BOUGHT WITH THE INCOME FROM THE SAGE ENDOWMENT FUND THE GIFT OF Sietirg W. Base 1891 ^...^.:^.:^.o..7..3: ' ^/e:;//..()... 6896-1 Cornell University Library QP 431.G81 Physiology of the special senses / 3 1924 024 828 810 The original of this book is in the Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/cletails/cu31924024828810 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES BY M. GREENWOOD, Junr. M.R.C.S., L.R.C.P., F.S.S. STATISTICIAN TO THE LISTEE INSTITUTE OF PREVENTIVE MEDICINE ; DIRECTOE OP THE LONDON HOSPITAL STATISTICAL DEPAHTMENT ; LATE SENIOR DEMONSTRATOR OF PHYSIOLOGY IN THE LONDON HOSPITAL MEDICAL COLLEGE ; AND EXAMINER IN PHYSIOLOGY TO THE UNIVERSITY OF ST. ANDREWS LONDON EDWARD ARNOLD 1910 [All rights reserved] ' PEEFACB I HOPE that this volume may be of service to two classes of readers. Students of Psychology may desire to obtain more information regarding the physiological side of the senses than is usually found in works professedly dealing with psychology. I think such students will find this book of use if they read it in conjunction with Professor Myers' admirable treatise on Experimental Psychology.^ Another class of readers which I have had in mind are those who are either taking up physiology as a branch of liberal education, or with a view of presenting themselves for certain higher professional examinations. In either case a somewhat more detailed knowledge of physiology is required than can be obtained from the general text- books of the subject, while time hardly permits of much use being made of original sources of information. The necessity in such cases arises for books intermediate between the text-book and the original memoir. In most branches of physiology this want is abundantly supplied, but I am not acquainted with any such aids to study in the case of the senses. This book may help to fill the gap. The object I have had in view has compelled me to 1 A Text-hooJc of Experimental Psychology, by Dr. C- S. Myers, 8s. 6d, net (London : Edward Arnold), vi PREFACE restrict the work within definite limits. I have abstained from describing the anatomy and histology of the sense organs, since information on these matters is to be found in any general text-book, and I assume the reader to be acquainted with the rudiments of physiological optics and acoustics. Bibliographical references have been confined for the most part to easily accessible works. If the reader is tempted to follow up the clues given, the ends of this book will have been completely attained. M. GREENWOOD, Junk. LoiTGHTON, January 1910. CONTENTS CHAP. PA8E I. Introduction — The Laws of Muller, Weber, and Techner 1 II. General Physiology of Cutaneous Sensation . 8 III. Pain 18 IV. Protopathic and Epicritic Sensibility . . 24 V. Taste and Smell 35 VI. The Sense of Position and Movement. . 48 VII. The Sense of Position and Movement {Concluded) 58 VIII. Hearing — Historical Sketch . . .64 IX. Physiology of the Bar ... 70 X. The Comparative Physiology of Vision . 86 XI. Retinal Processes, Electrical, Phototropic, and Chemical Responses .... .96 XII. Visual Adaptation — Peripheral Vision, Total Colour-blindness ... . . 101 XIII. Recurrent Vision-theories of Adaptation 112 XIV. Trichromatic Vision . . . . . .124 XV. Dichromatic Vision . . .139 XVI. After-images oe Successive Induction . . 155 XVII. Historical Theories of Vision . . 166 XVIII. The Young- Helmholtz Theory of Colour Vision . 177 XIX. Hering's Theory of Visual Sensations . .189 XX. Simultaneous Contrast ... . 203 XXI. The Physiology of "Space" 215 INDEX 237 vii PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES CHAPTEE I INTRODTJCTION— THE "LAWS" OF MULLER, WEBEB, AND FECHNER That province of Physiology to which has been assigned the investigation of our special sense mechanisms is of wide extent. We have to deal with the representation in con- sciousness of effects produced upon the bodily structures by different physical agents under conditions of the most varied nature. In some cases, the results are manifest; in others, their existence may be inferred with more or less plausibility; in yet others, although the conscious reaction, the sensation, is distinct enough, the physiological change which is associated with it eludes our imperfect means of investigation. From very early times sense physiology and psychology have proved themselves of absorbing interest alike to philo- sophers and men of science, and a history of the subject if adequately treated would make an interesting record of the progress of scientific thought. In this book I can do no more than indicate very imperfectly how the workers of long ago. and of to-day have endeavoured to mould their theories and observations into that organised body of knowledge which is a science. The main problem, then, is this : Given a series of physical processes of assumed constancy, and a number of conscious states also assumed to have a real existence — from the stand- point of science as distinct from metaphysics — we have to trace out the intervening physiological processes. It is scarcely necessary to remark that Sense Physiology has nothing to say regarding a causal link between physio- 2 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES logical changes and sensations. We are merely concerned with a time sequence, and make no attempt to bridge the gulf between the worlds of physiology and psychology. The scope of our inquiry being so wide, it is natural to ask, first, whether there are any general formulae, or laws, describing some relations between external stimuli and con- scious reactions valid for all the sense organs. Three such formulae have been enunciated, the " laws " of Miiller, Weber, and Fechner, and we shall begin our task with an investi- gation of them. The first in point, both of time and importance, is un- doubtedly Miiller's " law," which may be enunciated in the following terms : — 1. Different stimuli acting upon the same sense mecha- nism are followed by the same kind of sensation. 2. The same stimulus acting upon different sense mecha- nisms calls up diff'erent sensations. This formula is often called the " Law of Specific Sense Energy," but the terminology is bad, since the word energy has acquired a meaning differing much from that in vogue when the law was first published. In discussing this law, two phrases are employed which need definition. An "Adequate Stimulus" is that form which usually acts upon the sense organ under considera- tion ; for instance, light is an adequate stimulus to the eye. An "Inadequate Stimulus" is one differing in kind from those generally effective ; thus a blow on the eyeball is an " inadequate '' stimvilus for the organ of vision. If the use of common words in uncommon senses be bad, then this terminology cannot be defended ; be this as it may, in employing it one conforms to the established custom. It is clear that the, anatomically, more complex sense organs are shielded from the possible action of inadequate stimuli; for example, the organ of hearing is not readily exposed to the incidence of light waves, and the situation of the eye guards against its mechanical stimulation. It is not, therefore, easy to test Miiller's " law " in these instances, although many attempts have been made. Nagel remarks, with justice, that the supposed proofs in INTRODUCTION 3 the case of sight are not convincing ; the flash of light seen by an unanaesthetised patient on section of the optic nerve might well be due to mechanical stimulation of the retina, and a similar objection can be urged against other like experiments. The most elegant illustration of the law is afforded by the results of stimulating the central end of the chorda tympani, chemical, mechanical and thermal excitation alike calling up a sensation of taste. The reader will of course notice that this is not a proof, but an illustra- tion ; inadequate stimulation of the supposed end organs of taste themselves has not led to very definite results. Although we have no rigorous proof of the correctness of MtlUer's " law," the formula is of real scientific value. At. we shall see, many early thinkers were hampered in their speculations by an axiom dating back to remote ages which asserts that '' causes " and " effects " are necessarily similar in a narrow sense. Mtiller was, perhaps, the first physio- logist definitely to abandon this ancient and sterile dogma, his " law " defining accurately the modern point of view in discussion of this type.^ The next formula, Weber's " law," carries into the province of experimental psychology, and another definition of terms is required. If corresponding to a stimulus of given intensity A we experience a sensation of " magnitude " B {vide infra), then if the stimulus be increased or diminished very slightly no difference in the sensation will be experienced. When, however, the increase or decrease exceeds a certain limit, the intensity of the sensation changes. Again, if a stimulus be increased in intensity from nothing, only after a certain point has been reached will any sensation be experienced. The absolute magnitude of the stimulus in the second case, and the difference of the magnitudes in the first case, measure the threshold or liminal values, and may be alluded to as threshold or liminal stimuli. For information as to the manner in which such experi- ments are carried out, together with details as to possible sources of error, the reader must consult special works ; space 1 See Helmholtz, Ilandb. d. Pkys, Optik. 