes that "the curve of memory for duration follows more closely the ordinary statement of the 'law of forgetting,' in the case of the constant error, although the variable error undergoes little change up to an interval of 30 seconds. ' ' The various "types" of memory that we have considered in the preceding pages may be considered as native or ana- tomical-physiological. Each of them is to a greater or less extent influenced by education and environment. AVe thus have types of memory corresponding to the various profes- sions and trades — types that we may designate as the physi- cian's memory, mechanic's memory, chemist's memory, etc. The nature and direction of the attention is, in all such cases of course, an important factor, and this is especially noticeable in the case of certain special trades and professions. Take, for example, the professional chess player, waiter, or actor. Here in each case the memory is developed along the special line desired by the individual in question. Here the degree of development will, of course, depend partly on the will, the attention being habitually directed in one special direction. ''Cattell and Fuixekton, "Small DifCerences," p. 147. "Jastkow, Mind, Vol. XI, 1902, p. 552. =°r.ANDAU, Wissensch. Rev., 1896. ^Wagner's Handwdrterbuck der Phpsiologie, p. 2. ''^Paneth, OentralU. fiir Physiologie, Vol. IV, 1890, p. 82. ^'HoLLiNGWORTH, "The Inaccuracy of Movement." Archives of Psychology, June, 1909, p. 86. 28 Memory and the Learning Process Take, for example, the professional chess player ; he has, as he plays, a certain number of memory images clear-cut and sharply defined. To these alone he pays attention, to the exclusion of all others. Long practice makes this habitual and almost automatic. The process is but an example of the law of the conservation of energy. There is no fatigue, and all unnecessary action is eliminated. Where, however, education and environment show their greatest effect is in the case of sex. In all countries the en- vironment with which the boy is surrounded is so different from that of the girl, and the education received by the one is so different from that received by the other, that we are justified in distinguishing a masculine memory and a femi- nine memory. Apart from reasons biological it is but natural to suppose that the masculine mentality differs from the feminine. Women thinli in a different manner from men; their psychology is different; their interests are seldom the same, and their attention seldom centered on the same object. We usually give women credit as having a livelier imagination and stronger emotions, and it is well known that these things have their influence on memory. The effect of education and environment is also observed in the memory-differences seen in the various races. The psy- chology of the Indian is quite different from that of the Euro- pean, and the mental imagery of the Mohammedan is not the same as that of the Englishman. We could push the argument further, but the effects of education and environment on imagery and memory as wit- nessed in profession, sex, and race are sufficient. SufiSce it to say that, generally speaking, there are as many types of memory as there are individuals ivho retain. The method by which one learns depends upon his method of presentation — his type of imagery, his way of thinking. It will be noticed that we differentiate between learning -types'-'' and types of imagery.'''" An individual of the visual type will, '"In German, — Lcrnmethoden. =^In German,~TorgteUungst'!/peii, Sinesiypen, Gcdiichtnistypen, Anschau- ungatijpcn, or HpracMypen. The Forms of Mental Activity 29 when given a set of digits, memorize them by visual imagery ; but he may learn to memorize them in auditory manner if so directed. It frequently happens that one's profession or walk in life necessitates the employment of some other form of imagery than his dominant one ; for example, that a bank clerk of the visual type remembers numbers and words as read to him. In such cases the continued practice and exercise of the weaker or dormant form of imagery results invariably in a great increase of its power. Pedagogically, such questions are of considerable importance. To what extent, for example, should a man endeavor to choose his calling and select his busi- ness in the line of his dominant form of imagery? And to what extent should he endeavor to improve his other forms! To what extent does a man's habitual or strongest form of imagery influence his life and affect his relationship with his fellow- beings? As yet we do not possess very definite answers to these questions. We do, however, know that along any special line or direc- tion the faculty of attention is capable of unlimited improve- ment, i. e., the rapidity with which we may adapt ourselves, and the intensity with which we may concentrate. We also know that the attempt on the part of an individual to develop his lesser or dormant types of imagery results very frequently in a lessening in the power or vividness of his dominant type — the type to which he seemed to be born. A phenomenon of a similar nature is seen when we attempt to educate "light- ning calculators" of the type of Fuller, Buxton or Colburn. In the case of Colburn a repose of three months (during which period he discontinued his representations before the public) resulted in a very considerable loss of his powers. Experi- ments have proven that the acoustically born individual suf- fers a temporary diminution in his' acoustic memory when by exercise he attempts to increase his powers of visual imagery. The way in which one comprehends depends upon his form of imagery at the time. Individuals differ widely as to their types of imagery and combinations of these types, with the result that there are many ways of comprehending and meth- ods of learning. When reading a book the visualist "fixes" 30 Memory and the Learning Process a considerable number of bis impressions as written or printed images. He makes mental (visual) note of the words and groups of words, notes tbeir arrangement on the page and remembers on which side of the page occur statements or sentences of particular interest or beauty. In short, the matter is one largely of visual imagery. The acoustic or motor type of individual, on the other hand, sets himself to put the material that he r.eads into the form of heard or spoken rvords. Unlike the visualist, he does not perceive the ensemble in any one instant, but one word must follow after the other. This brings us to the main difference, or point of distinction, between these two forms of imagery. Visual imagery entails the factor space in its make-up, whereas acoustic and motor imagery entail time; and thus it is that visual imagery is, as it were, a form of external sensitivity, whereas acoustic and motor are forms of internal sensitivity. Unlike the visualist, the acoustic and motor types are not able to have, during reproduction, the various words or objects of a. "series" before the "inner eye" at the same time. The visualist, on the other hand, "sees" the whole thing in a ' ' glance. ' ' Another advantage possessed by visual imagery is that as a whole it is both truer and more reliable than the auditory or motor type. Several observers''' have proved this experi- mentally. Each type, however, has its advantages and dis- advantages. True though it may be that the visualist is able to observe and "fix" his images more securely, he takes more time to do this than does the motor or acoustically inclined individual. This subject of imagery is of capital importance to the pedagogue, for it is just with children that modifications in the method and manner of producing and reproducing images are most easily initiated. The plastic mind of the child is vastly more susceptible to changes in the form of imagery than is the adult mind, but unfortunately the average edu- cator pays no attention to the fact. To the kindergarten ^'Notably Pohlmann, Meumann, Munsterberg, Kirkpatrick, Whitehead and Finzi. The Forms of Mental Activity 31. teacher also, tlie subject of imagery is of importance. It enters to a greater or less degree in all systems of education and training of the young, especially in such as those devised by Montifiori; Frobel, and Pestalozzi. The results obtained with these systems coincide with the results we ]jave obtained from experiments performed in the psychological laboratory, namely, that the present system of education entails a great loss of time. There is a great expenditure of time and energy, and relatively but little of value accomplished. CHAPTEE II. THE SUB-DIVISIONS OF MEMOEy IN GENEEAL, WITH SPECIAL EEFEEENCE TO THEIE EELA- TIONS TO THE LEAENING PEOCESS. Sub-divisions of the memory process : — retention, reproduction, representa- tion, identification. Retention an essential feature of all life^-conscious and unconscious. Re- tention is influenced by: (1) Condition of the organism, (2) Strength and clearness of the impression, and (3) Repetition. Reproduction is the process by which objects that have previously been known are brought back into consciousness for representation and usp ,. Imagination v. study. Identification the highest department and final stage of memory. Three factors to be observed: (1) It is a form of cognition, (2) That which is known is re-known. (3) It is of my past. Methods of invfestigating the learning process: (1) Method of description, (2) Method of reproduction, (3) Method of identificatioUj (4) Method of com- parison. " Memory is usually defined as the mental capacity of retain- ing unconscious traces of conscious impressions or states, and of recalling these traces to consciousness. But this is not all. A further analysis of memory (if we use the word in its more general sense) will reveal the fact that the mind must be conscious that the impression, sensation or mental state in question has a certain relation to the past. In the fullest and most complete sense of the word, a com- plete act of memory will be found to involve four processes : (1) retention, (2) reproduction, (3) representation, and (4) identification. Retention may be defined as that capacity of the mind that keeps and stores up the various facts, sensations, images and ideas that may later be called on for reproduction. With re- tention pure and simple .the mind is not conscious that any of these knowledges exist, for as soon as consciousness enters we have reproduction and recognition. Repi'oduction is the process by which objects that have previously been known (be they mental images, states of feel- ing, or what not) are brought back into consciousness for representation and use. 32 The Subdivisions of Memory 33 _ Representation is that stage of memory process that con- sists in the mind presenting to itself the objects that have previously been known. It may or may not be accompanied by identification. When this accompanies it the process is finished, and we have a complete act of memory. When rec- ognition does not accompany it. we have fantasy—a form of representation in which the images brought before the mind are severed from their ordinary relations. Identification is that stage of the memory process in which the object is recognized as having belonged to or been in con- nection with a past experience. It includes not only the ele- ment of time, but that of self. That is, not only are we assured that the object in question represents some knowledge or experience of the past, but that this past knowledge or expe- rience was our own. The above are but definitions. We shall now consider each of these sub-faculties in detail, and endeavor to explain just how we retain, how we reproduce, how we represent and how we identify. Retention. The act of retaining or the maintenance of changes caused by external stimuli is an essential feature of all life.* In fact, in the broadest sense possible it is a feature of inorganic things as well. It is an axiom that the quantity of movement existing in the universe is invariable and constant, so that when we see force being expended on a body it follows that changes of some kind in that body must follow. The body is altered — an alteration that exists at the expense of the change in motion. For wherever there is motion there is displace- ment. The displacement may be permanent or only tempo- rary, affect the mass as a whole or only parts of it, but it is always there. *In much the same way that we define retention as the maintenance of changes caused hy external. stimuli, so we may speak of memory in general. This is the view frequently held by biologists, some of whom go so far as to say that memory is nothing but an acquired character. The trouble with such a definition, however, is that it includes instincts, which, resulting as they do from habitual modes of action accidently acquired, have been preserved and transmitted by natural selection and heredity. 34 Memory and the Learning Process When a current of electricity passes through a wire the wire is altered. This change, however (unless the current be very strong), is but temporary, though theoretically it is doubtful if the wire ever returns to exactly the same condition as be- fore. Similar phenomena may be observed with other forms of force. When we send a stone through the air or throw it into a pool of water, the fluid medium (in the one case air, in the other water) is agitated and set into motion. The material is, however, altered, but for a relatively short time, and though certain molecules are displaced and their places taken by others, the fluid may be said to return to its original condition. In other cases, however, we may witness a permanent change— this depending upon the nature of the material and the nature of the force affecting it. The current of electricity may be .90 strong as to cause a change of shape in the wire, or the water, if frozen, so non-elastic that the portions sepa- rated by the blow of the stone cannot return to their former position. In short, as shown by Herbert Spencer, the matter is also a physical one, and depends not alone on the nature and amount of the stimulus, but also on the solidity and elasticity of the body affected. We are now in a position where we may better understand the psychical process of retention — a phenomenon of a suffi- ciently similar nature as to be comparable to such physical phenomena as those cited above. In the psychical act of re- tention there must be a molecular or atomic change of some sort in the brain substance, although the exact nature of this change is unknown. We are still ignorant of just what takes place in the brain when we reason. We are unable to differen- tiate, in terms of change in brain structure, an emotion of love from one of hate, or a sensation from a judgment. We only know that during the process in question movements of some kind must exist, and that these leave their traces in the form of a more or less lasting change in the nervous tissue. The comparison may be carried further. It is a matter of everyday observation that the object or body that has once had a certain change wrought on it by an external force or stimulus can be made to adopt this new form with a lesser ex- The Subdivisions of Memory 35 penditure of energy every successive time. Tlie overcoming of resistance is a phenomenon of habituation. It is easier to fold a piece of tin if it has already once been folded, and where the water has made a channel there it flows most easily. Habit is not only a mental affair — something confined to the cerebrum. Any organ may show its effects. Liver, stomach, and intestines may become habituated as well as the brain. One may contract a strange manner of walking as easily as a strange manner of thinking, and the muscles are as easily deformed as the mind. Retention is influenced by several things. For purposes of discussion we shall group these under three heads : (1) Condition of the organism. (2) Strength and clearness of the impression. (3) Repetition. Condition of the organism. The results of operations per- formed on animals and man, certain psychoses, and the va- rious diseases of memory as witnessed by the specialist in ner- vous diseases, prove that the faculty of conservation is inti- mately connected with the cerebral organism. The effects of cerebral disorganization are best studied in the various forms of amnesias, but to a lesser extent we may also observje them in old age, fatigue, and the action of such drugs as alcohol, opium and cannabis indica. Strength and clearness of the impression. Six subfactors are to be noted : (a) Strength of stimulus. Generally speaking, the stronger an impression, the greater its chance of remaining in consciousness. It is a matter of everyday observation that feeble impressions are soon forgotten. (b) Clearness of stimulus. A stimulus may be strong, but not clear, or vice versa. By clearness of stimulus we mean distinction of ideas, i. e., to what degree is the image or idea clear-cut, sha;rply defined and separate from similar images and ideas. Good retention necessitates complete understanding, and ideas that are vague and confused are not well understood, 36 Memory and the Learning Process and hence not well remembered. It is obvious that attention is an important factor in this connection. (c) Organisation of ideas. Generally speaking, it is only by organization and classifying our various knowledges that they are remembered for any length of time. This is espe- cially true of the pure sciences in which perfection can only be retained by constant classification, the formation of sys- tems, and the rational ordering of new ideas as they enter. Analysis and synthesis, deduction and induction are all proc- esses to be considered under this head. (d) Independency. Facts that we have worked out for ourselves or that we have learned through experience, are relatively well retained. What we invent ourselves, be it in the form of an idea or in the construction of a machine, is not easily forgotten. In short, it is a question of origin, i. e., does the knowledge in question come from within or from without ? It is obvious that attention and interest have to be considered under this heading. (e) Attention. The influence of attention on the retentive capacity is too well known to need any discussion. (f) Emotion. Ideas and images that are formed during periods of great emotional activity are practically never for- gotten. A lively or vif emotion is retained forever. Repetition. This subfactor is of such importance that it may replace all other conditions. The effects of routine and habit are too well known to require any'discussion. Eeproduction and Representation. We shall discuss these together for the reason that they are very closely linked, and the one practically necessitates the other. The memory does not merely conserve ideas — it also reproduces them and makes them again "alive." Eeproduction, as we have already said, is the process by which objects that have previously been known are brought back into consciousness for representation and use. The reproduction of an idea or image may be passive and arrive spontaneously, or by the active process of association. In the former case the image arises without our being able to The Subdivisions of Memory 37 give the reason, as, for example, when in the middle of a piece of work a certain melody forces itself into consciousness. Generally, however, the reproduced idea or image arises by association. This latter may be voluntary, or more or less involuntary. When, for example, we behold a certain house we may recall our childhood days — children with whom we played and the recurrence of certain events, and a continua- tion of such recall may go on for some time undisturbed. But it is always in such cases under the control of the will, and may be stopped any moment. The process may be more or less automatic. If the element of volition enters but slightly, we call it imagination. When, however, the voluntary element predominates, we call it study, as in this case the endeavor to learn and acquire is brought about by means of mental application. At such times there is a process of effort and exertion, and we "endeavor" and ' ' try ' ' to get one idea or image to recall another. During our waking hours the various impressions and sen- sations that we receive from the world around us, especially if our attention is centered on them, direct our stream of thought and keeping out as they do, extraneous or unallied matter, exercise a sort of repression. In imagination- the stream of thought is more or less allowed free play, and there is but slight repression. In sleep and reverie, however, the stream of thought is let run as it will, and though one idea may follow another through the activity of the process of association, this process is not directed by the will — as is the case in study. In recalling a mental state, be it an idea, sensation, or what not, there is a passage of a current that affects the cerebrum in a manner more or less similar to the way it was affected the first time the mental state occurred. As to how far the two processes are identical will be discussed later. Suffice it for the moment to say that it is not probable that the brain tracts excited in the recall are perfectly identical with those excited by the event at the time of its original occurrence. The reproduction and representation of an image may be caused in various ways. As a rule, it arises because of a visual or auditory stimulus. I happened just now to recall St. Marco's Cathedral in Venice. I recalled it for the simple 38 Memory and the Learning Process reason that I happened to lift my eyes and saw the photograph that I once took of it. But the recall might have taken place in many other ways, e. g., by a photograph of some othei- cathedral, by the name ' ' St. Marco, ' ' or possibly by the mere word "cathedral." The degree of perfection of the image would naturally vary