2(c. Aujl., p.-. 248, etc. 4 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES only permits of a short note as to the leading principles involved. Most methods of experiment fall under one of the three following groups : — (1) The Method of Smallest Perceptible Differences. This simply consists of passing from one intensity of stimulus, by small increments, to the just noticeable next " higher " sensation. Most of Weber's own work was carried out in this way. (2) The Method of Right and Wrong Answers. Two stimuli, A and B, are chosen of nearly equal magni- tudes, e.g. a pair of weights. The subject is then asked which is heavier, his answer recorded, and the experiment repeated many times. If the number of correct answers be only about equal to the number of incorrect ones, it is concluded that the dif- ference between the two stimuli is inappreciable. The experiments are then repeated with weights differing by a greater amount, values being finally reached for which the number of correct judgments definitely exceeds — when tested by an adequate statistical process — the number of incorrect ones; it is inferred that at this stage a definite subjective appreciation of the difference has been attained. (3) The Method of the Mean Error. Using the former illustration, a subject is asked to choose from a large number of weights all those exactly equal to a standard. All the weights he chooses will not be exactly equal; the differences from the standard are recorded, ^ and their sum, divided by the total number of trials, is thought to give the average error the subject may be expected to make. This magnitude will be a lower limit for the threshold value. It may be said at onpe that results obtained by the employment of any of these methods require the use of most refined statistical reasoning before any importance whatever can be attached to them, and such an analysis has not always been forthcoming. As a result of investigations of this kind, carried out by Weber, Fechner, and others, the following conclusions were ^ Without regard to sign. INTRODUCTION 5 reached: — The just noticeable increase of a stimukis bears a constant ratio to the original stimulus, or, two stimuli in order to be discriminated must be in a constant ratio, the latter being independent of the absolute magnitudes of the stimuli. The actual value of this ratio, although constant for any one sense mechanism, varies from organ to organ. This is Weber's well-known " law." Accepting Weber's "law" as true, an attempt Avas made by Fechner to express sensations in terms of quantitative units. Having adopted certain assumptions of which the most important perhaps is, that all just noticeable differences of sen- sation contain an equal number of sensation-units, it was easy to deduce from Weber's " law''^ that the sensation varies as the logarithm of the stimulus. This corollary is Fechner 's " law." The validity of Weber's "law" and Fechner's corollary have been hotly contested. The problem is complex, and little is gained by either confident abuse ^ or praise. The following remarks, which agree essentially with v. Kries' opinions,^ indicate some of the more important difficulties in the way of accepting Fechner's conclusions. Experimental methods of measuring threshold stimulus values are open to criticism from both the theoretical and practical standpoints. The theoretical objections are inti- mately associated with certain deductions from the theory of correlation, and cannot be examined here. A practical diffi- culty is the tacit assumption that the perceptive mechanism of the person who is the subject of our experiments is a constant factor throughout a series of observations. This is not, however, true. What is a just noticeable stimulus at 1 The simplest way of putting it is : If E be the measure of a sensation and dE a liminal increment, R the measure of a stimulus and dR a small increment, then K — = dE, -where K is a constant (Weber's law), R hence / K =- = E, hf- or K loge R + Ki = E (Fechner's law). ' Such, for example, as will be found in Prof. W. James's Principles of Psychology, vol. i. pp. 548-9. 8 See particularly "Zur Psychologie der Sinne," Nagel's Handb. d. Physiol. d. Menschen, vol. iii. pp. 16, etc. 6 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES one time may be inappreciable at another, owing to a change, physiological or psychological, in this factor. A higher threshold value is often observed when we proceed from a. weaker to a stronger stimulus than in following the reverse order. It follows that the experimental methods employed to establish Weber's " law " may yield, and do yield, inconstant results, and a system of measurements which gives different readings when operating through the same range is of little service. It has also been shown that even under favourable conditions the formula is definitely invalid for very weak or very strong stimuli. We seem, therefore, forced to conclude that the experi- mental basis of Weber's " law " is not firm enough to permit our attaching much importance to the formula itself. These objections are directed to the foryn Weber's "law'" takes ; that some relation of this kind exists is probable, for all judgments involve comparison ; but it may well be doubted whether the present state of knowledge permits any more definite assertion. If the conclusions just stated be sound, Fechner's corollary evidently fails, but, over and above this, special objections to it can be assigned. For instance, Fechner's " law " assumes that sensations are quantitatively measurable, and this is, to say the least, not self-evidently true. Measurements only have a meaning when the unit is clearly and uniquely defined, or, under very special circumstances only, when its. existence can be inferred. Changes of temperature could be measured in various ways, and to say that the temperature of A is 1 degree higher than that of B conveys a definite idea to the mind of him who realises that the scale of units is fixed and has a constant physical significance. In the case of sensations, no such scale has been given.^ To say that one body has twice the mass of another is. intelligible, because the unit of mass can be defined ; * ' See in this connection Lloyd Morgan's Croonian Lecture, Proc. Roy, Soc, vol. Ixviii. p. 459. ' Even in such a case the definition is not particularly easy ; the reader should consult La Science et Vlfypothise, by H. Poincare, chap, viii. INTRODUCTION 7 to say that one sensation is twice as intense as another means nothing until the unit is fixed. It seems doubtful, then, whether we are in a position to measure sensations at all, and until we are, the formulation of a quantitative law is hardly justifiable. Some of these points may come up for consideration once more in connection with the various organs, but it is well to note from the outset that the numerous statements which have been made with regard to the sense organs in general are liable to very real objections. We are not yet in sight of that simple, all-embracing formula to supply which is the aim of science.! BOOKS AND PAPERS RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY (Bibliographical references under this heading are intended to help students desiring to pursue the subject further. They are invariably confined to works readily accessible in a moderately good library.) MtJLLicR's Law TV. Nagel, Die Lehre von den Spezifischen Sinnesenergien, Handb. d. Physiol, d. Menschen, herausgegeben von W. Nagel, vol. iii. pp. 1-15. L. Asher, Das Gesetz der Spezifischen Sinnesenergie und seine Beziehung zur Entwickslungslehre, Zeitsch. f. Psychologie und Physi- ologic der Sinnesorgane, 1906, vol. xli. p. 157 (this journal will be denoted in future by the letters Z.P.P.S.O.). Weber's and Fechner's Laws /. V. Kries, op. cit. (a thoughtful and instructive criticism). G. F. Lipps, Grundriss der Psychophysik, Leipzig, Sammlung Goschen, 1903. An elementary account from the psycho-physical standpoint. G. E. Miiller, Die Gesichtspunkte und die Tatsachen der psycho- physischen Methodik, Ergebnisse d. Physiologie, zweiter Jahrgang, 1903, second part, pp. 267-517. (A very full, and somewhat difficult account, with complete bibliography of modern psycho-physical literature.) Professor James's remarks can be read with pleasure {op. cit.), although his criticism does not appear adequate. ' It is not of course implied that to assume a, quantitative relation between stimulation processes and sensation processes is necessarily illegitimate. In fact, as will appear later, such an assumption often leads to extremely useful working hypotheses ; the student ought, however, to realise what caution the proceeding requires. CHAPTER II GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF CUTANEOUS SENSATION In studying any brancli of knowledge it is an obvious advantage to begin with its simplest and most readily in- telligible manifestations, and to approach only by gradual stages its more complex parts. In most cases this plan can be readily followed, but in sense physiology peculiar difficulties are encountered. To judge from the number of pages allotted in a general text- book to the sense organs of the skin, on the one hand, and to the eye, on the other, it might be supposed that the physiology of the former is much easier than that of the latter. Such is by no means the case; in fact, the converse proposition is quite arguable. At the same time, the study of cutaneous sen- sation is in many respects peculiarly instructive, and it will therefore be convenient to undertake it at once. At the outset we shall see that the tendency of modern workers has been to conceive of the cutaneous surface as a universe of points, each reacting in a specific manner to the application of stimuli. We shall study the results which have led up to this, and then examine important modifica- tions of the conception which have been rendered necessary by more recent discoveries. Pre- Aristotelian writings on cutaneous sensations are too vague to be of service to the modern reader. The scientific history of the subject really commences with Aristotle.^ Aristotle recognised distinctly that the sense of " touch " is separable into many sense modalities, although his limited means of investigation did not permit him to render the ' For information as to Greek Sense Physiology the reader is strongly advised to consult Professor J. I. Beare's admirable work, Greek Theories of Elementary Cognition from Alcmeeon to Aristotle. Clarendon Press, 1906. PHYSIOLOGY OF CUTANEOUS SENSATION 9 distinctions precise. He also anticipated the view that the organs are affected by a change of state. He regarded the sense of touch as fundamental, and the only one necessary to existence. In both these conclusions Aristotle has been followed by the majority of subsequent writers. Without attempting to detail the course of investigations Avhich have led up to the modern epoch, I shall describe in as few words as possible the state of knowledge gained up to a few years ago. At least four distinct qualities of sensation can be dis- tinguished when stimuli are applied to the skin — those of touch or pressure, warmth, cold, and pain. Some writers have advocated an even finer analysis, regarding, for instance, the sensation of itching as specific, but this is hardly war- ranted by experiment. It is quite clear that a series of points can be mapped out over the skin, stimulation of which produces the sensations of heat, cold, pain, and touch ; the question of pain, however, will occupy us subsequently. The principles of experimental methods for this sort of work are simple enough, although the actual instruments vary enormously in detail. For experimenting on tactile per- ception, V. Frey, who has done pioneer work in cutaneous sense physiology, devised a series of valuable instruments.^ They consist of a number of wooden rods, to the ends of which hairs of differing thickness are fixed at right angles. If the hair be applied perpendicularly to a point on the skin and pressure exerted, then with the application of a certain pressure the hair will be visibly bent. The amount of pressure required to bend visibly any given hair can be experimentally determined, and with a large series of hairs of different thickness we can use pressures measured and graded with some accuracy. Toulouse and Vaschide have justly remarked^ that dif- ferent readings, will be given with the same hair unless the pressure is arrested at the instant of bending, a point not ' Bloch described a similar but less perfect instrument in 1891 (Arch. dei'%s.,Avrill891). 2 Toulouse, Vaschide, and Pieron, Technique de Psychologic Experi- mentale, Paris, 1904, p. 64. 10 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES easy to determine with absolute precision. I also think that the degree of moisture in the air makes some difference. Notwithstanding these and certain other objections, the method seems the most accurate at present in use. To study temperature sensations, a hollow pencil-shaped rod through which water at a definite temperature can be circulated is the best, although French workers claim to have obtained good results by using minute drops of water at various temperatures as stimuli. The topography of sensation points was first completely worked out by Magnus Blix, and his conclusions have been endorsed by the best modern observers (Sommer). These conclusions are : — (1) Pressure points are closely related to the distribution of hairs, each hair having a pressure point near its site of eruption (v. Frey), the point corresponding on the surface to the situation of a hair follicle. There may, however, be some pressure points which do not correspond to hairs. (2) Cold reacting points are more numerous than warm points, Sommer finding 12-13 cold points sq. cm. against 1-2 warm points, but the distribution is by no means uniform. Respecting the anatomical structures which correspond to the sense points, hardly anything is certainly known. For instance, it is not easy to tell whether any given nerve fibre conveys centripetal or centrifugal impulses, the ordinary methods of determining the matter being generally im- possible of application. The relation of pressure points to hairs renders it probable that the nerve fibre entering the root sheath and forming ring-like arborisations round the upper part of the follicle are especially associated with the pressure sense. It is also possible that the Meissner corpuscles are pressure end organs, and may even, in the hairless parts of the skin, replace the hairs.^ It has been stated that Krause's end bulbs and Ruffini's organs are associated respectively with sensations of cold and warmth, but experimental evidence is of the slenderest. ' v. Frey, Beitrage s. Sinnetphys. d. Haut, Konig. Sachs. Gesellschnft d. Wisscnsoh., Marz 1895. PHYSIOLOGY OF CUTANEOUS SENSATION 11 Sensations of pressure can normally be evoked from the whole cutaneous surface, the mucous membrane of the mouth, the tongue, teeth, and nares. Kespecting the cornea and glans penis much difference of opinion has existed, but the reason probably may be sought in a failure to distinguish the exact forms of stimuli employed, as will be clear subsequently. Strtlmpell has recently^ asserted that sensations of pressure can be set up in internal parts, tendons, fasciae, muscles, and periosteum. Thunberg ^ has suggested that in this case tactile impressions are confused with those of pain. The adequate {vide supra) stimulus for the sense of pres- sure is of course mechanical, but since any stimulus applied to the surface does not operate directly upon the end organs — if such there be — the question arises as to what physical conditions in the immediate vicinity of the possibly recipient structures give rise to sensations of pressure. This problem was solved by v. Frey and Kiesow ^ in 1899. When a point, e.g. the end of a hair, is pushed against the skin, the pressure is. greatest at the part immediately in contact with the point, and diminished in the deeper and lateral regions ; that is to say, there is a fall of pressure from without inwards. Conversely, if a small plate be affixed to the skin and traction made upon it, the pressure is least externally, and increases from without inwards. In the first case we have a negative and in the second a positive slope of pressure from without inwards. V. Frey and Kiesow found that the sensations experienced in the two cases appeared to be identical, provided the magnitude of the pull in one case were equal to that of the push in the other; that is to say, the end organ is excited by a change in the ex- isting pressure relations of end organ and superficies, whether the change be positive or negative, increase or decrease. Although the variation in pressure in the neighbourhood 1 JDeuUche med. Wochen., 1904, pp. 1411 and 1460. ' Nagel's Ilandb. d. Phys. d. Menschen, vol. iii. p. 657. » Z. P.P. S.O., vol. XX. pp. 126, etc., especially p. 153. Those desirous of expeilmenting on the skin should note the cautions given in this paper. 