in the different cases. The most com- plete and perfect recoUective image would be produced only by again beholding the Cathedral itself. It is evident that this department of memory is more or less linked with, and may even consist of, association. The associa- tion may be one of ideas, sensations, images, emotions, or other form of mental activity, but in one form or another it is always there. Some psychologists hold that there are two varieties of re- production, depending upon whether the object is recalled from within or without, i. e., they distinguish between an image that is recalled by the individual himself (from within) and the image that is recalled by a stimulus from without. In short, they differentiate remembering from being remem- bered of. They hold in support of this classification that it is impossible to recall a series of mental images or representa- tions without one of them having arisen and been caused by external stimulation. No reproduction, they claim, is other- wise possible. This school explains reproduction by holding that real bonds or connections (anatomical or physiological) exist between the members of a series, and that we are able to pass to another group or series only because of the fact that there exists an element in common between the two. Some go so far as to hold that the bond is material. The other school claims that the connecting bond in association is purely a mental one, intellectual and ideal. Generally speaking, the rapidity with which a former idea or state of mind can be recalled depends both upon its degree of vividness and upon the frequency with which it has been recalled in the past. When for the first time we recall and restore before us a certain state of mind it does not ' ' arise ' ' with rapidity. In order to obtain it quickly it is necessary that it be a state of mind that has already frequently occurred The Subdivisions of Memory 39 in the past. [Eepetition, Practice.] To obtain the image or state of mind in all its details we have to apply our attention. Eecollection alone, however, is, as we have already said, not memory. It may, however, be said to be the activity that grows into it by the aid of the activity of introspection and attention. Indentification or Recognition. , " Unconscious retention, whether conceived of as a 'holding in store' of certain cerebral habits and dynamical associations, or as a 'keeping' of ideas within a metaphysical entity called the mind, might be absolutely perfect and yet no actual mem- ory-consciousness develop. Reproduction might be secured in perfection and might go on forever and yet no faintest shadow of a true remembrance pass within the soul. Memory, in the full meaning of -the word, is a lino wing of the past and of my past. It is re-cognition." ^ Identification is thus the highest department — the final stage — of memory, for not only do we thereby assure ourselves that the object in question represents some knowledge or experience of the past, but we cognize that this past knowledge or experience was our own. It is by this faculty that we distinguish the oldest from the newest and assign the time and place of the original image, emotion or idea. In short, ide'ntification includes three fac- tors : (1) It is a form of cognition; (2) that which is known is re-known, i. e., the affair is something of the past, and (3) the "something" is of my past — something belonging to my experience. The mere reproduction of anterior states of consciousness does not represent the entire process of reproduction. We only really reproduce when we are conscious that it is ive Avho had the experience. In this case we consider the idea not as the original experience, but merely as a reproduction of it. The two main factors of identification are, therefore, the following: (1) idea of time, (2) identity of me (self). When consciousness presents an idea it is necessary in order to recognize it that we be conscious of the fact that we "IjAdd. Outlines of Descriptive Psychology. Page 238. 40 Memory and the Learning Process have already had this idea (or experienced this sensation) in the past. It will be seen that there is an analogy between the process of identification and that of external perception, for in order to grasp and comprehend sensations they must be subjective, and perception consists in precisely such a process, i. e., it "projects," so to speak, these sensations into space. Our word representation to a certain extent implies this, for it entails an idea of space — a form of external sensitivity.^ The mental process of identification can only be rationally explained by an acceptance of the theory that in recall the brain cells that were affected by the original experience are reaffected in the reproduction. As an illustration, let us again take the Church of St. Marco. The visual image the first time I beheld this church had, we will suppose, certain factors and characteristics — certain parts of it being clearer and more detailed than others. We will also suppose that it was two days before I again saw the Cathedral. During these forty- eight hours the image became more or less reduced and effaced. When, two days later, I again saw the church the image thereby formed was so nearly like the first image that practically the same parts of the cortex were affected. But, having already once been affected, they were affected more easily the second time. In short, the second image "found,'- so to speak, certain brain cells already affected, — its work was easier, — there was recognition. It thus resulted that the essen- tial parts of the image (retained the first time) were repro- duced in the formation of the second image, and in each case more or less the same brain cells must have been affected. Now, in view of the fact that certain parts of the brain were affected or altered the first time, less remained to be affected the second time. There was less work to be done — -more room left for attention-to and refinement-of the image. The reappearance or recall of a mental state, sensation, -or image may or may not be recognizable. If recognizable, it is because there were in the mental comparison made certain factors found to be in common, or, to put it in other words, 'Time on the other hand is a form of internal sensitivity The Subdivisions of Memory 41 the same brain cells were found to be already affected. There was a feeling of ' ' having seen. ' ' Various explanations exist in psychology as to the nature of the process of recognition. Notwithstanding what we have said under reproduction concerning the probability of the same brain paths being affected, and how under habit we gave a similar explanation, e. g., that a coat falls more easily into its old folds, it is not probable that the paths affected in recall and recognition are exactly the same as those affected the first time. On the contrary, it is probable that the brain tracts excited by the event at the time of its first occurrence and those excited in its recall are in part different from each other. "If we could revive the past event without any associates," says James,^ "we should exclude the possibility of memory, and simply dream that we were undergoing the experience as if for the first time. Wherever, in fact, the recalled event does appear without a definite setting it is hard to distinguish it from a mere creation of fancy. But in proportion as its image lingers and recalls associates which gradually become more definite, it grows more and more distinctly into a remem- bered thing. For example, I enter a friend's room and see on the wall a painting. At first I have the strange, wondering consciousness, ' Surely I have seen that before, ' but when or how does not become clear. There only clings to the picture a sort of penumbra of familiarity, — ^when suddenly I exclaim : 'I have it, it is a copy of part of one of the Fra Angelicos in the Florentine Academy — I recollect it there!' But the mo- tive to the recall does not lie in the fact that the brain-tract now excited by the painting was once before excited in a simi- lar way; it lies simply and solely in the fact that with that brain-tract other tracts also are excited : those which sustain my friend's room with all its peculiarities, on the one hand; those which sustain the mental image of the Florence Acad- emy, on the other hand, with the circumstances of my visit there; and finally those which make me (more dimly) think of the years I have lived through between these two times. The result of "this total brain-disturbance is a thought with a •James. Principles of Psychology. Vol. I, page 658. 42 Memory and the Learning Process peculiar object, namely, that I who now stand here with this picture before me, stood so many years ago in the Florentine Academy looking at its original." Like perception, identification is a complex act, and neces- sitates the operation of several of the so-called faculties. Identification is apt to be confounded with several allied affairs or mental processes, as may be seen by comparing it with the dream, illusion, hallucination,* and certain acts of the imagination. We refer to a fiction of the imagination, such, for example, as when we are in doubt as to the occur- rence of a certain action, e. g., if we have really over had such an idea, seen such a place, or beheld such a scene. | The uncer- tainty of the memory is notorious. How frequently do we imagine ourselves to have said things that in reality we never said? And hpw often do we forget having seen a certain per- son or witnessed a certain event when in reality we did see the person and witness the event? It has doubtless occurred to everyone to ask himself: "Is that a dream of mine, or did that really happen ? ' ' This problem of identification is thus a double one — two questions arising: (1) In what way do^we know if our iden- tification is a true one, i. e., one of an actual former percep- tion? (2) By what process do we distinguish the creation of our imagination! In answer to the first question we may say that there are four things to be considered, i. e., there are four factors that distinguish the real perception of an object from the mental image we may afterwards form of it: (a) When we actually perceive an object the image as it exists in the mind stands out in relief; it is alive, vif, and distinct, (b) The visual perception is confirmed by other per- ceptions, and is in harmony, and en rapport with it. For example, when I behold my table I see not merely the table, but the pencils and books lying on it, the chairs standing around, the effects of light and shadow, and to a varying extent the surrounding articles of furniture. To a greater or lesser extent these would be lacking in the pure visual 'Strictly speaking there are two kinds of sensory deception. (1) Illusion, or the false interpretation of external objects: and (2) hallucination or sub- jective sensory images arising without the aid of external stimuli. The Subdivisions of Memory 43 image of the table as formed with the eyes closed, (c) Actual perception forces these images into consciousness, and even though we may so desire, we are unable to prevent their en- trance. For example, I can^ if I wish, form a visual image of my table, either with or without its books, but I cannot when I actually see the table prevent myself from seeing the books upon it. (d) The image formed by actual perception is not really capable of being modified or changed— it is not malle- able. Images of the other type, however, those, for example, formed with the eyes closed, are extremely supple and docile, and can be modified with ease. The image that arises during contemplation and meditation is felt instinctively to have been produced ivithin us and by us. We also instinctively realize that it is modifiable at will. We feel that we have created the image in question, that it was not made for us or thrust upon us, and that we may greatly modify it at pleasure. The image is also felt to be more or less hazy and indistinct. At times, however, the image formed may possess a clear- ness and vividness and stand out in relief to such an extent that it may be taken for a perception. In such cases we have what is known as an hallucination. In certain forms of in- sanity the image is vivid and alive to an extraordinary degree, and resists all attempts of modification. If, on the contrary, we can modify the image as we wish; if its attachments to other ideas and images are not too strong, if- it is not too clear, plain, and distinct, and especially if it is formed during a period that we instinctively feel to be one of meditation, we know that the image is one of our own invention — in short, that it is an image and not a real per- ception. « # . * * -* * * In Chapter I we considered memory in its relation to imagery and endeavored to show that memory, as the word is generally used, is complex in the extreme — that there are, in short, as many forms of memory as there are forms of imagery. We also showed that in order thoroughly to examine and investigate anyone's memory we should have to examine 44 Memory and the Learning Process separately his various forms of imagery. We may, however, go further than this and say that in order to completely know anyone's memory we would also have to examine sepa- rately his powers of retention, reproduction, representation and identification. It is difficult, nay, almost impossible, to measure these ' ' sub- faculties" separately, and as yet we have no special method for each individual process. In investigating the memory we have at our disposal some eight or ten methods. The four most commonly employed in psychological laboratories are: (1) The method of description. (2) The method of reproduction. (3) The method of identification. (4) The method of comparison. It will be noted that three of these correspond to three of the four sub-faculties of memory that we have already de- scribed. Various materials may be employed in an investigation of the memory by any one of these four methods. In the fol- lowing description we shall illustrate them as they are em- ployed when figures of different shapes are employed (such, a g., as those shown in Fig. 1). The figures used may vary Fifi^ The Subdivisions of Memory 45 only in size or in both size and shape. In some cases, color also is added, but this brings in a factor that so complicates two of the methods as to make them impracticable for the pur- poses intended. (1) Method of description. The method of description consists merely in describing from memory the figure as nearly as one can remember it. As a general rule, the de- scription given by the subject is not very precise, and where precision is required this method should be replaced by one of the others. With the method of description the subject is asked to describe the figure, i. e., he is asked to state, in terms of words, as to whether, for example, it is a triangle or square, and if a triangle, what kind of a triangle. He is also requested to state its size. This latter may be stated in terms of centi- meters or inches, but it is best described by comparing it with some object already known. Intrinsically there is no objec- tion to the subject stating the length of the figure in centi- meters or inches. If he does this, however, it is probably because he has retained the size of the figure in his mind not as a figure of such or such a size, but as a figure of a certain number of inches in length. In other words, with this method he should not use numbers in his retention. (2) Method of reproduction. The method of reproduction may be described as follows: The subject is given a pencil and paper and told to draw from memory the figure that he has seen. He may draw and re-draw as many times as he desires, make as many figures as he wishes and correct as often as he may think necessary. It will be seen that this method brings in the visual memory in a different manner from that employed in the preceding method. For here the subject is allowed to draw, and thus he is able to behold and visually perceive a figure that more or less approaches the original even though it may not be an exact reproduction. It frequently happens that, although we are able to recog- nize an object when we see it, we may be incapable of repre- senting it visually with the eyes closed. In such cases we may be able to recall an image of it and recognize it when we see a figure that nearly approaches it; in short, we may 46 Memory and the Learning Process "know" it in one sense of the word and yet not be able to reproduce it. We do not mean the reader to understand that reproduc- tion by hand is more difficult than reproduction by eye. But it is frequently impossible to place on paper the ensemble of visual images as introduced to the mind by way of the retina. (3) The method of identificatiovp. This method consists in presenting to the subject (at the time of the second ex- periment) a row of figures, one of which is identical with the figure originally seen. The subject is asked if he can recog- nize in the group the figure observed the day previously. We may here note an important difference that exists between Methods 2 and 3. It is that in the method of reproduc- tion the muscular sense is called into play. In order to give shape and size to the figure that the subject attempts to draw he must translate his visual image into an equivalent motor one. (4) The method of comparison. This method cansists in presenting to the subject a figure similar to, but not exactly like the one studied the previous day, and in asking him to state in what way the figure he studied the day before dif- fers. He is asked to state if it was larger or smaller, thinner or thicker, longer or shorter, etc. — in short, he is asked to make a comparison. It will be seen that this method is in one respect similar to the method of identification, in that it does not call into play a sensory organ other than that which was called into service at the time of the original presentation. CHAPTEE III. THE RELATION OF LENGTH OF MATERIAL TO TIME TAKEN FOE LEARNING and THE OPTIMUM DISTRIBUTION OF TIME. (27ic various Plates referred to in this Chapter icill he found in the Pocket of the front cover.) Problem l^The relation of length of material to time talteu for. learning. Problem 2 — The optimum distribution of time. Complexity of Problem 1 — (a) For each and every "length" various methods (both as to distribution of time and division of material) must be tried; (b) various materials must be used ; (c) various methods for calculating the "amount retained." "Once-per-day" method versus "contmuous" method. Relation of length of material to time taken for learning found to depend largely upon wliicli of these two metliods is used. Importance to Pedagogy of Problem 2. Length of the problem. Taking all materials and methods of presentation into consideration the most economical method is, generally speaking, to distribute the readings over a fairly lengthy period. Advisability of reviewing lecture-notes as soon as possible after taking them. Poor economy wlien once the original associations formed are lost. The use of nonsense-syllables in memory tests. Difficulty of forming home geneous sets. Kules for the formation of long sets. In this chapter we give the results of a somewhat elabo- rate series of experiments undertaken to determine the relation of length of material to time taken for learning. The materials used were nonsense syllables, digits, prose, and poetry. In view of the fact that these materials were memorized by two methods, viz., — by what we shall call the " continuous " method and the "once-per-day" method, op- portunity was given to compare these two methods both as to economy^ and durability of impression,- and in view of the fact that other distributions' of time were later used in an- other set of experiments, it was felt that the sum total of the results obtained on The Optimuyn Distribution of Time were {sufficient to warrant putting this down as part of the title. M. e., total time taken. 'i. e., retentiveness. "e. g., twice per week, once per week, etc. 47 48 Memory and the Learning Process Were one undertaking a really thorough investigation of the problem of the relation of length of material to time taken for learning, it is obvious that for each and every "length," various distributions of the study periods should be tried— i. e., not only should each prose passage, set of digits, or what not, be memorized by the "continuous" method,* but it should be memorized by reading twice a day, once a day, once a week, etc. Such a procedure would, of course, give us what I have termed above as "The Optimum Distribution of Time." By also varying the methods of presentation, e. g.,— reading the material" to the subject, letting him read it, etc., etc.,— we would but add another contribution to the more general problem of The Most Economical Method of Learning. Tho this problem will be discussed more in detail later on' in this chapter, it will not be amiss to say a few words on it here. Of the many factors' that must be considered in the problena of Economical Learning only three concern us at present — (1) the length of practice periods, i. e., how long should we study at each period? (2) Frequency of practice periods,— i. e., how often should we study? (3) Method or manner of practice, — i. e., how should we study ? Supposing now, that each of these three questions be de- 'i. e., in one sitting. 'V. p. 81. 'e. g., intensity of sound, when material was presented aurally ; color, when presented visually, etc., etc. These and many other such factors would have to be considered in any investigation of the most economical method of learn- ing, if the investigation were a thorough one. Another factor that would have to be borne in mind is that after a certain time a physiological limit is reached, beyond which further practice increases neither the speed nor the accuracy. Most important of all, however, is the fact that what is most "economical" for one individual is not necessarily the most economical for another. Again, certain factors count more with some individuals than with others, e. g., it has been shown (Cohn. Zeit. f. Psych. Vol. XV.) that in "silent" reading, the in- hibition of all articulation made a much greater difference with some indi- viduals than with others. Aside from attention it should be remembered that interest, or attitude oi mind, for want of a better term, plays a considerable part. It was shown by Witasek (Zeit. f. Psych. 1907, XLIV) that active recitations of the material being memorized were far superior to the more passive readings of the ma- terial. Witasek found that long before a list could be recited perfectly,, it was possible to recite portions of it, and he found that when the sub,iect relies' upon his memory in attempting the reproduction, (only being prompted when he hesitated) the list was learned in fewer repetitions. Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 49 cided for — say memorizing a passage of prose, it does not .necessarily follow that the same procedure should be adopted for digits. Still less does it follow that it would necessarily be the best procedure for learning a language or learning to typewrite. Individual experiments must be conducted for each and every material. The results of many experiments have now proven to us that the so called "natural" or "psy- chological" method^ of learning a language is not the most economical. It has been shown that such associations do not develop in truly natural learning — and, that any attempt to force these foreign associations into the subject's mind, re- tards, rather than hastens, his progress. Here, as with learn- ing to telegraph, the natural and most enocomical method is to allow the "habits" to grow and develop together. T^ ^ T> T^ ^ TP We shall now consider, but first from an historical point of view, the problem that it is the main object of this chapter to present, namely, the relation hetiveen amount to he learned and time taken for learning. I say "time taken" instead of number of repetitions (which at first thought would seem the better comparison) for the reason that we shall later compare the total time taken by the "once-per-day" method with the total time taken by the "continuous" method. This problem is one that has received but little attention from the experimental psychologist. The first in the field is Dr. Hermann Ebbinghaus. Ebbinghaus* found that after one reading he could repeat 7 syllables; 12 syllables took 16. G readings ; 16 syllables, 30 readings, etc. The following table expresses his results in tabulated form — with certain addi- tions of my own made for purposes of comparison that will be discussed later. 'We refer to the metliod of first thoroughly learnmg the various letters, then combining the letters into syllables, then combining the syllables into words, then combining the words into sentences, and finally combining the sentences into the desired thought. 'Vher das Oeddchtnis. Translation by Ruger & Busenius, p. 47. 50 Memory and the Learning Process TABLE A. Number of Number of DifCereuce in Time for one Total syllables 7 repetitious 1 repetitions" reading" 3.5 sec. time" 3.5 sec. 12 17 16 24. " 102. " IG ■ 30 13 32. " 240. " 24 44 14 48. " 528. " 3G 55 11 72. " 990. " It will be seen tliat the number of repetitions increases at first with great rapidity, but that later the increase becomes less and less — the increase in repetitions being relatively much greater than the increase in the number of syllables. Binet" found a similar tendency. He found that 11 digits could be reproduced after 4 seconds of study; 13 took 38 seconds ; while 14 took 75 seconds. Binet and Henri'= working together obtained the following : TABLE B. Number of Digits Number of Seconds 10 17 15 75 20 135 25 180 30 260 50 420 100 1500 200 4520 Offner" in attempting to explain such a result as the above said that it was possible that the greater the number of meni- bers in a series, — the less the attention paid to each member. This, however, would assume that each and every series de- manded, and always received, a certain definite amount of at- tention, which would mean that the greater the number of digits, syllables or what-not in a series, the less attention each digit or syllable would receive. Offner also suggested, as a partial explanation of the relative increase in time needed for "Figured at tbe rate of 2 syllables per second. Tbis was not Ebbinghaus's actual rate but it is near enough for purposes of comparison with my own rate of 2 syllables per second. "i. e., the increase in repetitions over that that was needed for the preceding set of syllables. "Binet, PsijcJiologie cles grands calculateurs. Paris, 1894. *=BiNET & Henei. La M&rnoire des Mots. L'Annee Psychol. I, 1895. '=Offneb. Das Oedachtnis. Berlin, 1909. Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 51 the longer series, the fact that in a long series the act of for- getting has been in operation longer than in a short series — since it takes longer to re.ad r^ hence, the greater number of repetitions required. Myers sought to explain these results on the grounds that the longer series fatigued more and that the individual members received, therefore, relatively less at- tention. This is somewhat similar to Offner's explanation and contains undoubtedly a considerable element of truth. It is doubtful, however, if the decrease in attention is proportion- ate. Ebbinghaus sought to explain his results by the narrow- ness of the span of consciousness and in retroactive inhibition. Although Ebbinghaus distinctly states in his preface: "The tests were all made upon myself and have prihaarily only in- dividual significance'"* — yet his "curve of forgetting" that developed from his experiments with nonsense syllables has given us what is sometimes known as Ebbinghaus' s law. Strange to say, however, no systematic inquiry has, until re- cently," been made to test the validity of this "law." Not only are his results quoted in nearly all the works on psychol- ogy — but conclusions, supposed to be of educational signifi- cance are drawn from them. It should also be borne in mind that on this subject Ebbinghaus 's experiments were few in number. His data for 24 syllables are based on but 3 experi- ments ; those for 36 syllables on only two. Both Eadosavljevich and Meiimann noted that the change from 8 to 12, or 12 to 16 syllables, did not demand a very great increase in the number of repetitions. In fact, frequently 16 syllables were memorized with fewer repetitions than 8 or 12 syllables. The following tables (C to I inclusive) give the re- sults obtained by various investigators of this problem of the number of repetitions required to memorize varying nmnbers of nonsense syllables. Excepting Table G, each table is for one, and only one, subject. As may be noted, the individual dif- "C6er das (?edac7i*MS— translation by Ruger & Busenius. Ruger and Bussenius touch on the point in question in their "translator's" introduction. They say. "in spite of the fact that his experiments were performed only on himself and that the numercial results obtained are consequently limited in significance, Ebbinghaus has" . . . etc. "The first serious inquiry into the matter was made by V. A. C. Henmon. The results of his work were read before the American Association for the Advancement of Science at the 1911 Washington meeting. 52 m El t n P •CO ■15 M o Memory and the Learning Process s >> 36_« KS no « I" dg S b-a 5 HO Mg « g«ori H'J, S ng « Eh o — en W Qg S IS a ^ oa Ms (B d ^ CDOM'*C0Mlr- OClCrHiHO (M 03 1-1 C-l s O O CO 00 (M OTtHt^t-OSeOOlfflTtllOLOt-tD M CO CO ^ rH 1-1 O IH CO CO ^ OM'^ttlOOOTHO T-li-liHrHi-IIMOJCO iH © iH OD-£ y •S" ^ g ss > ■a £?i J3 0) If C^ •" tis S o oS eq o s o £S ^ s« H to. o 3 <4-( |o ^ £ SJ t4 3 fl ^^ fcl) 0) 2 "^ s EQ sa +J a c S s^ p o Is o t» a ^'^^'S ^^ . s^P,"^ Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 53 ferenees are marked. The "difference" column is meant to show the relative increase, or decrease in the number of repe- titions. It will be noted that with Ebbinghaus the increase is always considerable, e. g., 24 syllables take 44 repetitions, whereas 36 take 55 — an increase of 11. With the same series of syllables, Meumann's increase is only 3. It will be noted that some of these results are very different from those obtained by Ebbinghaus, for while with Ebbing- haus there is a relative increase in the number of repetitions with increase in number of syllables, with Meumann there is a relative decrease in number of repetitions with an increase in the number of syllables. "Meumann holds" says Hen- mon" "that this is what might be expected. An increase in amount of work to be done, if it is not too great, makes little difference when once the initial disinclination or inertia is overcome, when adaptation of attention is secured, when the associative processes have been aroused, and a general ad- justment to the work is once attained. All of these formal con- ditions of learning should be effective for series no matter what their length within the limits of fatigue. Hence, it is reasonable to expect a relative decrease in energy required for learning with an increase in amount to be learned." The results of Henmon's experiments with nonsense sylla- bles that concern us here are shown in tables E and F in con- junction with those of Ebbinghaus and Meumann. His results differ widely from those of Ebbinghaus and Binet but are in fairly close accord with those of Meumann and Eadosavlje- vich. There is a relative decrease in the number of repeti- tions as the number of syllables increases. Particularly note- worthy is the fact that the number or repetitions for the series from 10 to 18 is practically constant. The results are even more striking than those of Meumann in showing the relative economy with the longer series. In investigating the relation of amount to be learned to repetition, Henmon also made a departure from his prede- cessors by using meaningful material. His results on three subjects in memorizing 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 stanzas of In Me- moriam by the "whole" method are given below: "Op. cit. 54 Memory and the Learning Process stanzas Words 1 28 2 56 3 84 4 112 5 140 TABLE J. Number of Repetitions for: — H (10) D (10) P (5)' 3 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 10 7 11 12 9 14 14 The results of my own experiments with poetry are given in Table K. Only two subjects were used. The stanzas aver- aged 25 words each and were of the same type as The Ancient Mariner. Table L gives my own individual results (as taken from Plate 3) and is placed here merely for purposes of com- parison. TABLE K. inzas 2 5 10 25 50 Number of Repetitions for : — Words G (6)" M (6)" 60 7 5 150 17 14 30O 19 16 750 22 16 1500 30 23 TABLE L. Stanzas 2 5 10 25 50 Words Number 50 125 250 625 1250 of Repetitions" 6 16 22 19 25 Henmon found that the increase in the number of repeti- tions with the increase in amount is relatively less than the increase in the number of lines or stanzas. If the increase was proportional to the amount the number of repetitions would be 3.5, 7.0, 10.5, 14.0 and 17.5 instead of which the series is 3.5, 6.3, 8.6, 10.0 and 12.2, which are the averages of the three subjects of Table J. There is, therefore, according to Henmon, a relevant economy with the larger amounts. The economy in relearning after 24 hours is greater with the "Tlie figures in parentheses indicate tlie number of experiments from wliich tlie averages were made. "Approximate only,^ — being computed (at tlie rate of 2 stanzas in 0.23 min- ute) from the total time as given under "continuous method" on Plate 3. Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 55 larger amounts and, according to Henmon, .is relatively greater with poetry than with nonsense-syllables. Henmon also made experiments nsing prose as the mate- rial. This consisted of 100-word, 200-word and 300-word pas- sages selected from the essays of Huxley and Matthew Arnold. Sixty selections |rom Huxley were made, and 60 from Arnold, — 20 passages of each length. One practiced subject learned 54 of these selections, (18 from each group), and re- corded the number of repetitions required for learning. The results are indicated below in Table M. Table N, giving the results of some of my own experiments, which will be taken up in detail further on, is appended for purposes of compari- son. In the paper read at Washington, Henmon stated only the number of repetitions. Assuming that the reading was performed at the ordinary rate (about 200 words per min- ute), I have taken the liberty to figure the approximate time taken for these repetitions, and have included it in Table M. It will be noted that with none of the passages do any of my subjects get so low a time as that obtained by Henmon. If his rate of reading was faster than I have assumed, the differ- ence would be even more marked. TABLE M.. Nuuibei" of Words 100 200 300 Repetitious 6.4 7.3 7.0 Approximate Tiine 3.2 mill. 7.3 mill. 10.5 lulu. TABLE N." Number or ' Subject, with Number of Minutes Taken ^ Words B. B. Ed. W. El. W. E. F. E. B. H. B. A. K. A. H. E. E. F. Wl. R. W. A. Q. E. A. F. Wo. D. L. 100 8 10 12 14 13 16 19 15 21 17 18 17 35 31 10 300 25 24 19 47 40 26 29 32 37 36 51 48 78 41 28 SCO" 42 59 67 98 103 57 88 75 .. 45 133 81 * •54 As will be noted from Table M, Henmon finds an approxi- mate constancy in the number of repetitions for the passages, irrespective of their length. Tho with the nonsense syllables some of my subjects gave results that approximate those ob- '°It should be noted tliat Henmon used 100, 200, and 300 word passages, whereas I used 100, 300, and 500. 56 Memory and the Learning Process tained by Hernnon, with prose I get no such results as those that he obtained, as mav be seen from Table N. I have given the preceding brief summary of the results of the various investigations on The Eelation of Length of Mate- rial to Number of Eepetitions, not only as an historical re- view of those who have worked along these lines, but also be- cause it allowed me to bring in at the same time for purposes of comparison, results obtained from certain experiments of my own. These experiments will now be considered in detail. It was stated at the beginning of this chapter that in in- vestigating the relation of length of material to< time taken for learning we may, in the learning, use various methods. In the following experiments only two were used. I have designated them as (1) the "continuous" method, and (2) the "once-per-day" method. In the former the subject is allowed to memorize the material en-masse, i. e., in one sitting; in the latter the subject memorizes the material by reading it once a day — and once only, until memorized. The curves exhibited on the Plates, therefore, show not only the relation of length of material to time taken for learning, but they give a com- parison of the total time taken to learn any passage by the one method as compared with the total time taken to learn a pas- sage of the same leng-th by the other method. tP w ^ w 4Ei ^ 4S As may be seen from the plates, with their accompanying tables, the length of time taken to learn a passage of prose or a set of nonsense syllables, depends in large measure on the method used in performing the learning. In one sense of the word, therefore, it would be more fitting to call this experi- ment The Relation of Length of Material to Time Taken for Learning when said Learning is Performed in one Sitting; and, The Relation of Length of Material to Time Taken for Learning when said Learning is Performed by the "Once-per- day" Method. The materials first used were nonsense syllables and poetry. With these the experiment was continued for I41/2 months. Digits and prose were then substituted and the entire per- Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 57 pormance repeated. The maimer of conducting the experi- ment was as follows : — On May 1, 1908, I memorized 8 non- sense syllables taking my time by the watch. In the evening of the same day I memorized a four-line stanza of poetry of twenty-four words. An interval of two days was then al- lowed to elapse. On the following day (May 4th) a similar set of eight nonsense syllables was read once. Eealizing that this reading was not sufficient for a perfect reproduction, the syllables were laid 'aside to be read the following morning. On the evening of May 4th, a stanza of poetry similar to the one previously memorized was read thru once. A perfect reproduction of the stanza was possible after this one read- ing, so this finished the two sets of eight nonsense syllables and the two sets of one-stanza poetry passages, both for the "continuous" method and the "once-per-day" metliod. From previous work with nonsense syllables it had been found that, when memorized by the "once-per-day" method they, "clung" with great tenacity, and it was deemed advisable, therefore, to allow an interval of one week to elapse before starting on the next set. Therefore, it was not until the morn- ing of May 15th that the twelve- syllable set was started. In the meantime, however, i. e., on May 5th, a passage of poetry consisting of tivo stanzas was memorized by the "continuous" method. An interval of one day (May 6th) was then allowed to elapse and on May 7th a similar passage of two stanzas was read once and once only. Three readings were necessary to get this two-stanza passage, i. e., it was read on the 7th, 8th and 9th. An interval of one day was then allowed to elapse, after which a three-stanza passage was started.'" In this manner the experiment was continued for over four- teen months. Nonsense syllables were always read in the morning, — poetry, in the evening. After finishing each pas- sage of poetry, whether by the "continuous" method or the "once-per-day" method, an interval of one day was allowed to elapse. With the nonsense syllables, however, a longer interval was deemed necessary— two days being allowed to elapse after each "finishing" with the "continuous" method '"This is all shown on plates I, II, III and IV. Plates I and II are for non- sense syllables ; plates III and IV, for poetry. 58 Memory and the Learning Process and seven days after the " once-per-day " method. The rea- sons for making the intervals longer for the nonsense syllables are obvious. It should here be said that no other experi- ments on memory were conducted while this experiment was in progress, and neither nonsense syllables nor poetry of any nature was read during the entire period. To make the conditions of the experiment as scientifically accurate as possi- ble, the nonsense syllables and poetry were taken at as widely different times of the day as possible, — the nonsense syllables being read in the morning before breakfast, and the poetry in the evening after supper. It was also deemed best to ab- stain from all reading for a period of at least one half hour both before and after thei reading of each passage. On June 28th, 1909, I finished with the passage of poetry consisting of 100 stanzas. On July 10th I finished with the set of 300 nonsense syllables. Feeling that the experiment had continued long enough with these materials as subject matter, I decided to repeat the experiment using this time, however, digits and prose. This new series of experiments was started on August 1st, 1909, and continued for three years, i. e., up to May 2d, 1912.^^ The nonsense syllables were selected and made into sets after a certain definite manner.-'- A typical set is given on page 73. As may be seen from Plate 3, the poetry ranged in length from one to one-hundred stanzas both for the "continu- ous" method and the "once-per-day" method. The poetry selected was what is known as common meter, i. e., iambic verses in which the first line contains four feet and the second line three feet. As shown on Plate 3, however, (where the "exceptional" tj-pe is printed in red) every fourth set of poetry used had six instead of four lines in each stanza. A typical example of the first type of poetry is given below. It is a stanza from Thomas Moore's The Ring.-^ -'This was so only for Ihe prose. The digits were diseontimied on August 10, 1010. '--Described in foot-note No. 40, page 75. ■-■=Thi.s poem consists of sixty-two stanzas. The first 50 stanzas were used as the "50-stanza" set. As may be noted, tlie poem is of the same type as "The Ancient Mariner." Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 59 "The female fiend no sooner heard Than, with reluctant look, The very ring that Rupert lost, She from her finger took." As a typical example of the second type of poetry, I give Thomas Hood's The Dream of Eugene Aram, "He told how murderers walked the earth, Beneath the curse of Cain,— With crimson clouds before their eyes. And flames about their brain ; For blood has left upon their souls Its everlasting stain !" It will be seen that this poem is of the same type as that of The Ballad of Reading Gaol. My reason for using these two types of poetry was that I wanted to see if the addition of two extra lines made any material difference with either the ' ' once- per-day" method or the "continuous" method and if so, if the difference was greater with one than with the other. ^* - It will be seen from a glance at Plate 3 that by neither method does the addition of the two extra lines malte any perceptible dif- ference in the time taken for learning, — outside of the fact, of course, that the stanza takes longer to read. For prose I decided to use selections from four different authors, alternating one with the other as shown on Plate 7. The authors chosen were Spencer, Hugo, Schopenhauer and IngersoU. Due to the fact that I alternated one author with the other, I at first made four separate curves, one for each author. It was seen, however, that there was so little difference with the different authors that one curve was thought sufficient. The selections from Schopenhauer took a somewhat longer time, due undoubtedly to the fact that the sentences are short and that one sentence has very frequently but little logical connection with what has preceded. Personal interest in each author and his subject matter is here of such importance that this factor must be taken into consideration. This, however, was one reason for using the different authors. Here, as in the experiment with nonsense syllables and poetry, the digits were read in the morning before breakfast -•'Stanzas of the shorter type averaged 24 to 25 words ; stanzas of the longer type averaged 35 to 40. In Plate 3 in computing the total number of words I used the figures 25 and 35 respectively. 