12 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES of an end organ is an essential condition for the latter's excitation, other factors modify the result. For instance, if the stimulus be a falling weight, its velocity is a factor. The greater the velocity, within limits, the less the weight required to produce a just noticeable sensation. The sensation does not, however, vary as the kinetic energy of the exciting mass. Two weights allowed to drop from heights inversely proportional to their masses would possess equal amounts of kinetic energy on reaching the skin, but the smaller weight produces the more intense sensation. The extent of surface to which the stimulus is applied is also an important matter. It might be thought that the sensations excited in two areas would be identical if the pressure per unit of surface were the same in both, cases. This is not so if, for instance, an area of -5 sq. mm. be com- pared with one of "25 sq. mm. ; a greater pressure per unit is required in the second than in the first case to produce a just noticeable sensation. The optimum surface is approxi- mately '5 sq. mm. ; as we increase or decrease the extent of surface stimulated from these dimensions, the liminal value increases (v. Frey and Kiesow, op. cit.). A partial explana- tion is found in the above-mentioned fact, that pressure sensations are excited by a slope of pressure from the point of application to the end organs. The greater the surface pressed upon, the less steep the slope from within outwards owing to lateral thrusts. This does not account for the fact that if the surface be diminished below a certain value the pressure must be increased, and the proposed solutions of the problem seem hardly adequate. The above-mentioned experiments were made on hairless portions of the skin ; the conclusions, however, are, with some modification, valid for hair-clothed surfaces. The tactile apparatus of the hairy skin is more excitable and more readily fatigued than that of the smooth areas. Stimuli not distinguishable on a smooth can be detected if applied to a hairy surface. This lowering of the magnitude of a threshold stimulus in the hairy regions perhaps depends PHYSIOLOGY OF CUTANEOUS SENSATION 13 on two factors,^ the diminution of the skin area stimulated, since the pressure is only effective through the base of the hair follicle, and the lever-like action of the hair itself. The comparison and discrimination of two pressure stimuli have been the objects of many investigations. The quantitative results are discordant, but the following con- clusion appears to be well founded. It is easier to compare weights applied successively than when they act simultaneously ; i.e. a more accurate com- parison can be made between the memory of a sensation and an actual sensation than between simultaneous sensations (Weber). Again, the rapidity with which stimuli operate influences one's powers of discrimination. Generally, the slower the rate of change, the higher the liminal stimulus value (Dohrn). It is also easier to perceive that a given weight differs from another than to say whether it is heavier or lighter. When such discrimination is possible, it is" easier to detect an increase in weight than a decrease. If successive weights be applied with a rapidity such that, in equal times, the same fractions of the original weights are added, the ratios of increase of stimulus to original stimulus give results in fair accordance with Weber's law. It has, however, been shown (Seashore, Hall, and Motora) that the results are variable with the experimental conditions, and no definite proof of the law as applied to tactile impressions is forth- coming. Experiments performed with the object of determining what frequency of pressure stimuli gives a fused sensation have yielded most discordant results,^ and need not be dis- cussed. The length of time the sensation persists after removal of the stimulating body depends on the skin area ; in some places the sensation vanishes at once, in others, e.g. the forehead, the after-effect may with moderately strong stimuli persist some little time. , . ^ . J IT 1 m,j..--. J. .7. ...1 li n m 4t'J.i. Nuel, op. eit., p. 26. COMPARATIVE PHYSIOLOGY OF VISION 89 demonstrate clearly that bees possess the faculty of distin- guishmg colours.' And, nevertheless, the observed facts could be just as easily explained by a simple difference in intensity of a single luminous sensation. But we shall see that even this single sensation is not demonstrated in the case of the bee by the experiments of the authors. The same writer, seeing that ants react to ultra-violet rays, draws the following conclusions : ' It is therefore probable that the ultra-violet rays produce in ants the sensation of a distinct colour, as different (for the ant) as is the green from the red (for ourselves).' He even asks himself 'if the white light of these insects is not different from our white, since it contains (for the ant) one more colour.' " At the base of this reasoning, erroneous but generally admitted in comparative biology, will be found another error which is just as fundamental, and consists in admitting that, at least in the case of man, visual movements or motor reactions to light are provoked either by the sensations of light themselves, or by other psychic states (psychic repre- sentation, pleasure, displeasure, will, etc.), which, according to the psychologists, are derivatives of sensations." * It seems to me, for the following reasons, that this criticism is purely verbal. The fact that we havp no objective know- ledge of any sensations in animals has long been a common- place of educated men. Nuel has no justification in supposing that the authors he criticises are not familiar with this plati- tude. Being men, however, and writing for men, they use the language of human beings. To say that an ant " prefers " red to violet light is a short way of saying that its motor re- actions under the two conditions are such that, were it endowed with a consciousness similar to that of the speaker, such a preference could be inferred to exist. If any one asserts that the ant does as a matter of fact possess a consciousness ^ Nuel, op. cit., pp. 11-12. Beer {op. cit., p. 34) writes, somewhat more •cautiously: " We have no right and no necessity to attribute to lower animals with no demonstrable associative memory sensations at all, and what is built up on sensations, pleasure, Unlust, Common Sensation, etc. They may have these or not ; provisionally we do not require for a functional knowledge of their activities to regard them psychologically otherwise than as to us a new feind of apparatus." 90 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES similar to that of man, and twists his experimental results to- agree with such a dogma, he is no doubt acting unscientific- ally, but I see no evidence of this in any of the numerous passages singled out by Nuel for attack. Indeed, the war might well be carried into the enemy's country. While ostensibly employing a terminology which is non-committal as to psychic states, Nuel, Beer, and others of their school often make merry at the expense of those who, however vaguely, attribute consciousness to "lower" animals. Now the one animal about the conscious states of which I " know " anything is myself; therefore when I have to consider any living creature, the structure and reactions to stimuli of which compel me to classify it with myself somewhere in the animal kingdom, I must, in default of evidence which, by definition, I cannot obtain, attribute to it some degree of "consciousness" or "blunt Occam's razor." When we turn to the inorganic world, or even to the vegetable kingdom, the physiological points of difference outweigh the points of resemblance, and the attribution of consciousness ceases to be justifiable. The suggestion of Beer and Nuel, that the con- ventional method of writing about the sense of animals is akin to the pre-Galilean physics which spoke of water seeking its level and nature abhorring a vacuum, is, I think, purely ad captandum. I admit that anthropomorphic language has- sometimes led writers to go beyond their evidence, and that Beer and his colleagues have done good service in pointing this out, but I cannot think that the evil is as great as they assert, or that the remedy they propose is really an efficient one. As a matter of fact, the very authors who are particu- larly scornful of others' anthropomorphism not infrequently commit the same fault themselves. For instance, Professor Nuel writes : " The insect which orientates itself in flying among small obstacles reveals a remarkable development of its moto-reactions, but no true icon-reactions. To judge by what occurs in man, the latter even appear to be impos- sible during so rapid a movement." ^ Why, on Professor Nuel's principles, s/ioicW we "judge by ' Nuel, op. cit., p. 91. COMPARATIVE PHYSIOLOGY OF VISION 91 what occurs in man " ? Further on he writes : " Of what nature is this visual memory of places in the hymenoptera ? There is certainly no question of a revival of previous icono- reactions, of which bees are certainly deprived, and which, besides, could not be produced during so rapid a translation as the flight of a hymenopteron." ^ The man who writes such sentences as these ought, I think, to be a little careful how he accuses others of anthropo- centric errors. I shall therefore, in the brief notes which ■ follow, make no attempt to avoid the common descriptive words used by Forel, Wassmann, and other comparative biologists. Our knowledge of the anatomy and histology