60 Memory and the Learning Process and the prose in the evening. No passage Avas started until the one then in hand was finished.'' Not only were no other memory experiments allowed, but ordinary reading was ab- stained from for a period of at least one half an hour both be- fore and after the reading of the passage. Since, with the "once-per-day" method, only one reading was allowed each day, the actual time taken per day was very short. "Where, however, the passage was memorized in one sitting, the time in the case of the longer passages was frequently very long, e. g., that for the 1500 word passage being nearly two hours. Passages longer than this were not attempted, except by the divided time method, which method was continued up to a passage of 15,000 words. Space permits neither a detailed statement and explanation of the facts shown in the various plates and tables nor the various psychological conclusions that might be deduced there- from. To a certain extent, however, these are self evident upon comparing the curves of the different materials with each other, — and again comparing the same materials as memorized by the two different methods. Take, e. g., Plate 8 with its tables. It will be seen that the passage of 500 words was memorized in as few days as the 250 word passage — nay, it was even one day less. But now, as the passage is twice as long, the total time consumed was twice as great and, there- fore, the time taken varies, approximately, directly as the length of the passage. The same relation holds true for the digits and nonsense syllables but not to the same extent, for the number of days needed for 200 nonsense syllables is con- siderably greater than that needed for 20. By the ' ' one-read- ing-per-day" method, however, it is evident that a long pas- sage (or set of nonsense syllables) is learned in nearly as few days as a short passage. Eeferring again to Plate 8. We have noted that the 500-word passage was memorized in as few days as the 250-word passage and that, therefore, the total time varied directly as the length of the passage. Looking at -"It should be mentlonec] that in the attempted reproductions, one error was allowed for every fifty nonsense syllables or digits. In the case of the poetry one "help" (but never more than one) was allowed for every three stanzas, e. g., for the ten-stanza passage three "helps" were allowed. In the case of the prose one mistake (of one word) was allowed for every 100 words. Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 61. Plate 7, however, (which shows the curve for the "continu- ous" method) we observe, that, whereas the 100-word passage was memorized in 9 minutes, the 500-word passage took 52 minutes, — in other words, multiplying the passage by five, multiplies the time by six. The red "curve" on Plates 3 and 7 show the amount of time spent on the various passages by the "continuous" method. The black curve shows the total time spent in reading similar passages by the "once-per-day" method, — the reading having been done at the rate of five minutes per 1000 words. A com- parison of the two curves would show that so far as poetry and prose are concerned, one method is as economical as the other, i. e., economical as far as time spent is concerned for the tenacity of impression is, of course, much greater by the "once- per-day" method. When we examine Plate 5, however, which shows two simi- lar curves for digits we find that the "once-per-day" method is considerably more economical. The same is seen in the plate for nonsense syllables — material which, like digits, is memorized in my own case by motor associations. As before said, Plate 3 shows (in red) the total time taken to learn by the "continuous" method the various passages of poetry ranging in size from one stanza to 100 stanzas, i. e., from 25 words to 2500 words. On the plate the ordinates represent the number of stanzas; the abscissas, the number of minutes needed. The average number of words in each stanza is about 25, except those jjrinted in red, which average 40 words the stanza. Plates 4 and 5 have already been touched on. It will be seen that here, as in the case of nonsense syllables, the total time taken by the "once-per-day" method is much less than is that by the "continuous" method. The plates show that the total time taken by the "once-per- day" method is, — for the poetry and prose, nearly always as long as by the "continuous" rdethod. For digits and non- sense syllables, however, i. e., material in which there is but little logical connection, there is a considerable saving of time by the "once-per-day" method. It may be said by way of objection that this is due to a more or less constant reviewing, 62 Memory and the Learning Process possibly unconscious, between the various readings. This is, of course, possible, but even if so does not materially alter the conclusions that may be drawn from the curves. As we have just said, the time by the " once-per-day " method varies approximately as the length of the material. When, however, we turn to the "continuous" method, we find that this relation holds only for the shorter passages. As soon as the passage becomes too long for the mind to grasp it as a whole, the time mounts up rapidly, as shown, e. g., on Plate 7. "This is much more strikingly shown when we examine the curve obtained for the digits. Here we see that although it took only 5 minutes to learn 24 digits, it took 2 hours and 34 minutes to learn 200 — more than 31 times as long instead of 8. In short it is obvious that the " once-per-day " method is — to say nothing of giving a far superior retention — far more eco- nomical than the "continuous" method. This is especially so for material memorized by motor associations such as non- sense syllables or digits. ' ' -" One thing, probably the most important, that may be said in favor of the "once-per-day" method so far as economy is concerned, is the fact that material memorized by this method is retained for a much longer period than that memorized by the "continuous" method. This is probably its chief, if not its only advantage. As no other subject was available who would undertake all of the various tests just described, I have no complete curves for comparison. I have, however, the records" of six subjects on various parts of the test, e. g., one subject memor- ized a 500 word passage by the ' ' continuous ' ' method and then memorized it by the "once-per-day" method. Another subject memorized 48 nonsense syllables by the "continuous" method, 100 digits by the ' ' continuous ' ' method, a passage of 500 words by the " continuous "" method and 30 stanzas by the "once-per-day" method. Space does not admit an entire re- production of the records of these six subjects." As one would ■"D. O. Lyon. The Relation of the Length of Material to Time Taken for Learning, Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, A'^ol. IX, No. 14. -'Some of these" have already been given in Tables G, H, K, and N. Relation of Length of Material to Learning Time 6;> expect, however, in an experiment of this kind, the various subjects differed greatly. On another set of students-^ (fourteen Normal ("'oUege girls) a somewhat different test was tried, the results of which, since they take but little room and are valuable for comparison with some of the preceding tables, are given on the following page. Of the various individual differences in the group of six re- ferred to on the preceding page, I found the greatest to occur with digits and nonsense syllables when memorized by the "once-per-day" method. I mean by this that the results ob- tained for memorizing such material by the "once-per-day" method differed more widely from the "continuous" method (in my own case) than did prose or poetry. To elucidate this fact, I give in Table P, the results of a certain experiment. Briefly stated, this experiment was as follows : Sixteen subjects were selected from a list of over 100 as having averaged the best in a certain miscellaneous set of memory experiments. Ten of the sixteen subjects"'' occurred in the group of fourteen referred to on the preceding page. These sixteen names were then mixed up and eight selected at random for work on digits. The renlaining eight were al- lotted prose. Of the eight digit-subjects, four were given a set of fifty digits, the other four were given a set of 200 digits. Of the eight prose-subjects, four were given a passage of prose 250 words long and the remaining four were given a passage of prose 1000 words long. The subjects were then read the directions and rules of the experiment. Briefly stated these were that the subject would be called on to read the material allotted her once a day, and once only (Saturday and Sunday included) until she felt confident she could write the passage without error. The method of scoring these re- '"These fourteen students were selected from the .Junior and Senior classes of tbe Albanv Normal College and were selected as being the fourteen foremost out of over sixty students, i. e.. they stood highest in their general avei'age in class. ""All the subjects were girls. They averaged in age from 10 to •-':!. 64 Memory and the Learning Process ►J P3 < d d ri d d d d 1 d ■ .9 .5 a a a .9 'h a .gaa** a o T-l 10 iM 05 . . . b- , -. 10 . . fi 0) iH tH iH r-( tH in tH rH .9 . ri 05 fi ^ aaaaaaaaaaaaoos 10 ■* 05 t- 1:0 05 IM t- tH op . rH lNlNrH-*-*lN 71 40 . ■" 37^ 9fi 50 . '26 49 / A. H. 26.40 4.50 83 ^ - 4 F. Wi. 28.00 2.33 92 48 65 R. W. E. S. 28.00 29.56 7.00 15.00 75 49 '75 . 18 8 -25 , 38 17 -42 F. Sc. 31.00 10.25 67 20 43 "" B. 0. 31.75 5.50 83 . 19 J 38 J A.N. 32.00 9.16 71 21 ^ 45^ M. T. 33.50 13.40 60 7 18 H. M. F. K. 34.00 34.00 8.80 6.40 74 81 -67 18 21 ^19 40 30 ^33 J. M. 35.50 15.66 56 17 37 L.J. 36.08 15.00 58 ^68 29, .01 29 C. C. 37.00 16.50 55 1 ^21 36 = >-3 E. R. 40.80 15.16 63 39 53 S. T. 42.00 13.00 69 -69 . 14 -23 18 .40 , J. McC. 42.00 12.68 70 18 41 G. L. 44.16 4.50 90 46 52 E. T. 45.00 15.40 66 J 6 38 J Aver. 31.06 9.34 70 23 40 Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 121 TABLE III. 20 WORDS. NoKMAi. College Senioks. Girls. J 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 ] METHOD METHOD METHOD THREE ONE TWO Time of First Per Cent, of Learning, Time After Time Saved Score, Score, Subject Min. Ten Weeks, or Amount Per Cent. Per Cent. Min. Retained A. H. 4.40 1.03 77 1 43 1 53-^ H. B. 5.00 4.66 7 12 43 El. W. G. L. 7.00 7.66 3.88 43 4.66 39 -45 35 11 -26 50 39 -44 ^ Ed. W. 9.00 3.08 66 31 51 A.N. 9.50 6.00 37 . -40 22 J 2o 28 J ^^^ 52 J R. W. 10.25 5.00 51 20" - 4 A. T. 10.40 6.75 35 14 41 M. T. 11.00 7.50 S2 ^35 . 20 -20 40 -45 E. S. 11.00 8.75 21 17 39 B. 0. 11.25 6.75 40 27 49 J. M. 11.50 8.00 30 J 22 J 48 . F. St. 11.66 6.88 41 1 26 1 46 ' S. T. 12.00 4.40 63 3 21 F. Sc. 12.16 4.00 67 > 54 22 >22 40 -S7 1 C. C. 13.00 3.75 71 32 49 r *^' M. K. 13.25 3.66 72 19 30 H. M. F. Wi. 13.33 13.80 12.25 8 4.16 70 1 ^61 28, 17 1 "^^ 37 1 .3 J. Mc. 14.00 7.25 48 16 31 F. K. 14.33 4.00 72 ^68 16 V 18 27 33 E. R. 15.00 3.50 77 40 y XO J 50 f- "" J L.J. 16.00 2.08 87 7 38 E. T. 28.50 13.00 54 J 9 J 17 J 12 44 Aver. 11.88 5.62 51 21 39 122 Memory and the Learning Process. TABLE IV. PROSE. 100 WORDS. (The Diamond Beight Dawn). Normal College Seniors. Girls. 3 4 5 6 7 10 11 12 METHOD Ml STHO D METHOD THREE ONE TWO Time of First Per Cent, of Learning, Time After Time Saved Score, Score. Subject Min. Ten Weeks, or Amount Per Cent. Per Cent. Min. Retained Ed. W. 10.00 1.33 87 1 97 1 98 1 F. Sc. 10.40 2.16 79 44 89 A. H. 13.00 1.00 92 -78 ' 93 ^66 99 ■82 ' E. S. 13.00 4.40 66 33 50 El. W. 13.75 4.00 71 69 85 c. c. 14.33 4.25 70 J ■69 62 J .::q 73. f-59 7g . n R. W. 15.00 4.66 69 ' 52 1 !-8 F. Wi. 15.25 6.00 61 45 68 F. K. 16.08 4.50 72 -59 39 -53 91 -79 G. L. 17.00 11.25 34 45 72 H. B. 17.00 5.40 68 84 87 J. M 17.25 8.50 51 . 51 J 75 , F. St. 18.66 6.50 65^ 32 1 64 ^ H. M. 19.50 10.00 49 20 52 A. N. 22.25 9.00 60 -64 ^ 69 ^46 76 -64 B. 0. 23.25 9.50 59 76 80 A. T. 24.26 5.56 78 27 37 M. K. S. T. 25.16 26.08 7.40 4.00 71 85 ^ -66 50 42 1 ^^^ 95 = 60 -6 M. T. 28.00 15.00 46 46 L. J. 29.80 17.00 43 .63 56 .47 54 .70 E. R. 31.40 6.56 79 77 89 J. Mc. 34.56 6.88 80 49 55 E. T. 45.16 11.75 74 J 14 . 67 80 -67 Aver. 20.84 6.94 67 53 74 Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 12.: TABLE V. POETRY. 100 WORDS. (Gentle Ellen). Normal College Seniors. Girls. i 3 4 5 6 7 10 METHOD METHOD METHOD THREE ONE TWO Time of First Per Cent. of Learning, Time After Time Saved Score Score, Subject Min. Ten Weeks, or Amount Per Cent. Per Cent. Min. Retained El.W. 2.08 1.00 52 1 36 1 59"! H. B. 3.00 1.75 42 95 98 Ed. W. R. W. 3.25 5.00 .33 1.40 90 72 ^65 ■ 64 30 ^56 100 39 ^72' E. S. 7.00 4.50 36 11 36 A. H. 7.50 100 J ^61 ^^ ^59 '11 L r B. 0. 8.66 3.88 56" r G. L. 8.80 6.16 30 28 50 A. N. 9.50 4.40 54 !>57 57 ^63 55 ^74 F. Sc. 10.00 2.16 78 59 87 H. M. 10.00 5.25 48 77 82 F. Wi. 10.16 2.25 78 . 60 J 71 S. T. 10.50 2.00 81' 10 1 35 1 E. R. 10.56 3.80 64 78 92 M.T. 10.75 5.00 53 .62 51 :.43 84 .65 F. K. 12.25 7.16 58 73 i9 M. K. 13.40 5.08 62 23 44 E. T. 13.75 6.33 54 .65 24, -40 48- -J C. C. 14.00 3.08 78- 46 1 *" 67 -^ J. M. 15.75 6.25 60 36 47 J. Mc. 17.00 5.75 66 >68 39 .37 42 .49 F. St. 17.16 5.08 71 " 13 31 A. T. 19.75 5.50 72 32 40 L.J. 20.00 7.66 62 , 53. 68 . 73 -57 Aver. 10.83 4.00 63 50 65 124 Memory and the Learning Process TABLE A. it b Total c d Nons. e f s h i Average Devia- Subject. Material. Digits. Syll. Words. Prose. Poetry. Average tion. El. W..., ... 1 1 2 3 5 1 2.4 1.3 Ed. W.., ... 2 2 3 5 1 3 2.8 1.0 H. B ... 3 3 6 2 11 2 4.8 3.0 A. H.... ... 4 9 7 1 3 6 5.2 2.6 R. W..., ... 5 10 9 7 7 4 7.4 1.7 M. K ... 6 5 1 17 18 17 11.6 6.9 F. Wi..., ... 7 4 8 19 8 12 1012 4.2 F. Sc... ... 8 15 11 15 2 10 10.6 3.6 E. S ... 9 20 10 10 4 5 9.8 4.2 F. St.... ... 10 16 4 13 13 22 13.6 4.3 A. N ...11 13 13 6 15 9 11.2 3.0 F. K . .. 12 8 16 21 9 16 14.0 4.4 G. L ,..13 6 23 4 10 8 10.2 5.1 H. M..., ...14 12 15 18 14 11 14.0 2.0 J. M ...15 7 17 12 12 20 13.6 3.9 C. C ...16 11 19 16 6 19 14.2 4.6 B. O ...17 17 12 11 16 7 12.6 3.1 M. T.... ... 18 14 14 9 20 15 14.4 2.5 A. T ...19 19 5 8 17 23 14.4 6.3 S. T ...20 18 21 14 19 13 17.0 2.8 E. R.... ... 21 22 20 22 22 14 20.0 2.4 L. J ...22 21 18 23 21 24 21.4 1.7 J. McC. ... 23 24 22 20 23 21 22.4 1.3 E. T ... 24 23 24 24 24 18 22.6 1.8 This table shows at a glance the rank (as to time of first learning) of each of the 24 Normal College Seniors for each of the materials memorized by her (Tables I-V). The average rank of each individual is given in Column h, and, as will be noted, compares closely with the rank for all the material shown in Column b. Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 125 sation. The entries in Tables XV and XVI correspond to the average results from all materials combined, as presented at the bottom of Tables XI-XIV. Another way of combining the results from the use of the different materials is illustrated in Table XVII, which again is derived from Tables I-V. The 24 individuals in the group were arranged in the order of their success in each test, and were given numbers indicating their "rank" or position in the group. The table gives the rank of each individual in each performance and his average rank in speed of learning, in retention as measured by the saving method ("method 3"), in recall ("method 1"), and in recall after partial re- learning ("method 2"). The average deviation of rank of each individual in each of these kinds of performance is also given. (In Appendix D may be seen the table from which Table XVII is taken.) Finally, Table XVIII is derived from the preceding table for a purpose which will be explained later. Time of Initial Learning. — ^An examination of any of the tables will reveal the fact that the time of initial learning varies widely with the different subjects, and that these differences in learning are more marked than the individual differences in relearning. In other words, it may be stated as a general rule that with a given number of individuals there will be a greater difference in their time of memorizing than in their retentive capacity. Generally speaking, with a group of 20 or 30 subjects the time taken by the quickest learner is to the time taken by the slowest learner as 1 : 4. This, however, would seem to depend partly upon the nature of the material learned. Among the 24 Albany Normal College seniors (Tables I to V) it will be noticed that with digits the time of the quickest learner is to the time of the slowest learner as 1:13; that with nonsense syllables the ratio is 1:4; that in the case of ivords the ratio is 1:7; that for prose it is 1 : 5, whereas for poetry it is 1 :10. The difference in these ratios is, of course, largely a matter of chance. Take, for example, the table for words; here the slowest learner takes 28 minutes and 30 seconds, whereas the next slowest learner takes only 16 minutes, It is obvious that 126 Memory and the Learning Process a mucli fairer form of comparison is that of comparing the average of the first four with the average of the last four. Doing this, we find that the ratios are as follows : For digits, 1:6; nonsense syllables, 1:3; words, 1:3; prose, 1:3; poetry, 1:5." An individual who is a quick learner of one sort of material tends, upon the whole, to be a quick learner of other sorts also. This is seen most conveniently in the first part of Table XVII, which shows the ranks of 24 individuals in quickness of learn- ing five sorts of material. Some individuals stand consistently high, and some consistently low. There is, however, a good deal of shifting from one material to another, and this shifting finds expression in the coefficient of correlation between the ranking in two materials. As computed by the rank-difference method,. the average correlation between the speed of learning any two sorts of material is, for this group of subjects, +.51. For the 17 subjects whose records are given in Tables VI-X, the average correlation comes out a little lower, -\-A2. The shifting of an individual's rank from one material to another is partly due to the accidental factors inherent in a single test, and partly, no doubt, to actual differences in the efficiency of the inidvidual's powers of memorizing different classes of material. Interval between Learning and Reproduction. — This in- terval varied in different experiments, as indicated in the several tables. In the majority of our experiments the in- terval that was allowed to elapse for digits and nonsense syllables was either three days or one week, whereas for words, prose, and poetry it was much longer, being from three to ten weeks. In an investigation of this nature, where we are concerned primarily with acquisition as related to reten- tion, we can, of course, choose any interval we wish. We might wait six months and still find a relation between learn- ing and retention. That this relation would differ with the interval, however, appears probable from certain tests we have "Even when we thus obtain our ratio by comparing the average of the first four subjects with the average of the last four, the P. E. is very large. It is a noticeable fact, however, and one of some interest, that with every group of subjects the greatest difference (ratio) occurs with the digits. Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 127 Subject B. B. F. Wo. E. F. J. S. E. C. I. S. A. D. B. C. El. F. A. Q. E. A. E.H. M. J. G. H. H. A. R. B. M. N. TABLE VI. 20 DIGITS. Class ix Experimental Psychology. Giels. 2 3 456789 Time of First Learning, Time After Min. One Week, , Min. 3.40 4.50 6.00 6.50 6.50 8.00 8.56 9.00 9.80 10.00 11.00 1].30 11.80 12.66 14.00 16.16 24.25 METHOD THKEE Per Cent, of . Time Saved "or Amount Retained 0.75 4.16 0.88 2.75 1.33 1.66 9.66 1.33 0.50 3.25 2.40 2.25 1.33 4.80 0.16 1.66 6.75 -57 -58 -80 82 -58 81 METHOD ONE Score, Per Cent. 39 19 34 27 29 35 8 10 38 24 20 18 25 6 40 27 22 -30 1 K25 -21 -25 24 24 10 II METHOD TWO Score, Per Cent. 30 30 32 Aver. 10.20 2.68 70 25 31 128 Memory and the Learning Process Subject J. s. E. H. B. B. E. F. E. C. G. H. El. F. B. C. A. D. A. Q. M. J. I. S. F. Wo. H. A. E. A. R. B. M. N TABLE VII. 12 NONSENSE SYLLABLES. Class ii«^ Experimental Psychology. Giels. 2 3 456789 Time of First Learning, Time After Min. One Week, Min. 6.08 6.75 7.50 9.16 10.00 11.00 11.50 12.00 13.66 14.00 14.25 16.00 17.00 18.50 19.33 31.00 32.56 0.75 1.33 1.16 1.66 2.80 5.56 1.00 2.16 5.33 3.08 5.16 1.66 9.16 4.88 5.50 12.08 19.25 METHOD THREE Per Cent, of Time Saved or Amount Retained METHOD ONE Score, Per Cent. 10 METHOD TWO Score, Per Cent. ^55 Aver. 14.72 4.74 71 30 42 Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 129 anbject B. B. E. F. E. A. F. W. E. C. B. C. A. D. A.Q. J. S. I. s. M. J. H. A. G. H. El. F. E. H. M. N. R. B. TABLE VIII. 20 WORDS. Class in Expekimental Psychologs. Gikls. 2 3 456789 Time of First Learning, Time After Min. Ten Weeks, Min. 5.80 7.33 7.40 8.00 8.56 9.16 9.16 10.08 10.08 12.00 13.16 13:50 14.50 15.08 15.33 17.00 19.00 2.08 2.66 2.25 6.00 4.50 4.66 6.40 4.80 1.88 6.00 6.33 7.33 7.75 6.40 4.25 5.00 6.08 METHOD THREE Per Cent, of Time Saved or Amount Retained •56 -45 K50 -57 63 r6i METHOD ONE Score, Per Cent. -28 ■15 J -23 19 J 21 21 10 METHOD TWO Score, Per Cent. -47 -37 ■37 33 42 35 Aver. 11.48 4.98 56 21 38 ]30 Memory and the Learning Process Subject B. B. E. C B. C. E. F. A. Q. I. S. H. A. G. H. M. J. E. H. El. P. M. N. R. B. E. A. J. S. F. Wo. A. D. TABLE IX. PROSE. 100 WORDS. (The Pbesent Study). Class in Experimental Psychology. Girls. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Time of First Learning, Time After Min. Ten Weeks, Min. 7.00 11.16 12.25 14.33 16.08 17.50 19.00 19.83 20.00 20.40 21.56 25.25 28.16 28.40 30.00 82.66 39.50 2.88 4.25 4.88 4.15 7.00 4.33 8.80 11.56 5.88 10.33 9.00 10.56 8.08 10.25 15.00 10.75 10.66 METHOD THREE Per Cent, of Time Saved or Amount Retained 59 1 60 I 71 J 56 METHOD ONE Score, Per Cent. ^51 21 10 U METHOD TWO Score, Per Cent. Aver. 21.38 7.88 63 36 68 Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 13 L Subject B. B. F. W. M. J. E. F. E. C. G. H. E. H. E. A. A. Q. K. B. A. D. El. P. B. C. J. S. I. s. M. N. H. A. TABLE X. POETRY. 100 WORDS. (To See a Man). Class in Experimental Psychology. Girls. 2 3 456789 METHOD THREE Time of First Per Cent, of Learning, Time After Time Saved Min. Ten Weeks, or Amount Min. Retained 5.16 7.00 7.33 8.50 9.56 11.25 11.50 11.80 12.00 12.16 12.16 12.75 13.00 14.00 14.00 14.56 14.75 3.56 31 2.50 64 2.40 67 3.50 59 4.33 55 8.08 28 2.66 77 5.08 57 2.50 79 0.00 100 6.56 46 6.00 53 3.50 73 0.33 98 4.25 70 5.25 64 5.50 63 -55 -54 70 r74 55 72 METHOD ONE Score, Per Cent. 10 11 II METHOD TWO Score, Per Cent. ^79 Aver. 11.26 3. J 64 60 74 132 Memory and the Learning Process TABLE B. a Subject. b Total Material. c Digits. a Nons. Syll. e Words. f Prose. s Poetry. h Average 1 Average Devia- tion. B. B .. 1 1 3 1 1 1 1.4 .56 B. F .. 2 3 4 2 4 4 3.4 1.32 E. C .. 3 5 5 5 2 5 4.4 1.76 B. C ,.. 4 S 8 6 3 13 7.6 2.88 A. Q ,.. 5 10 10 8 5 9 8.4 1.52 E. H.... ... 6 12 2 15 10 7 9.2 3.56 M. J ,.. 7 13 11 11 9 3 9.4 2.72 J. S ... 8 4 1 9 15 15 8.8 5.04 I. S ,.. 9 6 12 10 6 14 9.6 2.88 G. H ...10 14 6 13 8 6 9.4 3.28 P. Wo... ...11 2 13 4 16 2 7.4 5.70 El. F ...12 9 7 14 11 12 10.6 2.08 E. A ...13 11 15 3 14 8 10.2 3.76 H. A.... ... 14 15 14 12 7 17 13.6 2.68 A. D ...15 7 9 7 17 11 10.2 3.04 R. B ...16 16 16 17 13 10 10.2 4.28 M. N ...17 17 17 16 12 16 15.6 1.44 This table shows at a glance the rank (as to time of first learning) of each of the 17 girls of Tables VI-X for each of the materials memorized by her. The average rank of each individual is given in Column h, and, as will be noted, compares closely with the rank for all the material shown in Column b. Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 13;] made, where the longer interval gave a negative correlation. We have not studied the matter systematically, and the data here presented do not show any clear difference according to the interval employed. In general, we may suppose the difference in amount reproduced by quick learners and slow learners tends to become less, since the amounts retained by all approach zero with time. Amount Retained. Method 1. — Method 1 has already been described in Section 2 of the preceding chapter. It shows the amount that can be reproduced, after the lapse of a certain time interval, of the material originally memorized, this reproduction being with- out a fresh presentation. By consulting columns 7, 8 and 9 of any table it will be noticed that in the ease of all materials, both meaningless and logical, thete appears to be a positive correlation between quickness of learning and the amount retained. The score obtained by the first half** is, in general, better than that obtained by the last half. This is most marked in the case of prose and least marked with digits. In fact, with digits the score obtained by the last half is, in sev- eral cases, very nearly as high as the first half, and in the case of the high school students (Table XIV) the score of the last half is even better. In any case, however, the difference is small. With nonsense syllables, words and poetry the dif- ference is slightly in favor of the quick learners, although, oil the whole, the difference is but slight. The probable error, however, is in most cases so high that the value of index of correlation is considerably lowered. Were it not that what correlation we do obtain is positive for every table, the figures would have much less value. In the ease of prose and poetry a positive correlation is unquestionable. With prose, for example, the average score for the quickest learners is, as a rule, nearly double the score for the slow learners. This, in "In the following pages we shall by "First half" (or "Upper half") hereafter understand the quickest learners, and by "Last half" (or "Lower half") the slowest, or poorest, learners. 134 Memory and the Learning Process conjunction with the fact that the Pearson method gives a high index, makes the evidence practically conclusive. Method 2. — Concerning Method 2, little need be said, the individual diflferences being much the same as those observed by Method 1. With the exception of digits, the quick learners get the higher scores. Here again the greatest difference is with the prose and the least with the digits. The most noticeable fact with this method is that it gives the highest correlation of all, and that the correlation is high throughout, i. e., for all materials. The explanation of this is not hard to find. It lies in the fact that with Method 2, after the lapse of a certain number of days, the material is read once, and only once to the subject, after which reading he is asked to write down as much as possible. Obviously, the quick learner will get more from this one reading than the slow learner, and thus the index is raised. Method 3. — So many factors are involved in this method that it calls for a more lengthy discussion than either of the two preceding. With this method the correlation is, as a rule, negative, both by the Pearson method and by the rather crude "percentage method."*^ With the percentage method we find that with digits, words, and occasionally poetry, the quick- est learners retain less than the slower learners. With prose, on the other hand, the quicker learners would appear to retain more, while for nonsense syllables they stand about even. To repeat: Method 3 gives results that by no means in- variably agree with those obtained by the two preceding methods. This is due to the nature of the method, i. e., to the manner of computing the "percentage of time saved" and to the treatment of this as a measure of the amount retained. Whether this is fair to the quick learner is questionable. Ac- cording to Method 3, and, for that matter. Methods 1 and 2, those who memorize prose most quickly retain it better than those who memorize it more slowly. With poetry the same relation frequently holds, but the results are not uniform, aiid, as may be seen from Tables XI and XII, the quick learners "By tbis we mean the method shown in columns 4, 5, and 6, where the percentage of the first half of the class (comprising the quickest learners) is compared with the half of tb* 000>OI^OO* ^ „ MtDCMOO^^oocooq^. Ti;ioeoqpo a^BjaAYpj o^p^r-C^coevicoeot-^coiri'S-'eoiv^oitoisIioiri ^'.< ca H » B i-i S M ~ -() Q'^ H sw ^'1 o 3 o e5 ■oi o o 't-o)tDO>eooia)oof^eaootn^ 93ij JdAycM* CO ^' '- CO ^' c\i lo ^* ^ lo CO '^ u> T-' ^" T-' iri cvi 00 ^* o ■<- t-* CM^a-oocMCj^oo^cM'ttoqoqcMCjcj^oq^oq^otooo Oj>BjaAYc«3 CO ^* o r>^ CNJ c\i csi c\i ifl <£>* ci in T-' d 00 o c\i ^' CO oi Tt ^' w* T— Ol t— T-T— T-T- T-t— ^— ^— Wt-CNJt-T- T— W Wi-l^rH 1-1 rH(N " i-tCqC^rHiH tHiM SDJO AA"^rHiH'^M-^(MU3t-THOOOO'X''^[r-aS'*ThCOCOMOOM soiqBii.itg^ ^cDNooo5(-oiMOco'*t-oo'*'^oqi-(cDocotr-miA-<*< aSaaSUO^I r-l C^ r-lrn rH T-HiHrH iHNrHlMfM tHM S^tStQiM CiTt*0'^t™^oiC500COCDOOO^u:3ffOr-ti-lt~-lNu:5M'^ 9S0J-Ti~l |--oqo>r^p^i^^'r-c\ia»p'i-eoo aSuJaAY loioiniocNiiooi^towineNiwr^coioio^r^rj^odioco^ O to r^ CM (D CM O ^ 8jju.t9AV ddinod^'^^i^cModinoo'T-^in<^CMcoo6o>u>CMi^WcM' ifjaaOrT fMiCiHcooocDoo-^o-^MTPoiTHt-ioc-ccicococOiHoas OSlldSUOJsT =^'-' (NiHM (M W THr-lrHi-tNrHrHT-l sitSirr ioooit-cocn"*oM'<*^'X>t-(MiOiHcocqqpi-«o>ooioh*^cooq 93BJ9AY T^coctit^i-^^i-^^''4'tncocQ^*CMeocol£5rH05"^CarHC^t-i SD JO M 10lOTHOCOCDt>airHTt^Cgcqir0005DTHOOt-Tt*OieOoqO-^ '^-^*- 1-1 iH r-t rHj; rn iH i-l iH(M i-i iHiHi-I CNiHi-tcatMCSI S^JIStq; Nir3a50iHiHO'"*00asOiHCqCCTHl0CDt-00(5SOiH(MC0TP "■ iHi-liHiHiHiHi-liHTH-rHOacqMC^S^J CM* CO in 00 CM CO in N. cm'cm CM CO in CO ^^Kt=b fi^^S&'-JsifibOSi-if^': ■^ K -< « M 3 i4 H 1-; K <1 Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 145 five different materials. Table XVII, giving the ranks of 24 individuals in learning each material and in recalling and relearning it after an interval, affords an opportunity for look- ing for individuals who are consistently quick or slow learners, and for finding how well they do in retention. Table XVIII (extracted from Table XVII) gives the records of the most consistent individuals. TABLE XVIII. Average Ranks and ^'ariabilities of the Most Consistent Individuals. First Recall Relearning Individual Learning (Method 1) (Method 3) A. D. of A. D. A. D. Av. Rank Rank Av. Rank of Rank Av. Rank of Rank Ed. W 2.8 1.0 3.2 2.2 10.0 5.2 A. H 5.2 2.6 4.8 4.1 5.4 5.4 K. W 7.4 1.7 12.8 2.1 11.0 2.4 A. N 11.2 3.0 10.2 1.9 14.8 3.4 S. T 17.0 2.8 21.4 2.0 9.8 7.2 E. R 20.0 2.4 4.0 0.8 7.0 5.1 J. Mc 22.4 1.3 14.6 1.5 11.5 3.3 B. T.'. 22.G 1.8 22.8 1.4 12.4 4.0 General Average 12.5 3.2 12.5 3.5 12.5 5.0 Regarding these individuals, it may be noted that : Ed. W., while consistently high in learning and recall, oc- cupies a medium and rather variable position in relearning. A. H. stands high on the average throughout, but is rather variable in recall and relearning. E. W. and A. N. are consistently medium throughout. S. T. is consistently low in learning and recall, but variable in relearning. E. E., the most interesting case, is consistently low in learn- ing, consistently high in recall, and high, though only mod- erately consistent, in relearning. J. Mc. is consistently low in learning and consistently me- dium in recall and relearning. E. T. is consistently low in learning and in recall, and con- sistently medium in relearning. There is no one, then, who is consistently high throughout, 146 Memory and the Learning Process or who is consistently low throughout, though there are two who are consistently medium throughout. On the other hand, no one who is consistently high in learn- ing is consistently low in retention. But there is one indi- vidual — E. E. — ^who is consistently low in learning and con- sistently high in retention ; and another — J. Mc. — who is con- sistently low in learning and consistently medium in reten- tion. It is reasonable to suppose that these two persons, espe- cially the former, overlearned the material on the first learn- ing. Through excess of caution they delayed presenting themselves for the test of learning until they were extra sure of their material, so we may suppose. If so, this moral trait of caution would produce the spurious appearance of a con- nection between slow learning and good retention. Examination of Tables VI-X shows that among the 17 in- dividuals included there is one who was consistently low in learning and rather consistently medium in retention, and one who is consistently medium in learning and rather consistently high in retention, but none who is consistently high in learning and consistently medium or low in retention. Of the 41 individuals whose records are here presented in full, then, there is no case of an individual who can be defi- nitely classed as a quick learner and as a poor retainer. There are several quick learners whose average position in retention is considerably lower than their position in learning, but the variability of their position as regards retention makes it im- possible to place them definitely. Intellectual Standing {Mental Ability).— A. comparison of the results given by the various groups of subjects (e. g., hos- pital attendants with college students) leads us to suspect that there is a direct relation between "capacity" or ability to learn and general intelligence. Most of those who have investigated this matter have arrived at the same conclusion. Jacobs,*** for example, states that there is a "notable con- comitance" between school standing and "span of prehen- sion." Others, however, Bolton and Ebbinghaus, for ex- '^Mina, Vol. 12, 1887. Others who have obtained a positive correlation, though not so Msrh as Jacobs, are Binet, Bourdon, Burt, Pohlmann, Smedlev, Winch and Wessley. Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 147 ample, deny that any such correlation exists. Their results, however, were derived in large part from examinations of single groups that were fairly homogeneous and not by a comparison of one type of intellectuality with another. We cannot here go into a detailed examination of all the results shown in the tables, for the reason that space does not permit giving the complete data. But we may say that, to a certain extent, the correlation depends upon the material used. It was found that where the material is logical in char- acter, especially in the case of prose, the college graduates do better than clerks and office men of the same average age, and that these do better than asylum attendants ; that college students do better than inmates of reformatories, and that Barnard College seniors do better than the female servants of the same age. The differences, however, are not marked. Upon taking any one form of material contrary results may obtain. Thus, e. g., the business men and clerks do slightly better with prose than do college students of the same age, while with poetry they do worse. With nonsense syllables the order is very much the same as for prose, although here the senior college students do best of all. With digits the group of clerks and business men do better than any others, excepting the classes in experimental psychology. We cannot help but infer that this is due in a large measure to practice. Such statements as the above, however, are more or less loose, for the reason that they refer only to groups of subjects. Some of the hospital attendants may have been more intelli- gent than some of the college students. More exact results were obtained in classes where the general rank in class of each scholar could be ascertained, all his studies being taken into consideration. An examination of the class records of the 132 normal-college students proved "that the students who rank highest in their classes and who can be classed as the most intelligent have, as a rule, the best memories.^" With the group in question the correlation between memory and standing in class was found to be .31. ■"D. O. Lyon. Jour, of Phil., Psych, and Sci. Methods, 1912, Vol. IX, page 74. 148 Memory and the Learning Process Social Standing (Occupation, Environment and Moral Standing). — By social standing we mean not only one's stand- ing in society from a "worldly point of view," but also from the point of view of the sociologist. Other things being equal, we wish to determine if any one occupation is more conducive to tenacity of impression than another. It will be noted that this problem is closely linked with the preceding, and as we used as subjects prisoners and reformatory inmates, we may say that it also bears on morality. The group differences here, like those for mental ability, differ with the materials used. The differences where the total material is considered are best seen by consulting and comparing the appropriate tables. With digits no correla- tions of any account were obtainable, though here, as before said, the business men and clerks seem to do better than any of the others, with the exception of the group in experimental psychology. The business men are rather slow in learning the nonsense syllables, and their degree of retention of these is worse than any other group, with the exception of the gram- mar-school students. With words they stand on a par with the high-school students, but for prose they seem to do slightly better, both for time of initial learning and for relative amount forgotten. This is what we should expect, since we have found that there is a positive correlation between mental ability and memory capacity. With the poetry, however, they do not do so well as the high-school students, and even drop slightly below the grammar-school students. The inmates of the reformatories, and, to a slightly less extent the prisons, rank fairly high for digits and ivords so far as quickness of learning is concerned. For nonsense syl- lables they do not rank so high. For prose and poetry they stand very well, ranking even higher than the college students. I feel that the explanation of this seeming discrepancy lies in the fact that the minds of reformatory inmates are very receptive while in confinement. Their life being for the most part a dull monotony, they welcome any novelty, and enter upon the experiment with considerable zest. This is undoubt Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 149 edly even more true as explaining their high degree of reten- tiveness, for we are led to believe from a study of the "intro- spections" handed in that they review the material to them- selves as a diversion. The average time for the criminal group is probably increased considerably by the fact that such groups invariably contain a small percentage of inmates of feeble mental mentality, either bordering on idiocy, or afflicted with one of the numerous psychoses. Age. — Speaking of memory in its broadest sense, we may say that memory capacity increases with age. This statement is general in the extreme, in that the rate of increase varies with the material used. Generally speaking, it was found that the increasie in efficiency with age is greater in the case of prose than it is in poetry, and in both of these greater than for either digits or nonsense syllables. Several investigators have performed experiments from which they conclude that there is a state of maximal efficiency that comes in the "teens." At this period they maintain that the memory is stronger than at any other period, whether preceding or following it. To me it seems that the materials they have used have been too meager to deduce any sucJi general conclusion. For example, Bernstein and Bogdanoff find this special "memory period" to occur about the age of 15. The only material they used on which they based this conclusion consisted of geometrical figures. To me it seems that the only conclusions one can derive from such an experi- m'ent is to say that when geometrical figures are presented to various subjects in a certain way and then tested for rec- ognition after the lapse of a certain interval, the subjects around the age of 15 do, on the average, better than older students and adults. With Method 1 I find that high-school students averaging from 16 to 17 years in age retain more of the poetry than do the younger grammar-school students and the older college students. With prose the height of efficiency, as before said, appears much later in life, and the more abstract and difficult the material, the later it appears. This would probably have its limit, however, and even with Kant's "Critique" men of 150 Memory and the Learning Process 50 would undoubtedly do better tban men of 80. The difficulty experienced by the younger children in memorizing a non- sense syllable is undoubtedly ascribable to many causes. Probably the chief of these is that nonsense syllables are un- interesting, and though children may have a better retentive capacity than adults, their attentive capacity is decidedly inferior. Sex. — Our object here is to discover if there is any differ- ence in retentive capacity between the male sex and the female sex as a whole, and in particular if there is any difference between the sexes in the relation of the time of learning to the amount retained. An examination and comparison of my tables (including some not here reproduced) show that as a whole the women and girls do better in their initial learning than do the men and boys. Thus, e. g., the Barnard College girls memorized the total material in a shorter time than did Columbia College men of the same age. For digits, words, nonsense syllables and poetry the girls average better than the boys. With prose the women do better with the passage starting "The diamond bright dawn," — the men, with the passage starting "The present study of monistic philosophy," but the differ- ence in each case is small. These statements apply only to time of first learning. When we conae to retentiveness, as ascertained by averaging Methods 1, .2, and 3, we find that though the girls still hold their superiority over the men for digits, nonsense syllables, and poetry, the men stand equal with them in the case of words, and for prose even do better. Summary and Recapitulation of the Main Results. In the preceding chapters we have endeavored to set forth, more or less in detail, the various results obtained from the experiments performed. In a few cases conclusions were drawn, but in the main they were reserved for the present chapter. Any attempt to classify these conclusions in a strict and exact manner meets with failure. An analysis of them, how- Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 151 ever, shows that they may be roughly put under two groups : (1) Those relating to methods and modes of experimentation and correlation and that partake more of the nature of inferences; (2) those results that are drawn from the experi- mental data given in the tables, be their limitations and im- perfections what they may. We shall now give a brief reca- pitulation of the results, conclusions and inferences of these two groups. (Several of the following have been drawn from an examination of the "introspections.") Group 