of the eyes of Invertebrata apart from Insecta owes much to the work of Hesse, ably summarised by Beer in the memoir already referred to. Exner has studied the faceted eye of insects in great detail, while the development of histological technique, particularly the silver methods of Golgi and the work of Ramon y Cajal, has enabled anatomists to describe the structure of the eyes of most vertebrata with remarkable precision. In some quite simple animals, simple, that is, from the point of view at which we have so far arrived, the light- perceiving structures are decidedly complex; indeed the complexity of the latter by no means goes hand in hand with the general complexity of the organism, so that no satisfactory zoological classification is possible. Generally speaking, we find one or more elongated structures formed from cytoplasm or excreted by it, often exhibiting a laminated structure, and, in the majority of cases, associated with pig- ment. Pigment, however, is not invariably present, as, for instance, in the case of the " photirzellen " of several annelid worms and the more complex photirzellen of the leech (Fig. 7). A stage beyond the " photirzellen " is seen in the ocelli of leeches (Fig. 8), while yet more specialised are the ocelli of flat worms (Fig. 9). Some of the polychsetes exhibit true camera eyes (Fig. 10), and in cephalopods the retinal structure is highly complex. ' Nuel, op. cit. , p. 104. 92 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES Fig In arthropods we meet witli a specialised type of eye, the form of which may be studied in the cockroach (Periplaneta orientalis). The outer surface shows many divisions, and a vertical section through the principal axis proves, that the superficial divisions mark the bases of long truncated cones, the ends of which rest on a semicircular basement membrane. Each cone is a single unit (or ommatidium) of the eye, con- sisting of numerous parts, for an account of which reference must be made to works on compara- tive histology. Our knowledge of the physio- logy of ocelli and "photirzellen" ^ is so incomplete, that we must content ourselves with saying that they are concerned in some way with the reception of lumi- nous stimuli; with respect to the compound eyes of insects, we know a little more. The work of Exner, Grenacher, and Oscar Schmidt has demonstrated that a separate image cannot be formed on each retinular ele- ment, and that the older theory of Johannes Mtiller is more plausible. According to this theory, each facet will receive rays from a different part of the object, owing to the small size and great number of the facets, and as a result of the co-operation of all the facets a sort of mosaic pattern will be produced on the retina, not an immense number of small distinct images. Exner seems to have proved that the transparent structures cannot project an image, but only concentrate the rays which form the visual field of each facet. Evidently, if this theory 1 Which may be translated by " Light-receiving Cell." 7. — Section tlirough aa Ocellus of Hirudo mcdicvnalis (Beer), ejj^ Epithelium. LC Light-receiving Cell. S Sensory Bulbs. SN Nerve to Bulbs. no Optic Nerve. m Muscle. COMPARATIVE PHYSIOLOGY OF VISION 93 be correct, the distinctness of vision, or, to speak more accu- rately, the localisation of the source of the different rays, depends on the co-ordination of the facets ; the greater the number of facets, the more accurate will be the localisation in eyes of the same total volume, since each facet will be smaller and illuminated by a smaller portion of the object. The advantage of having numerous small facets will be increased if the whole eye is markedly convex, because in that event not only will the visual field be increased, but fewer facets will receive rays from the same external point. We should further expect, if the theory of mosaic vision be -^m Fie. 8. Fig. 9. u, Light-receiving Organ of Zumbricus castaneus (Beer). LC Light-receiving Cell. ep Ordinary Epithelial Cell. h Light-receiving Organ of Lumbricus ruiellus (Beer). LC Light-receiving Cell. ep Ordinary Epithelial Cell. sound, that visual acuity, in the human sense of being able to distinguish accurately the forms of objects, would be poor in insects, while the perception of a movement should be good. All these statements are supported by the experiments of Forel. In support of the assertion that insects perceive movement better than form may be cited his observations on wasps. Forel noticed that a wasp hunting flies was frequently deceived by a nail in the wall about the same size and shape as a fly, pouncing on it more than once. When dead flies or spiders of about the same size were placed on a table the wasp carried them off indifferently, but took no notice of insects 94 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES much larger or much smaller than the dead flies mixed with them. Forel also adduces, in favour of the view that the more facets the more distinct the vision, the great visual powers of the dragon-fly as compared with the much less endowed ant. Forel has also proved hy varnishing the eyes that humble-bees, wasps, and numerous dipterous insects are unable when thus blinded to direct their flight satisfactorily, while this power remains if both antennse and the forepart of the pharynx are removed. The numerous experiments of Lord Avebury and Forel have made it probable that bees and wasps can distinguish certain colours, and the objections of Plateau to their results do not seem to be well founded, but I have no space to give the detailed evidence. There is also reason to think that some power of distin- guishing the form of objects is possessed by these insects. Forel carried out numerous experiments on a wasp, of which the following may be quoted : — riG.io.— SectionthroughanOoeiius "The following day my wasp re- of Pianaria torva (Beer). tumed twice following to eat at the i? ffght-'r^Sving Cell. cross left at the same place. I then R EoTuke structures. took her and removed bothantennse. She flew away, but returned half- an-hour later to eat, always at -the cross, which I had left in the same place. After her departure I put a similar cross, at the side of the first cross, but without honey, then on the other side a narrow band with honey, finally removing the honey cross. The wasp returned, flew straight on to the cross, alighting just in the centre (where the honey was on the other cross), and searched it vainly for a considerable time. Then, although deprived of antennse, she began to search, doubtless recollecting that the white papers on which the honey was had already often changed in place and aspect. " She quickly found it on the narrow band, not without, COMPARATIVE PHYSIOLOGY OF VISION 95 however, having passed -within a few millimetres of it two or three times without noticing it, which would not have happened to her if she had had her antennae. She only noticed it when her mouth touched it.''^ The great difficulty of such experiments as these is that of eliminating all disturbing factors, and the reader who desires to form definite conclusions will be compelled not only to study the works of these observers, but to perform experiments on his own account, otherwise he will be in danger of neglected sources of error which are not usually described in experimental records. It is unnecessary to refer to the experiments which have been made on the vision of vertebrata ; in general terms, it may be said that the perception of movement can be more easily shown to exist than the kind of vision associated with stimulation of the fovea centralis retinaj in man. This is, perhaps, true even for animals, such as the monkey, which possess a retinal fovea histologically similar to that of man, although in the monkey many fine movements seem to prove the existence of a type of vision altogether comparable with that of man. Of course any one who has played with a dog does not need to be told that the friend of man is far more keenly interested by moving than by stationary objects. It will be clear that many important problems in con- nection with the comparative physiology of vision still await solution, and that the subject is one of peculiar interest. BOOKS AND PAPERS RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY The student should first read the account of the histology of visual tissues in Principles of Animal Histology by V. Dahlgren and W. A. Kepner, Macmillan, 1908, then the works mentioned in this chapter in the following order : (1) Beer's paper, (2) Nuel's book (first part), (3) Forel. Ample references to the vast literature of the subject will be found in these sources. '■ Forel, The Senses of Insects, Yearsley's translation, London, Methuen, pp. 27-28. CHAPTER XI RETINAL PROCESSES, ELECTRICAL, PHOTOTROPIC, AND CHEMICAL RESPONSES The physiology of the eye falls naturally into two main divisions : in the first we regard the organ as an optical instrument, investigate its structure, and determine the constants of the lens system together with the latter's defects and compensations. We also investigate the relations sub- sisting between the optical mechanism and the afferent or efferent nervous paths, together with the manner in which these relations may be modified or destroyed. This branch does not fall within the scope of a treatise such as the present. I assume the reader to possess some knowledge of the easier parts of physiological optics, and intend to deal solely with the other division of our subject. This investiga- tion starts with the arrival of a stimulus at the retina, ex- amines the resulting changes in this structure, and attempts to associate them with the materials yielded by an analysis of visual sensations and stimuli. In order that we may not lose sight of the wood for the trees, it is necessary to dis- tinguish carefully between the probable and the possible, under a penalty of losing ourselves in a maze of pure specu- lation. When a beam of light falls on the retina, certain marked physical changes occur; in some of these cases a distinction is possible, because the reaction does not appear to be the same over the whole surface of the retina, a fact which apparently depends on the non-uniformity of structure displayed by this membrane. This statement cannot be generalised, because certain forms of response are such that we can, under possible experimental conditions, only consider their relation to the retina as a whole ; an example is the photo-electric response. If the retina and optic nerve be connected up through a galvanometer, a " current of rest '' is RETINAL PROCESSES 97 observed, its direction depending on whether the inner or outer surface of the retina is used. When hght falls on the retina, this current undergoes a somewhat complex variation. In the isolated retina there is first a positive then a negative variation ; on cutting off the light a positive variation is produced. In conditions most like the normal state the positive phase is better marked in the case of the frog, but in birds and mammals a negative phase alone is obtained. This result appears in the absence of the visual purple, although the reaction is more intense if this pigment be present, and it is clear that the whole effect varies in in- tensity with the part of the spectrum employed. Another general change of state is the fact that after exposure to light the retina is less readily stained with acid dyes, but this is not so definite nor so generally accepted as the electrical response.^ Thirdly, we have the well-known phototropic reaction of the pigment epithelium. In a frog's eye which has been kept in the dark the pigment layer is easily separated from the rods and cones ; the pigment granules are in the cell bodies round the outer limbs of the rods. After exposure to light, the layers can only be separated with difficulty; the pigment is much more abundant between the outer limbs of the rods, and also passes between the inner limbs as far as the external limiting membrane. This change can be induced in the frog by ten minutes' exposure to light. The various parts of the spectrum are not equally efficient in producing this change, the long waved lights, especially red, being but feebly active. The pigment will not move if the central nervous system is destroyed, and the reaction may take place in one eye as a consequence of light falling on the opposite retina. A third response is cone shortening, which appears to be due to contraction of the inner limbs. Just as in the last case, cerebral injury abolishes the power of movement, and the change may be produced indirectly by stimulation of the opposite eye. Here, again, the various spectral lights differ in power to stimulate, but certain peculiarities may be noticed. Red light produces a 1 See Dittler, Pflilij. Arch., 1907, oxx. 44. 98 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES greater effect than on the pigment cells, and shortening may follow stimuli other than light, e.g. changes of temperature. Cone shortening also seems to take place in mammalian eyes, while the pigment change is only definiteljestablished for the frog. ^ S«, ^.ci^^-? P^^i /f/7 . V^jZ" Finally we come to the important question of Visual Purple or Retinal Red. This is a substance of a deep purple- red hue, present mainly {perhaps entirely) in the outer limbs of the rods. On exposure to light the pigment is rapidly bleached without passing through an intermediate yellow stage, as has frequently been asserted. This bleaching is limited to the part upon which light falls, and the latter is alone capable of decolourising the pigment in a living eye. Regeneration takes place either in darkness or red light, apparently depending on the existence in or production by the pigment epithelium of a substance called by Kiihne Rhodophyllin, as suggested by the following experiment. When a frog is curarised, oedema occvirs between retina and choroid, and the former is separated from the pigment layer, which tends to adhere to the choroid. If the frog be now exposed to light until bleaching results, no regeneration occurs on detaching the retina and placing it in a dark chamber ; but if the pigment layer is simply placed in contact with the retina, new formation proceeds as under normal conditions. We seem, therefore, entitled to assume that regeneration is independent of direct continuity, and is due to chemical or physical processes. It was once believed that the formative substance is derived from the coloured globules in the epithelial cells, but this is probably incorrect, as the process seems to occur when they are wholly or mainly absent, as in the albino rabbit. Visual purple can be ex- tracted from the retina with a solution of bile salts, and its absorption spectrum has been examined ; the absorption does not appear to be identical in purple from diiFerent animals, but the significance of the variations is unknown. If we compare the bleaching powers of the homogeneous lights, we find that red and yellow are practically inoperative, the parts of the spectrum falling between the Fraunhofer D and E lines having, on the other hand, maximal powers. RETINAL PROCESSES 99 If we ask ourselves what value these facts have for a study of visual processes, we shall conclude that some dis- tinctions must be made. In the first place, experimental conditions requisite for observing electrical changes render the co-ordination of the latter with sensation-differences hardly practicable. Haas suggested that the electromotive response could be regarded as a measure of sensation-in- tensity, and plotted the curve obtained when the light intensities were taken along one co-ordinate axis and the electromotive response along the other. If the stimuli were increased in geometric and the electromotive responses varied in arithmetic ratios, as they should if Fechner's law holds, the curve would be a straight line. Haas' figure shows that this is only very roughly true, and in any case the assumption that the electrOmotiva response is a measure of sensation-change appears to be quite arbitrary. Perhaps all we can safely infer is that light produces some change of state in the retinal structure, a change not of the same magnitude, nor even, it may be, qualitatively identical Avhen we employ stimuli of different wave lengths; the exact relationship between physical process and psycho-physio- logical response is still obscure. These remarks also apply to the phototropic movement of the pigmented epithelium and the shortening of the retinal cones, but on turning to the facts relating to visual purple, we are tempted to pursue our investigation further. Here we have a substance only, or at least mainly, present in certain areas of the retina; if it play a part in the physiological processes of vision, these should be different in different regions. We might, of course, have the final result, the visual sensation, identically the same even if the physiological precursors were quite difierent ; but if, in fact, we find that visual sensations yielded by stimuli acting on different parts of the sensitive area are not uniform, that a modifying factor applied to the whole retina affects it differentially, we have some ground for attributing definite importance to the visual purple. To obtain the grounds necessary for forming a judgment on this point, we must first ascertain what alterations in visual responsiveness are produced by changing the totality of conditions to which the 100 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES eye is subjected; secondly, we must knoAv whether the changes are entirely or chiefly confined to any particular region ; and lastly, whether any structural peculiarities give us a plausible explanation. It has long been known that the nature of the response of the eye to a stimulus largely depends upon whether, before the experiment, the subject has rested in a dark room or been exposed to light, that is to say, whether there is a condition of light or dark adaptation. The detailed study of these and allied phenomena will form the subject of the next two chapters. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY TV. Nugel, Die Wirkungen des Liohtes auf die Netzhaut (Nagel's Hand- bueh der Physiol., Bd. iii. pp. 91-108) contains an excellent account, with full references to the literature. CHAPTER XII VISUAL ADAPTATION— PERIPHEEAL VISION TOTAL COLOUR-BLINDNESS As was mentioned at the end of the last chapter, the fact that different parts of the retina are unequally responsive to light has long been known, and the path which led up to this knowledge was a study of the facts of visual adapta- tion to varying intensities of light. Aubert was one of the first exact observers to notice that the response of the eye to feeble stimulation was heightened by a sojourn in darkness. He also noticed that the threshold value of a stimulus in such cases varied inversely as the area stimulated. Since this could only mean that when light fell on the peripheral part of the retina its value as a stimulus was enhanced, Aubert's observation really contained the nucleus of all subsequent work. His general results were speedily confirmed. Charpentier, among others, found that the central part of the "dark" retina, although exhibiting an increased responsiveness as compared with the same region in the "light" eye,'^ was much less responsive than the periphery. This increase was more marked in the case of the short waved spectral colours, and there is even some doubt whether adaptation makes any difierence at all to the activity of red rays. It is also rather uncertain whether the fovea centralis retinae is affected by resting in a dark room ; the evidence is conflicting, and the matter is still too con- troversial for it to be discussed here. A particular case of adaptation which is of much interest is " Purkinje's Phenomenon," an effect which can be described in the following way. If one examines an ordinary spectrum, the brightest part of it seems to occupy the neighbourhood ' I sball speak of an eye which has been rested in the dark as a " dark " and one previously exposed to light as a "light " eye. 101 102 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES of tlie yellow or orange-yellow ; if, however, the physical intensity of the spectrum be diminished, for instance, by moving the source of light farther away from the prism, the maximum of apparent brightness shifts towards the violet end. With the feeblest illumination which enables one to distinguish the spectral colours at all, the brightest part is at the junction of the green and blue. Ewald Hering has, in my opinion, demonstrated that these changes are due to adaptation, using an experimental method of characteristic simplicity and elegance. Two rooms which could be in- dependently darkened are separated by a light-proof partition, in which two slits are cut and covered with pigmented glass. The amount of light transmitted by these slits could be varied independently with the aid of reflectors. So long as the room occupied by the observer is kept at a constant illumination, diminishing the physical intensity of the light traversing the two slits, which are covered with blue and red glass respectively, does not change their relative in- tensities. If, however, the observation room is darkened, the blue slit immediately appears brighter than the red one, even while the physical intensity of the light passing through them is unaltered. The effect is much enhanced by a stay in darkness, and is more noticeable in indirect (peripheral) than in direct (foveal or central) vision. Burch has noticed a similar phenomenon, and we may fairly consider Purkinje's effect as dependent not on physical intensity, but adaptation. Before going further, we must ask ourselves one question, What does one mean by saying that different colours are equally or unequally bright ? I am not acquainted with any really complete answer, and shall fall back upon a purely empirical justification. Ask a dozen normal persons to look at a spectrum in daylight, and one finds they all agree in picking out some part of the orange-yellow, which they call the brightest point of the spectrum. They mean, I take it, that this part produces somehow a predominant effect in consciousness. How this comes about is matter for a psycho- logical discussion; it is no part of the pure physiology of vision. If we adopt this relatively humble standard of brightness, comparative results are attainable in numerous VISUAL ADAPTATION 103 ways. One of the best methods is that of " flicker," to which Haycraft and Kivers have devoted much attention. When a series of sectors are whirled round on a machine called a colour mixer, the velocity necessary to produce a fused sensation depends upon the brightness of the sectors ; hence with different sets of sectors equal in size, the velocity of rotation which just extinguishes the sensation of flicker affords a measure of brightness, which may be taken, within fairly wide limits, to vary inversely as the rapidity of rotation. It may be well to remark here that work of this kind, like most researches on the sense of sight, is easy neither to perform nor to interpret. To carry out satisfactory experiments on a single person requires much attention to details, and even when the obstacles have been surmounted we have to consider the question of individual variations. The problem of variations in the reaction to sensory stimuli is just as urgently in need of adequate statistical treatment in the case of sense physiology as in any other biological field, and even less likely to obtain it. Hering several years ago noted the relative darkening of red as one passes from central to peripheral vision ; he compared a pure red, a spectral mixture of red and blue- green (656 /i/i + 470 /ti/i), and daylight. The converse was found to hold for spectral green (505 /ti/t), but his results were not quite satisfactory, for momentary dark adaptation occurred during the experiments. Tschermak, who studied the whole question systematically in the " light " eye, found in indirect vision a relative diminution in brightness for light of wave length between 693 and 525 /i/i, no change from 525 to 516 jxii, an increase from 516 to 466 /n/i. Similar changes were observable in " dark " eyes. Another method was to start with a large field of colour- less light, produced by mixing together complementaries, and then to diminish its size. It has been found that a colourless mixture of spectral red and bluish-green becomes, with such a real diminution, redder and darker; if the change of size be an increase, it becomes greener and brighter. In fact, both for " light " and " dark " eyes colourless matches valid for the periphery do not hold for the fovea centralis. 104 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES and vice versa. Apart from adaptation, it is interesting to see whether merely changing the intensities (physical) of two mixtures renders the match invalid. On this point much difference of opinion exists, which is not surprising in view of the experimental difficulties which have to be en- countered. One subsidiary phenomenon which was studied in this connection deserves a word of description, but the main question is too complicated and too controversial to be worth following up.^ In studying Purkinje's phenomenon, we found that if the intensity of a spectrum were diminished a point was reached at which the brightest part appeared to have been shifted towards the violet. What, it may be asked, happens if the illumination be still further diminished ? Under normal circumstances we soon reach a point at which the whole spectrum appears colourless, differing however in brightness in the various parts. Keduction beyond this yields an intensity which is associated with no sensation at all. Hence it seemed necessary to distinguish between the absolute liminal intensity of a spectrum, that is, the least intensity corresponding to a colourless sensation, and the " specific " threshold value for which the spectral zones could be seen to differ in hue. In determining these absolute and specific thresholds, great divergences were found between the results of different workers. The observations of Burch go far to prove that the existence of a specific threshold, at any rate in the case of foveal or direct vision, depends upon the after-effects of previous stimulation. This physiologist first performed some qualitative experiments which are most suggestive. A Bunsen burner was completely covered by a metal chimney, so as to prevent any escape of light while not interfering with ventilation. By bringing the flame into contact with the metal chimney the latter could be heated gradually to a point at which it became luminous. If the experiment were performed in a room with windows covered by ordinary blinds — that is to say, in a room from which light had not been absolutely excluded — thefirst appearance of light ' Further information will be found in the article by the present writer contributed to Further Advances in Physiology, London, 1909 (Arnold), pp. 354, etc. VISUAL ADAPTATION 105 Adaptation Values {Burch). During the period of increasing adaptation. Time in Dark. (Mins.) Intensity of Minimum Visible Bed. Intensity of Minimum Visible Blue- Violet. Eatio, V:E.. 9 21 60 16-63 11-14 2-4 254-76 57-61 6-39 15-32 5-17 2-24 B. After spending two hours in the dark room. Time in Dark. (Mins.) Intensity of Minimum Visible Ked. Intensity of Minimum Visible Blue-Violet. Eatio, V : E. 120 122 125 127 130 1-0 1-20 2-69 5-04 60-02 1-0 1-63 6-14 12-44 225-72 1-0 1-34 1-91 2-47 4-51 c V QE Fig. 11. — Buroh's apparatus for the study of " achromatic thresholds." A. An electric lamp, 16 candle-power. B. Paper reflector. CC. Wall of dark room. D. Stand with polarismg prism (E). F. Spectroscope with double image prism (ff) over eyepiece. There is a stop with two slits (allowing red and blue-violet light to pass through) in the eye-piece. By rotation of the polarising prism the relative intensities of the red and blue-violec lights can be varied. A. Relative Stimulus Values of different Spectral Regions (Parinaud). Fraunhofer Lines. Adapted Retina. TJnadapted Retina. B OTU tJtj lio ToD D T^ tAt E 1 tJu F 1 tJc G iJo T.