1. — (1) Memory is not a distinct, separate and con- crete faculty of the mind, but is complex in the extreme. Experiments such as those described in this and the preceding chapter, being limited in character, apply but to a small part of mentality — and hence memory. (2) Association and retention are closely related. It is a question as to how much we should consider the former in investigating the matter. (3) Memory should not be confused with attention, nor should this latter even be considered as part of it. We have found that many of the experiments that are supposedly performed on memory are really experiments on attention — or on attention combined with some other faculty. (4) The relation of quickness of learning to retentiveness depends upon the method used of ascertaining this "retentive- ness." The different methods [v. p. 98) give opposite re- sults, and yet, in one sense of the word, one method is as "correct" as another. (5) Two people may have equal degrees of retentiveness, but very unequal degrees of reproduction. To test briefly the power to recall is but to test a certain factor of memory. (6) With the same subjects and the same method of ex- perimentation, different materials give different results. (7) In testing "memory" when taken in the fullest sense of the word we should test not only the so-called "rote" memory, but we should also consider the subject's ability to perceive relationships and associations, and his ability to memorize them. 152 Memory and the Learning Process (8) In relearning it is impossible to distinguish a facility of forming new associations from a retention of subliminal associations. This disadvantage is carried by Method 3. (9) "Method 1" has several drawbacks. The chief of these is that reproduction without a fresh presentation of the mate- rial originally learned reveals only the strongest of the original impressions — the so-called "supraliminal associa- tions." (10) An examination of over 400 "introspections" would seem to show that it does not pay to attempt to multiply the various forms of imagery. The quickest learners employ the type, or combination of types, to which they ar.e naturally accustomed. (11) A factor that must be taken into consideration in inves- tigating retention is the general attitude of the learner toward his work. If he is much interested in the problem in hand and takes great interest in his task, he naturally takes more care, and consequently is able to retain longer than another who has not this same feeling of interest. Where there is zeal and desire in learning, there follows an earnestness and interest in the work which will eventually result in greater retentiveness. This is particularly noticeable in the tests on subjects who are working in psychological laboratories, — sub- jects interested in experimental psychology. Group 2. — (1) With any given number of individuals it may be stated as a general rule that they will differ more in the time they take to memorize than they will in retentive capacity. (2) The students who stand highest in their various studies and who prove upon examination to be "the most intelligent" have, as a rule, the best memories. They not only learn more quickly, but they retain better. (3) Those who employ logical associations and visual imagery in the memorizing of a series of words or nonsense syllables recall them more slowly than do those who memorize in an auditory or motor manner. The latter type of subjects reproduce the associations easily and quickly immediately after the first learning, but forget them just as promptly. Relation of Quickness to Retentiveness 153 (4) As a general rule it is best to memorize thoroughly be- fore attempting to recall. When in doubt, do not waste time and form confusing associations by continuing the attempt, but consult the text immediately. (5) In general, the women and girls do better in their initial learning than do the men and boys. In retentiveness, however, the men and boys are, on the whole, slightly supe- rior, but this is not so for every material. (6) The quickest learners tend to learn their material more as a icJiole than do the slower learners, and this is invariably so with the second learning. The quick learner only divides his material into parts when he is totally unfamiliar with it. (7) The quick learners tend more to employ rhythm in the learning of digits, nonsense syllables and words than do the slow learners. If the element of rhyme or rhythm enters, it aids the ability to reproduce after learning. Those who learn by means of rhythm can somehow or other reproduce better than those who do not employ such methods of rhythm. (8) The relation of quickness of learning to retentiveness depends on many factors, the most important of which are method) material, and interval. (9) A change in the interval does not affect different forms of material to the same de,gree. (10) As to the relation of quickness of learning to reten- tiveness, the most general statement that can be made is that those who learn quickly remember longest if the material is logical in character. Where the material is "illogical," espe- cially if it is memorized by "motor associations," the converse is true. This, however, has many exceptions, depending both upon the material and upon the method used. The exceptions are most notable in the case of nonsense syllables and words. (11) There would appear to be a positive correlation be- tween quick learning (be it of sense or nonsense material) and retention of logical material (ideas). The correlation, however, is low. Quick learning is no guarantee that the time of retention -w-ill be relatively long. We should not consider economy in learning merely as economy in time spent in learning. 154 Memory and the Learning Process (12) By Method 1 the score obtained by the first half of the class is invariably better than that obtained by the second half."' The difference between the first quarter of the class and the last quarter is naturally even more marked. The dilference is found to be greatest in the case of prose and least in that of digits. With prose we frequently find that the first quarter of a class of 24 will remember (by Method 1) half as much again as the last quarter. (13) The experiments on school children show that girls from the years of 10 to 24 learn more quickly than boys of the same age. The results also show that the number of retained members of any series increases from year to year. (14) There is a positive correlation between education and memory. Inmates of prisons and attendants in State hos- pitals do not do as well as boys of 15 years of age. Education thus has much the same effect upon retentiveness as has age. (15) With both nonsense syllables and words the first few syllables (or words) of a series are retained longer -than those in the middle of the series. "Kxecpting in the case of two groups of stibjects, the digits. CHAPTEE V. THE EDUCATIONAL VALUE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL EESEAECH, with special reference to ECONOMY IN LEAENING and ' MNEMONIC SYSTEMS. Meaning of "economy In learning." Multiplicity of factors to be considered, because a method is the most economical in time does not mean that it is really the most economical. Permanence of retention and facility of use of greater importance than amount of time spent. "Sgavoir par coeur n'est pas sgavoir." Mnemonic systems. No limit to the number of digits the mnemo-technician can recite. The mnemo-technician assumes an excellence of memory that in reality he does not possess. Forgetting. The process of forgetting a selective one. Psychology in its relation to the science of education. Practical value of the results of psychological experimentation. Scientific conclusions as a rule are but relative and subject to change. The introspective method v. the experi- mental method. A large part of the writings of introspective psychology con- sist of loose, indefinite and unwarranted generalizations that have never been subject to experimental investigation. Only by experiment can we learn the truth. Experiment slowly changing the general aspect of the old psychology. Exp. psych, has performed its best services in correcting erroneous ideas con- cerning the mind's actions. Exp. psych, still in its infancy. Psychology still remains in large part a science of probabilities based on experimental data the validity of which is frequently doubtful. It will have been noticed in the preceding pages that several problems of interest to pedagogy have been touched upon. The chief of these is probably that of economy in learning. A definition of economical learning is not as easy as it may at first sight appear. We are too much in the habit of consid- ering that economy in time and economy in learning are synonymous terms — ^that a saving in the one means a saving in the other. In judging the degree of economy in any cer tain method of learning the following points should be con- sidered: (1) Amount of time spent; (2) amount of labor (energy or mental force) expended; (3) leng*th of retention; (4) amount (completeness) of retention; (5) fidelity and exact- 155 156 Memory and the Learning Process ness of reproduction ; a, — as to the wording — or acquisition of elements; b, — as to the idea — or formation of associations; (6) rapidity of reproduction; (7) degree of comprehension or understanding (this includes form of imagery employed as well as nature and amount of association employed). It will be seen that the problem is not a simple one, and that the relative proportions of the factors we have mentioned may vary with the same individual, this depending upon the nature of the material and the purpose for which it is in- tended. The aim — or goal — of learning is not always the same, and what may be considered as an " economical" method for one form of material- may not be for another. In studying his lessons the average schoolboy's sole aim is to be able to repeat once, and once only, the knowledge before him — to answer the question when called on. Acquisition with much a similar aim is, however, also witnessed in the adult, e. g., in the minister with his sermon, the politician with his speech and the actor with his piece. What we wish to emphasize here is that though a certain method may result in leading rapidly to an immfediate recitation, it does not follow that it will be the most economical method where permanent retention is desired. As we have already seen* a certain method may consume a greater amount of time, but secure the desired effect, viz., that of permanent and lasting retention. It wUl be seen that no set rule can be laid down. Generally speaking, all that we may say is that that method is the most economical that attains the end desired (1) in the shortest time, (2) with the least trouble,** and (3) with the minimum degree of fatigue. The seven factors that we have mentioned are not all equally susceptible of measurement. Time can be measured with a watch — and in like manner length of retention. But to meas- ure the amount of mental work — the amount of energy ex- pended — is not so easy. A few things, such,- for example, as number of repetitions during a stated interval, may be taken into consideration and thus help us in forming an opinion, but this must always rest but approximate. *We refer to the "once-per-day" method. **A "twice-per-week" method, e. g., may he economical as far as "total time'' is concerned, and not at all fatiguing, but its observance may be extremely troublesome and annoying. Educational Value of Psychological Research ' 157 It is not always an easy matter to differentiate economy in time from economy in force. Not only is the actual amount of mental force saved by the use of a certain method impos- sible of determination, but we are unable to state the rela- tionship of the force saved to the time saved. A saving in time may or may not at the same time represent a saving in force. In short, about the only definite statement that we can make— and one that is self-evident— is that of two methods demanding equal mental force (both quantitatively and quali- tatively), and where the length of duration* is the same, that method is the most economical that necessitates the least ex- penditure of time. It should be borne in mind, however, that a saving in time may be accompanied by such a greatly-in- creased expenditure of energy that no true economy can be said to take place. In like manner, although a certain method may result in a saving in time of first learning, the amount of time the material is retained may be so short that no true , economy may be said to have resulted. But this is not all. Not only must we define what we mean by ' ' economical learning, ' ' but we must define what we mean by "learning." In our Introduction we endeavored to show that both "learning" (in the sense of knowing) and "mem- ory" were extremely vague and indefinite terms. We there stated, by way of example, that, although a person may be able to repeat a list of words, he may yet, in one sense of the word, not really know them. This we exemplified by way of one who learns a set of words by using only his motor-auditory memory. We are probably safe in assuming that most people recite the Creed or the Lord's Prayer in this manner. The prayer is started and goes on automatically, the subject's mind being occupied with other affairs. The same is, to a large extent, true of all that we learn when we use merely our auditory or auditory-motor memory. Teachers and educators have recognized this for some time. "Sgavoir par cceur n'est pas sgavoir," said Montaigne. A similar truth was recognized by Kant. "When a child," said he, "does not put into practice a rule in grammar, he cannot strictly be said to know it, even though he may be able to *Perinanence of retention. 158 Memory and the Learning Process recite it. On the contrary, he who invariably applies a cer- tain rule may be said to Moiv this rule, even though he may not be able to recite it. The best method of comprehension^ therefore, is to do— to use — to act." That which one learns the most thoroughly, and which. one remembers the longest is what one has realized the truth of, or has learned or in vented by himself. In discussing economy in learning, therefore, the question is not only alone one of what will give the best result as to economy in time, but what will give the best and most com- prehensive understanding. The astronomer who says "the distance to Mars from our earth is 35,000,000,000,000 miles" knows the fact in a way altogether different from the school- boy who repeats the same words. In like manner, however, the schoolboy may be said to know this better and. in a dif- ferent manner from the savage who may be taught to repro- duce, in a parrot-like manner, the same sentence. We may observe a similar, though not identical, case in a simple prob- lem in multiplication. A child may be able to give the correct answer for 8X9, and yet fail on 9 X 8. The fact of the mat- ter is that he has learned only the formula "8X9 = 72." He falls down on 9 X 8 because he does not know in as complete a sense as he should the meaning of "8 X 9 = 72." The case is similar, but not identical, with that which we gave of the astronomer. In one sense of the word, 8 X 9 = 72 is no more the same as 9 X 8 = 72 than BOX is the same as XOB. "While on this matter we cannot do better than quote from Wells — in an article* which, though not directly on the point in question, is nevertheless extremely elucidative : "Pragmatically, to know a thing is to have established an association or co-ordination path which causes us to react according to it. We know a thing more certainly according as we react more certainly or in any way more effectively as though that thing were so. Though the certainty has a differ- ent source, you do not know that two and two make four more certainly than the dervish knows that the prophet's paradise awaits him on death in battle for his faith. To say that one, *"A Note on the Retention of Acquired Capacities," Amer. Jour, of Psychol., 3915, Vol. XXVI, pages 66-67. Educational Value of Psychological Research 159 takes a few sigma longer to add six aiid five than one did two years ago, is another way of saying that he does not know their sum so well." The lesson to the teacher and educator should, however, be obvious : See that the child knows (understands) as completely as possible whatever he may have, to learn, and call in the "logical" memory in preference to any other form. With some of the subjects experiments were performed to ascertain the time necessary to memorize the nonsense syl- lables and words in the reverse order. One might, imagine that those subjects who knew the list best (in the original order) would be able to relearn it quickest in the reverse order. Although this was frequently the case, the inference is unjustifiable. Because one knows a poem by heart does not mean that he can repeat it backwards. Some subjects take almost as long to relearn a series of syllables backwards as they took to memorize them in the first place. The dif- ference depends to a great extent upon the maimer in which the work was originally performed — the form of imagery used and the nature of the associations made. - ' In discussing economy in learning permanence of retention and facility of use are of greater importance than amount of time spent. As we have seen in Chapter III, with the con- tinuous method the time of learning may be relatively short, but the permanence of retention relatively poor. Permanence ! of retention and amount of time spent are capable of being measured,, but, owing to its complex nature, facility of use is more difficult of precise measurement. As we have already stated, a man may be able to repeat a set^ words and yet, in one sense of the word, not know them J The same remark may be more or less applied to digitsj The remark is strictly applicable when the acquisition^ of the digits depends upon the employment of a mnemonic device. Several mnemonic devices exist for the "memorizing" of long lists of digits. One of the most commonly used is the following: The subject commences by establishing a bond or relationship between the ten digits and the various cou- sin the case of tbe muemo-technician we can hardly use the word memorize. 160 Memory and the Learning Process sonants of the alphabet. To him each digit invariably stands for and is represented by a certain consonant. It is obvious that to the individual employing such a system it is but a very simple matter for him to replace each number by its appropriate consonant. For example, 582783 is replaced by I, ng, r, d, s, nd., because to him 5 corresponds to I., 8 to ng., 2 to r., 7 to d., 8 to s., and 3 to nd., etc. "With these consonants before him he has a skeleton, so to speak, on which he can work. In order to create a phrase for them it is necessary that he in- troduces vowels. In this he allows himself the greatest lib- erty, putting them in wherever and in any number that he may desire. Thus, for example, with using the letters I, ng, r, d, s, nd. he may invent the phrase, "long roads end." As a matter of fact, the mnemo-technician ascribes to each digit two or more letters or combination of letters, thus giv- ing himself a larger selection, and, therefore, a greater facil- ity of forming a phrase. For example, the figure 4 may be represented by ze as well as by se, and the figure 6 by te as well as by (Ze. It is thus obvious that, given any reasonable number of digits (not more than 100 or 200), the mnemo-technician has no great trouble in preparing and memorizing his prose or poetry to fit them. All he has to do is to learn by heart the phrases he has formed — an affair that for 100 digits may not take more than fifteen minutes. It will be remarked that the process employed by the mnemo-technician in a case such as this is one of substitution, i. e., in place of committing digits or numbers to memory he commits phrases or sentences. In short, he translates his numbers into words, and with these he forms ideas that are relatively easy of retention. It will, of course, be remarked that the mnemo-technician cannot use the same phrase several times unless it so happens that the digits presented to him repeat themselves — an affair that is not probable if the digits are selected by chance. However, in view of the fact that he can (if he has an average memory) commit one-thousand lines of prose or poetry in one sitting, he thereby has at his command some ten or twelve thousand Educational Value of Psychological Research 16 L digits. As he is able to repeat these in exactly the same order as many times as he may be called upon to do so, he appears, to the ordinary observer, to have actually "memorized" them. If, however, the mnemo-technician is himself allowed to select his digits, the number that he is able to acquire is prac- tically unlimited. He may with ease recite three or four hun- dred thousand, even one million, and then, if called upon to do so, repeat them in exactly the same order. When once the method employed is understood, the seemingly remarkable feat becomes most commonplace. The way in which he works is as follows : Placing before him an easy and interesting poem (of, say, a couple of hundred verses) that he has pre- viously learned by heart, he substitutes, using the poem as a skeleton or framework, the consonants in each word with digits. Years of practice in this work give him such a facil- ity in this "substitution work" that he is able to "see" the appropriate digits stand out before him as he recites (to himself) his verses. The process is one of translation, so to speak. A couple of hundred ordinary verses will furnish him with about two thousand digits. Having finished these, he has only to again recite them, this time, however, increas- ing the number by one as the digit ' ' comes ' ' to him or appears before him. Further variations are made by subtracting one or by adding two, three, four, etc. It is thus evident that, without any great expenditure of mental energy, he may re- cite and re-recite one million digits — an affair that, on the face of it, would be impossible for brute memory to accomplish. In fact, when the digits are more than one hundred in number, even the most experienced of the lightning calculators cannot compare with the mnemo-technician. A comparison of the times taken to memorize digits between the mnemo-technician (Mr. Arnould) and the lightning calculator (Mr. Diamandi) may be seen in the following table, which we borrow from Binet's^ most excellent work on that subject: =A. BiNET. Psychologie des Grands Calculatenrs, Paris, 1894, p. 173. Mr. Diamandi. Mr. Arnould. 17 s. 20 s. Im. 15 s. 1 m. 45 s. 2 m. 15 s. 2 m. 30 s. 3 m. 2 m. 30 s. 4 m. 20 s. 2 m. 45 s. 7 m. 2 m. 45 s. 25 m. 15 m. Ih. 15 m. 20 s. 45 m. 162 Memory and the Learning Process Number of ,— Time necessary for acquisition.- digits acquired. 10 15 20 25 30 50 10O 200 As may be seen from the table above, it is only when the digits are less than twenty-five or thirty in number that brute, memory (be it even of a Diamandi) can surpass the perform- ance of the mnemo-technician. For example, to learn twenty digits takes Mr. Arnould 2 m. 30 s., whereas Mr. Diamandi takes but 2 m. 15 s. With twenty-five digits the respective times are nearly equal — Arnould, it will be seen, took 2 m. 30 s., Mr. Diamandi 3 m.^ 8 6 4 3 9 Vieux faucheur aime bieu. 2 5 7 G 2 Nie le cas, ou ecbafaud. 3 17 3 5 A moi ta gamelle. 5 18 4 3 La, — tu veux ramer. 2 3 5 8 1 Un homme a la fete. A comparison of the results obtained by the mnemo-tech- nician with a native memory such as that of Mr. Diamandi is both interesting and instructive. One remarks that with in- creased number of digits there is naturally an increase in time, but the increase in the two cases is not proportionate. Mr. Arnould is a mnemo-technician by profession, and frankly admits that his work is one of simulation and deceit ; he assumes an excellence of memory that in reality he does not possess. His work, as we have said, consists in substitut- ing ideas for numbers, or, to put it in other words, he gives to numbers an artificial significance which permits of their being easily retained and recalled. Other professionals are not so frank, and it is not always an easy matter to distin- guish the mnemo-technician from the individual who in reality does possess a remarkable memory for digits. In order to differentiate between the two an examination should be made ^As an illustration of the method employed we give below the phraSes and sentences employed by Mr. Diamandi during an actual reproduction by him of twenty-five digits : Educational Value of Psychological Research 163 along the following lines: (1) Number of digits capable of being memorized, i. e., extent of memory; (2) rapidity of acquisition; (3) rapidity of repetition— verbal repetition; (4) length of time remembered. * * * * * * ,:- In connection with economy in learning it is necessary that we discuss forgetting. The process of forgetting is more or less based upon one of selection. In order to have an effective logical memory we should not burden ourselves with the un- essential, The act of forgetting judiciously, therefore, is as important a function as that of retaining judiciously. ' ' Selec- tion," says James, "is the very keel on which our mental ship is built. If we remembered everything, we should, on most occasions, be as ill-off as if we remembered nothing." Our memory of each day's events is constantly disappearing and being obliterated. Passed in review, after a few days' inter- val but few of the sensations previously received, ideas pre- viously formed and emotions previously felt can be recalled ; "most of them have made shipwreck in that great nonentity from which they never more will emerge" (Eibot). It is not, of course, to be understood that the line of de- marcation is sharp and distinct — that is, that knowledges are either forgotten or remembered. There are various grades. Upon analysis the problem resolves itself into one of amount — of amount recalled. It is obvious that a distinction should be made between total recall and partial recall. Some events are recalled in detail with a complete "setting," so to speak; other events, on the contrary, are recalled in a vague, indefinite and in- complete manner, and merely the "skeleton," as it were, is seen. Theoretically, total recall, in the sense of perfect, com- plete and absolute recall, never occurs, nor would it be often desirable. To have to recall all of the details without the power of omitting the unessential would be most undesirable. Generally speaking, the well-ordered mind is distinguished by its ability to omit the unnecessary and unessential. We witness this with the story-teller. The good story-teller knows what to bring out and what to select in order to produce the effect and emotion that he desires; the poor story-teller, on 164 Memory and the Learning Process the other hand, not having the power of selection developed to such a degree, dwells so long on the unessential points and lays such emphasis on unimportant matters that he misses the drift and arrives nowhere. As a rule, this power of proper selection of ideas and con- trol of the imagination is more or less concomitant with, age and education. The educated man is distinguished by the facility with which he can pass from total to partial recall, and the aptness with which he is able to throw off the unes- sential. In short, he is characterized, by his ability to forget judiciously. As we have already said, perfectly total recall really never occurs. What we mean by it in this discussion is a recall where the various experiences and emotions are revived in all their minutiae. Such recall might be valuable in scientific work and in the witness-box, but nowhere else. * -jf * * * * * In a thorough investigation of economy in learning the question should, of course, be considered as to whether the use of a combination of two senses may not be better than either alone, i. e., may not the employment of two or more senses be more economical, and at the same time give a higher degree of retention. The results obtained by the various ex- perimenters who worked on this problem vary widely. Among the most precise experiments performed on this subject are those of Miinsterberg aiid Bigham.* These authors compared visual memory with that of auditory (or auditory- motor) memory. In the experiments to which we refer Miins- terberg and Bigham used as material small squares of colored paper. Each square was of a different shade of color; they also employed other squares of white paper on each of which was written a number that the subject had previously mem- orized in connection with or standing for a certain definite shade or color. Thus, the number 5, for example, stood for blue ; 7, for yellow ; 6, green, and so forth. In this manner it was possible for a subject to memorize a series of colors (in an auditory manner) by digits as well as by names. Their •iluNSTBRBERG. Studies from the Harvard Psychological Lahoratory. Psych. Rev., Vol. I, p. 34. Educational Value of Psychological Research 165 subjects were but five in number, and their results, therefore, have but a limited value. We cannot here discuss the results of their experiments and their conclusions, which are some- what complex. We shall merely say that they found that : (1) Visual presentation of color gave a higher degree of retention than did mere auditory presentation. .. (2) A series of tests presented at the same time to two different organs of senses give a higher degree of retention than when one is presented to the eye alone or to the ear alone. [In certain cases, however, the employment of two senses is undesirable, the one seeming to act against the other.] Others who have worked on this problem of Economy in Learning are Steffens, Pentschew, Jost, Meumann, Schneider, G. E. Miiller, H. Miiller, P. Ephrussi, Neumann, Witasek, Radosavljevich, Gamble,. Busemann, Ogden, Lakenan, Lar- guier des Bancels, and Pyle and Snyder. The more important of the results obtained by these investigators have been already given (in Chapters III and IV.) It has become so customary of late to associate the science of Education with that of Psychology that it is common for those not working in psychological laboratories to infer that a considerable, if not a major part, of psychological experi- mentation is performed with the sole purpose of furthering educational advancement. The inference is an erroneous one. It is only here and there that the results obtained by the ex- perimental psychologist are of any great value to the teacher or educator. Experimental psychology has made but few changes in the science of education, and we have no reason to assume, as we so often do, that the latter is based and founded upon the former. It is only when psychology applies itself strictly to educational problems that results of any moment to the teacher may be expected, and here again the "results" were quite probably already known to the educator in a more or less vague way. In short, a large percentage of the results obtained by the experimental psychologist are all very well as facts — scientific 166 Memory and the Learning Process facts— but they have little to do with every-day life ; at any rate, to the average man they are unemployable. We should here say, however, that experimental psychology and its appli- cations are distinct, and that it is not necessarily the duty of this science to confine itself to matters of practical import. The true investigator centers his attention solely on the search for truth, irregardless as to whether the results he obtains may or may not be of practical application. This may seem somewhat by way of defense — as though we were trying to defend the experimental psychologist and justify his existence. He does not need defense. Apart from what we have said, we cannot but suspect that he invariably hopes that the results he obtains may be of practical value. The zoologist who spends months and years in the study of the chromosomes of the fruit fly DrosopJiila ampelophila — crossing red-eyed females here with white-eyed males there, or individuals of the "vestigial" winged variety here with individuals of the "long-winged" variety there — doubtless hopes that some day he will be able to tell us just what determines sex in man^- possibly even, how to control the sex of his offspring. To the breeder such a discovery would certainly be of great economic value. In short, with all such work the practical man of affairs may fail to see the connection, but it is nevertheless there. And so with a considerable part of the experimental work that has been done on memory; — no true educator can behold the results without perceiving that here are exact knowledges— knowledges which, though they may not be di- rectly applicable, must none the less have a bearing of some sort on the learning process and, therefore, on education in GENERAL. It is when the psychologist applies himself to the study of memory that the teacher awaits most eagerly for the results, but even here his conclusions are generally disappointing. "Do not endeavor to improve the memory"; "Do not break the material to be learned into parts, but learn it as a whole" ; ■ "Avoid mnemonic devices"; "Trust the memory wherever possible" — these and possibly a half-dozen other rules tell us all that the teacher will receive from the psychologist on memory. The great trouble is that even here the psychologists Educational Value of Psychological Research 167 differ even among themselves. Some tell us that the memory (memory in general) is capable of improvement; others tell us that it is incapable of improvement, and that as it is given us at birth, so it remains. All this is in no way meant to disparage the science of ex- perimental psychology. We desire merely to limit the idea that experimental psychology— even when applied to mem- ory—must necessarily jdeld results that are of great practical importance, or that are so exact and well defined that no dif ference of opinion may exist. Conclusions in science— even when the data on which they are based are of the best— are but relative and subject to change at short notice. The con- version or alteration of scientific "facts" is more apt to mean that the original conclusions were too hastily drawn than that the data were wrong. Thus in the field of memory e. g., it may be that the "piece-meal" method of learning is, for gen- eral purposes, the most economical, nothwithstanding that the results obtained by Steffens, Ephrussi, and others, would seem to point to the contrary. As we have already said, econ- omy in learning depends upon the object or result that is di- rectly desired. In short, we must define what we mean by "economy." Thus it is that no determination of the economy of a method can ever possess universal validity. We can only say what method will give the best results — ^will be the most economical — for the object the learner has in view. Such re- sults as those obtained by Steffens, Pentschew, Ephrussi, Pyle, and Jost are relative in the extreme, though, with the exception of the two latter, these- authors have failed to appreciate the fact. It is the habit of each science to extend the basis of its knowledge by experiment, and psychology is far from being an exception. But psychology is not an exact science, and thus it is continually tempted to make generalizations from im- perfect observations or too small a number of observations, and it is constantly the work of the experimental psychologist to check up the inductions and theories of his "arm chair" colleagues. The desire to form theories is at all times laud- able, but with so inexact a science as psychology they should be formed with great care — and then only after careful and 168 Memory and the Learning Process repeated experimentation. If for no other reason than the great diversity of opinion held among psychologists regard- ing the laws of the mind, it behooves us to demand an increase in both the exactness and number of the data before forming generalizations. Psychology is not able to express its laws with the mathematical exactness that characterizes chemistry, and physics — and, notwithstanding that mathematical for- mulas are being more and more applied, to certain operations of the mind, psychology remains in large part a science of probabilities based on experimental data the validity of which is frequently doubtful. Psychology is, however, trying, and becomes every day a more exact science. For this she may thank both Education and Psychology for the demands put upon her; these sister sciences have formed an inseparable trio, each asking ques- tions of the others. We are no longer satisfied with rule-of- thumb methods, and now demand the how, why, and wherefore of each and every operation. The major part of the literature on the mental sciences still consists of loose, indefinite, or unwarranted generalizations that have never been subjected to careful experimental investi- gation. This is especially true of the old psychology, and so it is that experiment is slowly changing the general aspect of this science. This, then, is the chief task of the experimental psy- chologist — the accumulation of data in so great a number and on so many subjects that generalizations may be safely de- duced. Modern psychologists have more or less abandoned the exclusively introspective methods as employed by their arm-chair brethren, and have turned to experiment. Deduc- tion has given way to induction. We shall not say they are going too far, but as yet it must be admitted that, although a few erroneous conceptions concerning the mind have been corrected. Psychology as a science has not been revolutionized. Unfortunately, a considerable portion of the results that have been obtained are as yet either not amenable of application or are of doubtful validity. Parmelee is undoubtedly correct in saying that the prime need in psychology to-day is the de- velopment of its genetic aspect. Here, by tracing the evolution of psychic phenomena from the lowest up to the highest Educational Value of Psychological Research 169 species,^ and by keeping in mind that psychic phenomena can be profitably studied only from a viewpoint of objective be- havior, it may be that a considerable amount of useless experi- mentation on human beings will be done away with. Experimental Psychology has performed its best services in exploding erroneous ideas regarding the mind— many of them so old that they were uprooted with difficulty. For ex- ample, it has long been held that improvement in one opera- tion of the mind may materially improve others. Thorndike and Woodworth, however, in their paper entitled "The In- fluence of Improvement of One Meiital Function Upon the Efficiency of Other Functions, ' ' have conclusively proven that such is almost never the case. Again, for example, it has long been held that the brain is liable to fatigue, in much the same way as one of the muscles. "Common observation," says Woodworth," "seemed to show that fatigue comes on very quickly in mental work, and this apparent fact has done duty in many psychological explanations." Experiments; however, have shown that this familiar form of fatigue is/ largely a sensory or emotional affair. "It is a feeling of fatigue, not a true fatigue in the sense of incapacity."" In the field of memory experimental psychology has done great service in exposing numerous "systems" for the improve- ment of memory as well as the mnemonic devices supposed to facilitate retention and reproduction. Now psychologists are supposed to hold definite ideas regarding such themes as these, though it would seem that the public pay but little at- tention to them. The reason for this lies partly in the fact that in matters relating to the mind there is so much charla- tinism the psychologist hesitates to popularize his subject. Scientific psychology may definitely prove some prevalent idea to have no foundation, but he may not send the exposure broadcast. In the meantime, the' professional memory teacher acquires fame and support from a public that ought to know better. We have already said that now and then the results ob- '-Psychiatrti and Experimental Psychology, Proc. of the Am. Medico-Psy. Ass., June, 1906. "Op. cit. 170 Memory, and the Learning Process tained from psychological experimentation may be of value to the teacher, and that when it came to memory, nearly every investigation contributed some gain, small though it may be. What, now, are the contributions that such experi- ments as those described in detail in Chapter IV may make to the science of education! For answer we can only refer the questioner to the summary of results given at the end of the chapter in question. It may, for example, be of value to the teacher to know that a rapid learner is at no disadvantage so far as his retentiveness is concerned, but, generally speak-^ ing, such facts are of but limited value to the ordinary teacher. Space does not permit a consideration of each of the con- clusions given in the preceding chapter, but it will be noted that scarcely half of them have any direct bearing on, or sig- nificance to, educational problems in general. A positive cor- relation, for example, was found between quiclc learning and amount retained, but it will be noted that it is a very low one. This, as a scientific fact, however, is of value. It may also be valuable for the teacher to know that the popular impression that slow learners retain a relatively greater amount of what they have learned than do quick learners is, as a rule, erroneous. To those who are connected with schoolg for feeble-minded and backward children it may be of value to know that here the correlation is slightly higher, especially in the case of prose and poetry. It would seem that with a class* of de- fectives those who learn exceedingly slowly are relatively poor retainers. It should be borne in mind, however, that the memory tests described are not satisfactory tests of rea- soning, notwithstanding the fact that they are tests of a cer- tain kind of mental ability. . The results of the experiments would seem to show that in general the power to remember meaningful material increases both with age and intellectual standing. This, however, is but what we would expect. When we examine the various tables and notice the quan- tity or percentage of the material that has evidently been for- gotten after the expiration of the time interval in question we *i. e., a group or class in school, for example. Educational Value of Psychological Research 111 may associate this with an observation that doubtless all of us have at times made with reference to our early school work, viz., that a very considerable fraction of what we memorized in our early years has seemingly been totally forgotten. I say "seemingly" because we are apt to forget that what has here been forgotten has — as is proven by our ' ' Method 2 ' ' — neverthelss left associations and impressions of value, even though these may not be readily recalled. We should remem- ber that the mental work put on any problem, be it on a set of nonsense syllables, has nevertheless necessitated a certain amount of mental activity which may be of value in some way or other in the future. This is undoubtedly more true of the slow learner who (though* according to our tabulated results, may stand lower as to amount retained) may acquire a "some- thing" that the quick learner does not. The teacher should impress her pupils of the fact that what is learned and memorized in school is not meant to be useful for the school days alone, but for the pupil's entire life. The final value of whatever we learn is conditioned very largely by our ability to recall it later at pleas.ure. If we are unable so to do, then what we have learned is not really a "posses- sion" in the sense of the word as we used it in Chapter II. Only time can show what real value anything once learned may have. APPENDICES. Nonsense Digits. Syllables. Words. 5 VUS TUB YIF PIN 9 MAV HEN 4 JEP BED 7 VOB LID 1 FBG GEM 5 WOP BUD 2 TIB CAR G NUZ MAT 1 BOF ROD 8 JED JUG KIB FOG 4 VEL LAD 7 ZID SOD 3 BOL PEN 8 SEP CAT 1 YAB RAG 6 KUV BOX 2 TEP NET 9 NAD GUN APPENDIX A. Some of the Matebials Used. Poetry (100 M'ords). And gentle Ellen welcomed her With courteous looks and mild : Thought she "what if her heart should melt, And all be reconciled!" The day was scarcely like a day— The clouds were black outright : And many a night, with half a moon, I've seen the church more light. The wind was .wild ; against the glass The rain did beat and bicker : The church tower swinging overhead, You scarce could hear the Vicar : And then and there the mother knelt, ^\nd audibly she cried — Oh ! may a clinging curse consume This woman by my side. Prose (100 Words). (From Kipling's "Kim.") The diamond-bright dawn woke men and cows and bullocks together. Kim sat up and yawned, shook himself, and thrilled ^ith delight. This was seeing the world in real truth, this was life as he would have it — bustling and shouting, the buckling of belts, and beating of bullocks and creaking of wheels, lighting of fires and cooking of food, and new sights at every turn of the ap- proving eye. The morning mist swept off in a whirl of silver ; the parrots shot away to some distant river in shrieking green hosts : all the well wheels within earshot were at work. Poetry (100 Words). To see a man tread over graves I hold it no good mark ; 'Tis wicked in the sun and moon. And bad luck in the dark. You see this grave? The Lord he gives. The Lord he takes away : O Sir ! the child of my old age Lies there as cold as clay. Except that grave, you scarce see one That was not dug by me ! I'd rather dance upon 'em Than tread upon these three! Ay, Sexton ! 'tis a touching tale ! You, Sir, are but a lad ; This month I'm in my seventieth year, And still it makes me sad. 172 Nonsense Digits. Syllables. Words. 6 LEB BIRD 1 DUT RUG .5 NIV EAR 8 POZ SLATE 2 DIB CAP 7 PEG DOOR 5 ZAD BOX 3 TOB TREE 9 KBD CORN 4 BUP AXE 7 KIF SAIL 2 RUZ HINGE 5 BUG 9 SPOOL 3 DOG BOOK 8 POST 1 GUN 6 BEAN o LAMP Appendices 173 Prose (100 Words). (From Preface of Baeckel's "Riddle of the Universe.") The present study of the monistic philosophy is intended for thoughful readers of every condition who are united in an honest' search for the truth. An intensification of this effort of man to attain a knowledge of the truth is one of the most salient features of the Nineteenth Century. This is easily explained, in the first place, by the immense progress of science, especially in its most important branch, the history of humanity ; it is due in the second place to the open contradiction that has developed during the century between science and the traditional "Revelation." Prose (160 Words). (From Kant's "Critique.") Time is nothing but the form of the internal sense, that is, of our intuition of ourselves, and of our internal states. Time cannot be a determination peculiar to external phenomena. It refers neither to their shape, nor to their position, etc., it only determines the relation of representations in our internal state. And exactly because this internal intuition supplies no shape, we try to make good this deficiency by means of analogies, and represent to ourselves the succession of time by a straight line progressing to infinity, in which the manifold constitutes a series of one dimension onl.y ; and we conclude from the properties of this line as to all the properties of time, with one exception, i. e., that the parts of the former are simultaneous, those of the latter succes- sive. From this it becomes clear, also, that the representation of time Is itself an intuition, because all its relations can be expressed by means of an external intuition. Prose (100 Words). (From Franklin's "Autohiography.") But I soon found that I had undertaken a task of more difficulty than I had imagined. While my care was employed in guarding against one fault, I was often surprised by another ; habit took the advantage of inattention ; inclina- tion was sometimes too strong for reason. I concluded, at length, that the mere speculative conviction that it was our interest to be completely virtuous was not sufficient to prevent our slipping, and that the contrary habits must be broken, and good ones acquired and established, before we can have any de- pendence on a steady, uniform rectitude of conduct. Two Lists of Disconnected Sentences of 100 Words Each. 1. From the Kingdom of Heaven those angels have come. 2. This horse ate nothing but oats. 3. The wise man seizes every opportunity. 4. Ten years had elapsed and they therefore crowned him. 5. From my own experience I know he will never achieve success in that line. 6. The farce entitled "The Telltale Coo" was written by Haljass, an author who lived in Venebaft. 7. The wise man is one who realizes the value of industry. S. The laboratories here are so arranged that the r6om for psychological work receives the most sunlight. 9. The work executed in prisons and reformatories is quite frequently very well performed. 1. Opportunity comes both to the wise man and the fool. •_'. The two walls met and the waters were dammed back. 3. We can say from Christ's teachings that God is Love. 4. I had dreamt that either rain or snow would fall. 5. Sin begets sorrow, as any knave can tell from experience. 6. The fairy thought that either. son or daughter would suffice. 7. The school system is but one of our many failures. 8. A short time ago that nation was rich and prosperous. 9. From his writings we considered him a man who smoked. 10. "Waste not, want not," and "Grain in the morning sow." As may be noted, the sentences are not only peculiar, but many of them are awk- ward. Many of them come fromold Hindu fables, the awkward translation being desir- able for the experiment in question. 174 Memory and the Learning Process APPENDIX B. Typical Reproductions of Three Subjects Showing Method of Scoring for Digits, Nonsense Syllables, and Words. (Tlie Reproductions of the Prose and Poetry of the same three subjects are given on pages 115 to 117.) Subject — AwiiTA F. List studied 5 9 4 7 1 Tt 2 6 1 8 4 7 3 8 1 G (Senior, Albany Normal College). Digits. Reproduction (with Score attached) 5 9 4 Total Score for Digits: 28 Nmisense SyllaMes. Reproduction (with Score attached) VUS YIF MAV JEB VOS WEF FEG TIB NUZ LOD* GBR* KUL YAB List studied VUS YIF MAV .TEP VOB FEG WOF TIB NUZ BOF JED KIB VEL ZID BOL SEF WAB KUV TEF NAD *LOD and GER both receive a score of 2, notwithstanding that they have only one letter correct, 1. e., the vowel. As said on page 93, even though a syllable has two letters correct, but is not in the correct position (either relative or absolute), it receives no score at all. When, however, the absolute position is correct (as it is in the above case), each letter that is correct is scored. Therefore each of the above two syllables receives a score of 2, — 1 because it is in the correct absolute position and 1 for having a correct letter. Total Score for Nonsense Syllables : 39 Appendices Words. List Reproduction studied (witli Score a ttaclied) TUB TUB PIN PEN . , , HEN CAT . . . BED MAN LID PEN . . GEM RUG BUD 5IAT CAE RAG , . MAT WIT ROD RAT JUG BOY FOG RUN LAD SOD PEN CAT Total Score KAG for Words 15 BOX NET QUN 175 Subject — Jennie Mc. (Senior, Albany Normal College). DioHs. List studied 5 4 7 1 Reproduction (with Score attaciied) Total Score for Digits: 26 176 Memory and the Learning Process Konseiise SpUaTilcs. List studied VUS TIF MAV JBP VOB FEG WOF TIB NUZ BOF JED KIB VEL ZID BOL SEF WAB KUV TEF NAD Reproduction (witli Score attached) VUS . • . • VIF • • • JEP • ■ • RIL BOV • • SIR WOL • • • TID . • • Total Score for Nonsense Syllables : 18 Words. List studied TUB PIN HEN BED LID GEM BUD CAR MAT ROD JUG FOG LAD SOD PEN CAT RAG BOX NET GUN Reproduction (witli Score attaclied) TUB . • • PILL • • RAG • • CAN BAR* DOG SUN FLY SIAT • • BAG BOX • ■ . TOP LID • . MAN Total Score for Words: IC •BAR receives a score of two, altliougli it Is neltlier preceded by tlie correct word nor is it the correct word Itself. The correct word here is CAR, and BAR receives a score of 2 as follows : 1 for having two letters correct and 1 because it is in the correct absolute position. List studied 5 9 4 ITeiJi-oduction (with Score attaclied) ■i 8 • • 1 • . • • 2 . . 9 • . Total Score for Digits : 37 Nonsense Syllahles. Reproduction (with Score attaclied)' vus .... YIP .... TIB .... BIF* JEB . . • NAB Total Score for Nonsense Syllables : 17 List studied VUS YIF MAV JEP VOB FEG WOF TIB NUZ BOF JED KIB VEL ZID BOL SEF YAB KUV TEF NAD *BIF can receive no score, it being in neitlier tlie correct relative position nor in the correct absolute position. It has, however, two letters that occur in BOP and for which it may have been mistal^en. JEB (JED) therefore receives three counts, it being in the correct relative position with reference to the preceding syllable HIP (BOP). It will thus be seen that in cases like this what one syllable loses it gives to the other. Words. Reproduction (with Score attached) TUB • . • HEN • • JUG . . RAT TAN MUG CAT RUG PEN BED GUN List studied TUB PIN HEN BED LID GEM BUD CAR MAT ROD JUG FOG LAD SOD PEN CAT RAG BOX NET Total Score for Words: 19 178 Memory and the Learning Process APPENDIX C. The tables given below are practically the same as Tables Nos. 1 and 2 as given in the body of the work (pages 119 and 120), with the exception that the numbers under "Method 3" denote percentage of time lost (i. e., amount for- gotten) instead of percentage of time saved (i. e., amount retained). Due to the addition of extra matter, the column numbers do not correspond. o H Q OOi:OOOI^TJ'*-«*rHrHT* a K CO ti o g. "i a — ' ^/— ir V 1 d _ jr corf Tj* oi t- 050 1- "^ «£> oos CO o as t^co 00O5 ojcd CO ooco P «« g CO CO CO CO rH CO N eg (N Oa (M eg CO t-H (M (M iH r-< C < '^ H ggag, -I ^ '° «? ^ jj » M O.S<)M OS Tj; o od •^ S SBi^o CO CO CO eg Z o B fM^ofe Q H S g'S «S O U5 C0^«> ^ OCO OO O CX300CDCO COOO^ COiO x>o o " o a " >0 OO t- CO "-1 to CO O CO US O 00 00 O to pH CO O "-H CO C- rH lOlO .SH,^ r-irHCgcO^ COC0C0^COCDir3I>-^COOSt-^lOC0t-00COt- ^ W' - f-H T-t 1— t ,-H ,-H ^0) ■ 0«0>OU30COCOOOOOOCOtO«50 tooooooo oo ^^5 t- rH IXM in CO CO O 00 us O -^ CO »-l CO US CO 00 00 113 -^ O O "3 .:^ w a . '^(MCOOqN(N'-HC^i-tCOlM00000'-ii-HOO 0} a> rH ?-H 1— I rH W U3 W N Oi CO 00 »H lO CO O CD Oi "^ OQ O t- I-* 00 CO W i-f Tj< '^ rt 1— H-H 1-t (M rH T-H C4 1-1 (M i-H W 1— I C<\ 1-i i-H M a> a o S u 03 ^ & o 9 H oi O «> s t- eg (M in iHO t- 00 00 OOO 05 tH I> 00 rH t- O^ C= Q CO CO S eg CO r^ iH eg lo CO r-i eg CO eg ,-1 Tf -* ^ CO CO C0 1-1 CO hi O rrt a 3 oji^s om o in o o o co o co m 10 o 00 co coooo o coo co co co 00 "■' t5 iggajS^QOt-oSoSoSt-cgooorHOoooincDOoocoin co ~ oR ^ 06 00 1- 00 1- -^ ^H in 1— ' ^ o CO eg o m t- 05 1— ' o in 05 OS Oi OS ?— 1 l^'^M """^ SSegcgeqeartcgcgcgcgegcgrnegegegeqegcoeg eg _^ *jaj- (— iinoOOCOOCOOOinOCDOOO^OOCDOCDOO Tji S „ '^S oe5oSoiiHSccooegSi-i'*oo^«Doini-;ocDinTj< co_ 2 o)i^ rjini>Lnod'*^'egt>'inoin05coo6oinooinoooooo^(-3 53 „bS egco'-*'-*uitocoododosrH;-Heg£23;2incDC-oeg2a2|in jh <" (u ^ rH .-( eg eg eg eg eg eg eg CO CO CO CO CO CO CO CO CO ^ -^ Til ■* ■^ eo I i^^^SHW'w^^wmO^HSMSH^dW^gJH s • _■ -a > « SHHfe'/ /3(9 //7 /^S /86 Vm >. / ^ ( '//o X / . ^/f X / ' (Ym^ x3 ' (7/ik^ X S - ('/S K7 ^ if^ X ^ = (■3//0 AC/ = ( ^^ X /-f = (^S\//-- (7^2^ A-a = (7^ X //^ = ( ff yZd = ( 7^ ^^^ ' (77^ y zs ' ( / y Z-^ ■ ( /p. y Z77 . ( r/z X M ( Z y. Zf ' ( Zi X Z6 = ( 3 y IS' - i 3/x X 2^ { -4^ y 3^ - i^'/z y Z^ - { S y 33 = ( t X 3/ .'A = 7f^ ^ /z^ - /' - z^' ^-^^) 3, 3-1 9} /^) - zz.) - z^> j4 36) Jh) S--4-,l 9/] /36\) ///.' //oS) = /c96) I <5s I 27 ' 34.1 12 50 18 421 14 46 32.0 9 56 4 77 13 49 24 14 66.31 !-59.4 52.6 45.6 l 47.3 46.4 106 5S METHOD TWO. Rank. 2 5 1 23 8 13 10 16 4 15 7 12 18 22 11 9 24 14 3 19 21 6 20 17 Score. Per cent. 981 89 99 50 85 73 78 68 91 72 87 75 64 52 76 SO 37 73 95 60 54 89 55 67 82.3 I .'8.5 J 80.4 63.' 70.0 66.8 1767 73.6 147 73.1 SECOND LE.VRNING. Time of First Learning. Time. Min. (1 10.00 10.40 13.00 13.00 13.75 14.33 15.00 15.25 16.08 17.00 17.00 17.25 18.66 19.50 22.25 23.25 24.25 25.16 26.08 28.00 29.80 31.40 34.56 45.16 500.13 20.8.'f 6.79 —10.84 —10.44 — 7.84 — 7.84 — 7.09 — 6.51 — 5.84 — 5.59 — 4.76 — 3.84 — 3.84 — 3.59 — 2.18 — 1.34 1.41 2.41 3.41 4.32 5.24 7.16 8.96 10.56 13.72 24.32 163.05 6.79 117..50 108.99 61.46 61.46 50.26 42.38 .34.10 31.24 22.65 14.74 14.74 12.88 4.75 1.79 1.98 5.80 11.62 18.66 27.45 51.26 80.28 111.51 188.23 591.46 1667.19 69./,G 8.34 Time of Second Learning. Rank. Time. 2 1.33 3 2.16 1 l.OO 7 4.40 4.00 4.25 4.6(i 6.00 4.50 11.25 5.40 8.50 6.50 10.00 9.00 9.50 5.56 7.40 4.00 15.W 17.00 6.56 6.88 11.75 4 6 9 12 8 21 10 17 13 20 18 19 11 16 5 23 24 14 15 166.60 H.9.i 3.08 —5.61 -^.80 —5.94 — 2.,54 —2.94 —2.69 —2.28 — .94 —2.44 4.31 —1.54 1.56 — .44 3.06 2.06 2.56 —1.38 .46 —2.94 8.06 10.06 — .38 .06 4.81 73.86 3.0S 31.47 23.04 35.28 5.24 8.64 7.1'5 5.20 .88 5.95 18.57 2.37 2.43 .19 9.36 4.24 6.55 1.90 .21 8.64 64.96 101.20 .14 .00 23.14 366.77 15.28 3.91 23 24 25 26 2X 28 29 30 31 -COMPUTATION OF INDICES OF COBREL.\TION (PEARSON METHOD). METHOD THREE), METHOD ONE. 60.81 .10.11 46.57 10.91 20.84 17.51 13.31 5.25 11.61 — 16.55 5.92 — 5.60 .95 — 4.10 2.90 6.17 — 4.70 1.99 — 1.5.40 57.71 90.13 — 4.01 — .82 116.98 +477.49 I'ercentage of Time Lost. Rank. 2 4 1 15 10 12 13 17 9 24 14 20 16 21 18 19 7 11 3 22 23 6 Per Cent. 13 20 08 34 29 30 31 39 28 66 32 49 35 51 4i) 40 23 29 15 53 57 21 20 26 -.21 .14 .26 .00 .05 .04 .113 .< )5 .116 .32 .02 .15 .01 .17 .ik; .06 .11 .05 .19 .19 .2.3 .13 .14 .08 '.89 .0169 .0196 .0676 .0000 .0025 .0016 .0009 . 98 24.4 595.36 44 — 9 SI 93.96 .- SO 15.4 237.16 !^3 4^) 1600 — 313.60 1 99 25.4 645.16 33 —20 400 156.80 23 50 —23.6 556.96 69 16 256 — 113.44 8 85 11.4 129.96 62 !l 81 — 5S..59 13 73 (; ..36 52 — 1 1 5.84 !(• 78 4.4 19.36 4.. — 8 64 4-1.72 16 68 — 5.(; 31.3(i 39 - -14 196 66.(J4 4 91 17.4 302.76 4!) — 8 64 30.72 15 72 — I.e. 2.56 84 31 961 — 119.04 87 1.3.4 179.56 51 o 4 7. IS 12 75 1.4 1.96 32 --21 441 45.78 IS 64 — 9.6 92.16 20 —33 1089 44.22 • >•► 52 —21.6 460.56 69 1(! 256 22.56 11 76 .6 76 23 529 55.43 <» SO 6.4 39.96 27 —26 (576 — 88.66 24 —36.6 1339.56 50 9 — 12.96 14 42 — 11 121 — 57.64 95 21.4 457.96 46 — 7 49 — .50.12 19 tJO —13.6 184.96 56 o !t 26.88 21 54 —19.6 384.16 77 24 576 253.44 6 89 15.4 227.1(! 49 — 4 16 — 54.88 20 55 —18.6 345.96 14 —39 412 1521 — 948.4.8 17 67 1767 — 6.6 315.2 43.56 !72 10056 —1440.20 6284.52 i>S n j,iy T3M ,?.).i ici.sr, 17 24.1 — 264.49 — I(i0.77 — 199.13 185.02 — 80.82 3.!X» — 25.69 :',1.30 — S2.S2 6.14 — 51.45 — 5.02 20.03 28.94 .84 15.42 — 124.80 — 2..59 112.13 — 97.37 — 175.61 1(52.62 — 255.19 — 160.51 —1119.02 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 8 4 9 1 16 12 13 3 15 10 14 11 17 19 6 20 18 22 21 23 24 15 9 2l» 1 11 10 4 8 6 16 12 13 17 19 5 18 14 21 22 24 11 7 10 •'> 19 9 8 16 23 (5 17 4 15 13 12 5 1 21 14 18 20 22 24 5 15 1 10 o 16 7 19 21 4 2 12 13 18 6 11 8 17 14 9 23 22 20 24 10 1 19 4 12 16 8 O 20 22 11 9 7 23 17 13 15 24 14 21 IS 2.8 10.6 5.2 9.8 2.4 14.2 7.4 10.2 14.0 10.2 4.8 13.6 13.6 14.0 11.2 12.6 14.4 11.6 17.0 14.4 21.4 20.0 22.4 22.0 J.:2.< 4.3 1.0 3.6 2.6 4.2 1.3 4.6 1.7 4.2 4.4 5.1 3.0 3.9 4.3 2.0 3.0 3.1 6.3 6.9 2.8 2.5 1.7 2.4 1.3 1.8 3.2 8 1 2.3 17 7 10 2 11 24 20 22 12 21 16 5 18 4 (i 19 9 14 13 G 15 20 19 7 13 9 14 10 o.> 24 16 17 12 5 11 6 21 4 3 1 18 8 15 4 24 18 8 1 6 •> 3 21 7 9 10 5 13 14 12 19 20 17 11 16 8 2 9 22 14 7 12 6 4 17 24 21 15 23 IS 16 19 5 10 20 1 O 13 11 4 1 15 10 12 13 17 9 24 14 20 16 21 18 19 7 11 18 6 8 4 20 24 22 14 9 21 17 15 7 12 16 13 11 10 19 20.49 16.18 10.0 10.6 5.4 18.2 14.(> 11.2 11.0 7.6 12.2 18^2 15.8 18.6 ll.S 15.8 14.8 12.2 13.8 8.8 9.8 16.2 12.0 7.6 11.5 12.4 12.5 2.7 5.2 5.3 5.4 5.7 2.7 .5.1 2.4 4.7 6.9 6.9 5.8 2.9 2.7 7.0 3.4 5.7 5.4 4.1 7.2 4.9 8.0 5.1 5.3 4.0 oM 13 1 22 6 11 19 8 9 16 4 14 20 10 7 5 21 18 — O 15 12 3 17 24 19 14 20 4 17 11 22 9 18 3 6 10 8 16 15 23 1 21 7 12 5 13 24 'A 1 11 6 o."> 8 19 13 2 10 O o 4 17 18 14 9 12 21 16 20 23 7 5 15 24 11 1 16 O 4 12 15 17 21 20 10 8 6 9 7 19 14 24 13 IS 20 6 8 10 15 19 16 O 11 21 23 7 ■5 22 12 IS 14 9 4 13 24 9 1 23 15 13 18 8 6 19 O 16 10 o 17 21 24 12 11 4 14 20 13.4 48 20.2 7.2 12.4 12.S 12.4 12.2 15.4 6.6 12.0 15.8 11.2 10.2 8.2 20.4 12.S 21.4 13.S 12.4 40 14.6 22.8 12.5 3.8 3.9 41 1.5 3.4 4.9 2.1 5.9 4.6 49 5.3 3.6 4.8 5.4 1.9 42 1.9 5.0 2.0 .3.7 42 1.0 1.5 14 1 {\ 23 7 10 V, 8 9 14 4 13 19 12 15 5 24 17 21 11 IS 3 20 P S IS 9 19 20 16 21 10 13 15 12 14 23 1 11 24 A 10 8 18 15 1 20 4 6 17 12 13 9 16 19 7 22 24 21 3 11 14 3 13 1 15 4 14 10 8 9 18 21 6 11 20 23 12 16 5 19 24 8 13 10 16 4 15 7 12 IS 2'^ 11 9 24 14 3 19 21 6 20 17 13 12 21 10 6 15 4 17 24 9 14 20 18 23 8 11 5 19 16 3.2 10.6 42 17.0 7.0 144 13.4 12.8 13.6 13.8 7.4 13.4 15.6 14.8 1.3.8 7.4 19.4 13.0 17.6 15.5 15.2 4.0 16.0 19.0 12.5 3.7 5JD.* 1.9 3.9 3.4 6.8 2.8 4.4 .5.9 5.8 6.9 3.9 2.1 2.8 43 4.1 3.0 41 3.5 5.2 6.1 7.1 49 1.6 4i) 4.0 i.S -•^^v ^'ttg 4^. 1909. — May Z igiz 73 59 40 38 Z3 /s ij\ I I <3 PLATE VIII. PROSE. "Once-per-day" method. The "curve" shows the numher of readings necessary lo memorize iinssages of prose ranging from 1.5 to 15,000 words. "lug 4-. I90g, — May Z . fgfz /s ZO zs JO JS SO bo 70 80 /CO /zs /so /p zoo zsv 3co ■^6 SW ^00 SVo /OOO /too /■soo 3 coo -9-000 SOOO ycoo /oooo /svao / J s 7 hS = ihlS) , ZOSS : (36 - S9 ^Zobs) 4Z00 - ( OO X 5^ - -4-ZOo) .. 54-7S .. [7^ ^ 73 = S-nS) 1000 3000 4000 SVOO 7000 JOOOO /5000 Words 63 «5 PLATE Vila. PROSE. The shadetl portion in the lower left corner is a miniature of Table VII and contains "curves" both for the contiiuioiix and the oiicc-pcr-ilaij methods. The remainder of the plate shows the "once-per-day" method (in black) continued up to 15,000 words. ^-ug / if)og Aiaij z f^fi 6i-7S 4-zoo zo65 /6zs /500 Words took S 00 Mmutcs ( f/z % 4o = 3oo) lOOO . „ 550 , (/O y 55 ^ JSo) 3000 ., , 780 : ( /J y 5Z ^ JSO) c-^Mj / /gog — c-^tc'/' • • Ji ZfC , ., /5^ D/^/ts 'Once fi^y c da< / " /?UtAcd F (tcgod tbric /fe /Z ■ ; 'A- /(■ . - /^ Z-1- „ ., ■f 3Z , ,. 7 ^f .. 9 "I ■. . Zl ^04 ■■ - 35 ZOO „ . f'S -fOO ^ „ ZJJ Trutv^ ri-ATE V. DIGITS. The black curve shows the total time spent by the "nnce-per-ilay" method, al the rale of two digils rer second, for sets of diKits ranging from S t. liHi. The "continuous" method is shown in red. 70 I iJjo 400 PLATE VI. DIGITS. "Once-per-day" method. The curve shows the number of readings necessary to memorize sets of digits ranging from 8 to 400. Tn^y 4-. 1908 - June Z8 /pop. \ Zj Z -^ 5 <6 T i? PLATE IV. POETRY. "Once-per-day" method. The curve shows the number of read- ings necessary to memorize passages of poetry ranging from 1 to 100 stanzas in lengtli. The stanzas average 25 words each, excepting those shown in red, which average 35 words each. y^^y 4^. /908 - June Z8, /goq / St dS '/ '/ Zf „ Z4 „ ^ „ -/ zz „ ZZ ., SO _,^ ^ zs ,, „ Z^ rf ^0 „ zz O .. ZZ '. 7S *f ■f 3o " ., 30 " wo r* ri J.3 ,, ,. 32> // -^O 50 60 'sQO FRY. "Once-per-day" method. The curve shows the number of read- ' to memorize passages of poetry ranging from 1 to 100 stanzas in itanzas average 25 words each, excepting those sliown in red, which rds each. //(,iy J^T/gos Jccly /0-'\/go<;. 105 f3S 93 43 37 25 lb ConTtnuotM Tflcthad S n.s &a<( /f moft . /z .. ■ 6 niin s /6> ., , 9 Z4 .. . /b 3Z ■■ , ^s f^ -, ,. ^3 72 •■ .. /3S Once per day "m.£ thc^ IS took '/.f 7ncn, /z 3Z 7z /CH wo 300 14- is 95 /9S 300 8 \1 16 \ 'if/iihcr S\fIaJ:'/es PLATE I. NONSENSE SYLLABLES. The black "curve" shows the total time spent by the "once-pei-day" melhod, at the rate of two syllables per second, for sets of nonsense syllables ranging from 8 to 300. The "continuous" method is shown in red. 78 56 43 35 2=^. 2S 23 15 /flay -f 1^08 — July /o. 1909 /z /6 3Z 48 yz roi- zoo 300 Zc/ays , L e. Z rte£c^i-'n May /, 1908 —June Z8,1909. 3 19 262 220 190 H7 \^6 Con/inaom i^?e//ic^ /J yanzci (ZJ M&rc/s)7do.^- fs /P/n 9 Ax ., (JO „ ) . //i . .3 - (/o3 ■■ ) ■^ ^ . -^ „ (m ^ / ., 5 „ S o (/ZJ „ } - /o „ 6 -. (/JO . ) . 9 . 7 '■ (.Hi" ,. ) ' /6 . 8 ', (loo ■■ ) , /9 - W .. (ZJO - ) - 26 . /Z ■- (300 . ; . Z8 .. /s „ (Jr.^ ,. ) ,. 3/- /s . (450 . ; „ 30 ., z/ . (SZ5 ^^ ; . 44- „ zs ., (H5 ., ; ., 56 ; 30 , (/cm . ) . /zz ., 35 . (875 .. ) ,, //i^ ., ^ „ (woo ■■ ) ., /Z^ .. SO . (/zso .. ) ./// „ ^^o .■ (?/€0 , ) .. /^6 ■■ 7S . (/S7^ ., ) . /90 ., /oo V (Z600 - ; ..3/Z „ 1M ■ — VIZ F= I ■5: O/weype/^Day TJ'i^t/iod ^ 2 s 6 ., 7 8 '/8 3/; 3 6 8 /S /o Zf m'rt(./Z X „ (..Z3 X .. (..^0 V ., (30 X .r (.GZ X . (. 67 X \/Z0 X „ (.9/ X ., ojy X I 7= 3) 5) //= /o> /2- y/snz.a. f 1 1 day 1 1 ^ 1 ^ /bo/C '/s 7?1/rv(..'Z X 7- /r) // # „ (.13 X 3= ^/^ // 3'i „ (.30 X y=A'i) rr 3 ., (30 X 7= 3) // 6 ., (.Gz X ^= 5) „ 8 . (. 67 X /2- ,3-; // /8 / 9-ZO X /5-^ /?) ff /O „ (.9/ X //= /O) " Zf ., (/J7 X /S^ Z7) ', Z3 ., (/.^ X /^= zi>) .// DZ ... {Z-^7 \ Zf = SZ) fi 46 . (Z.09 X ZZ^ 46) ,/ 60 ; (Z.50 X 14=- (>0^ '/ 58 ■' (Z.90 X ZO=5S) f.' /''5 '. (s.w X 23 -//-y) /f 98 „ (fm X Z4^98) // W3 „ (4-67 X ZZ-^ /03) /f /46 ., (S.84 X £^= /46) // zzo <■ {/C.ro X ZZ=ZPjyl // Z6z V (F.77 X JO=-Z&Z) " o8S .. U/6B X 35 = dS6) J Z b4 5 6 100 PLATE III. POETRY. The black "curve" shows the total time spent by the "once-per- day" method at the rate of 7 seconds per stanza,* or 5 minutes per 1000 words. The passages of poetry range from 1 to 100 stanzas in length. The "curve" for the "con- tinuous" method is shown in red. The red figures— both in the tables and on the lower maririn — denote those passages composed of (Mine stanzas of ?,'< words each all other stanzas being 4-Iine and averaging 25 words each. *ExcoptiEg tlio G-liiio stanzas;, wliioh rcqiiiro 10 seconds poi- stanza.