^ H ^hs ? B. Increased Responsiveness of Peripheral Retina {Dark-adapted). Stimulus : A bluish-white object, 35 degrees in diameter (v. Kries, p. 171).' Eesponsivenesa (Arbitrary Scale). Temporal Eccentricity in Degrees. Nasal Eccentricity in Degrees. Insensitive Zone. 1-0 1-07 0-85 1-92 1-78 1-22 1-06 2-28 7-12 1-70 1-38 3-08 16-02 2-3 1-92 4-22 28-48 3-0 2-58 5-58 44-50 3-75 3-33 7-08 64-08 4-04 4-04 8-08 1 Distances from the fovea centralis of any point on the retina can be conveniently measured in terms of the angle subtended at the nodal point by a segment of the retina out by a plane passing through the fovea, nodal point, FiQ. 12. and position of the object, and bounded by a straight line through the nodal point from the object intersecting the retinal segment and by the principal axis. E.g. if A be an object, B the nodal point, and CO a section of the retina, the position (eccentricity) of the image of the point A is defined by the angle a. VISUAL ADAPTATION 107 was a pearl-grey tint, the achromatic threshold which others had noticed. But when the room was changed to one without windows and absolutely dark, the first appearance of luminosity was not grey, but dull red. On repeating this experiment, after spending a few minutes in a lighted room, the former grey appearance was once more obtained. Quan- titative experiments were then performed with the apparatus shown in the diagram on page 105. Burch was also able to show that a form of after-images (his " dazzle tints ") are very persistent, and may endure as long as two and a half hours after exposing the eye to light, so that his contention can almost be regarded as established in the case of direct vision. That an absolute as distinct from a specific threshold exists in the case of peripheral vision seems, on the whole, probable in view of the careful work of Armin Tschermak, who used very prolonged dark adaptation ; but Burch's work is quite sufficient to show how cautious one must be in interpreting results obtained by these methods, and how complex they are. Direct experiment, therefore, seems to have established the following points : — (1) The peripheral regions of the retina are relatively more sensitive than the fovea to light of moderate or short wave length. (2) Adaptation to darkness is characterised by an increase in responsiveness to short-waved light, and this change is mainly, if not entirely, extra-foveal. The tables illustrate these statements. The intensity values are arbitrary, the measurement of eccentricity is in terms of the angle subtended at the nodal point of the eye. We now come to indirect evidence tending in the same direction ; of this the facts relating to total colour-blindness are the most striking. Total colour-blindness is almost always a congenital defect, and is characterised, apparently, by a complete absence of colour perception in the ordinary sense; it differs toto cielo from the condition of partial colour-blindness, which will be described in a later chapter. A person in this state may see a spectrum merely as a grey strip unequally bright in the 108 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES diiFerent parts which seem to us of distinct hues. To de- scribe the sensations of a second human being must be impossible, but perhaps we may say, for the sake of com- parison, and without pretending to real exactness, that a totally colour-blind man receives from a coloured print im- pressions similar to those excited in ourselves by an un- coloured one. A summary of the observations of Hering on a typical case will give the reader a better idea of the facts than any catalogue of signs and symptoms. Hering s Case of Total Colour-hlindness. Equally Bright Circle for Colour-blind. White Valency of a Coloured Circle. White Valency.' Grey Circle matching the Coloured Circle for White. Black. a Normal "Light" Eye. Degrees. Degrees. Degrees. Degrees. Bluish-red . 13-0 347-0 18-8 40 Yellowish-red 5-5 354-5 11-4 46 Orange 37-0 323-0 43-4 159 Yellow 136-5 223-5 140-2 283 Arsenic-green 228'0 132-0 230-5 £05 Green . 152-0 208-0 155-5 137 Greenish-hlue 109-5 , 250-5 113-7 89 Ultramarin e-blue 88-3 271-7 92-8 34 Violet . 47-5 312-5 52-7 32 The subject was a man of twenty years, whose colour vision had always been abnormal. He said that he could read without difficulty, provided the light were not too intense, but that his eyes were readily fatigued by bright illumination. In twilight his vision was especially good, particularly if the light were very feeble. On examination, the following results were obtained. No objective changes were detected with the ophthalmoscope, nor was any part of the retina insensitive ; there was no totally blind area (or scotoma). His power of distinguishing two spots unequally bright — physically — was much below that of a normal person, in ' For methods of measuring white "valency" consult Hering, pp. 567, etc. For the present purpose the figures in the third column of the table may be regarded as a recalculation of the amounts of white in the sectors which match the coloured circles, so as to admit of comparison with the figures of the fourth column. VISUAL ADAPTATION 109 bright light ; in a dark room, it was much superior. With a spectrum, it was found that the area of red which produced any sensation was much diminished; there was shortening of the red end, and those parts which were effective seemed less bright than to a normal eye. The violet end, on the other hand, was not shortened, and it seemed relatively brighter than to the normal " light '' eye, while the region of maximal brightness was in thp neighbourhood of Fraunhofer's E and C lines. Brightness matches between coloured sectors and mixtures of black and white gave the results indicated in the table, which contains comparative values for the nor- mal "light" eye. It is to be noted that the colour-blind's matches were valid, i.e. good matches, for a normal "dark" eye. Tests were also carried out polariscopically. At one end of a horizontal tube, blackened on the inside, a cork plate was fixed ; the plate was perforated in the middle and a doubly refracting prism inserted. The other end of the tube was closed by a lid, in which two equal and symmetrically placed semicircular openings were made. With this con- trivance the ordinary and extraordinary images obtained by polarisation appeared to form a series of circles when the tube was directed to a source of light, e.g. a piece of baryta paper stretched over a glass plate. Between the eye and the prism a small telescope and a Nicol prism were introduced, together with a graduated arc. The diaphragm of the tele- scope removed the lateral images, leaving only two magnified white circles, the halves of which could have their brightness altered in opposite directions by rotating the Nicol prism. In front of one opening in the tube a coloured glass was placed, and the Nicol so arranged that for the normal " dark " eye or for the totally colour-blind eye, both halves appeared equally bright. The next table gives the readings for two observers. Some of the variations may be explained by the fact that the normal-sighted person was unpractised in this sort of work. Precisely similar results were obtained in matching spectral colours. The analogy between normal vision under conditions of 110 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES Polariseopic Matches (Hering). Yellow Glass. Blue Glass. Total Colour-blind. Normal Dark-adapted Eye. Total Colour-blind. Normal Dark-adapted Eye. D egrees of Eotatio n. Degrees of Eotation. Degrees of Rotation. Degrees of Rotation. 22-3 22-9 18-2 18-35 22-6 22-6 18-0 17-95 22-3 23-0 180 18-15 21-9 22-5 18-1 18-4 22-3 22-7 17-8 18-9 21-8 231 221 21-8 dark adaptation and the vision of tlie totally colour-blind will also be apparent from the next two tables. The first gives the intensity values of the different parts of the spectrum for a normal "dark" eye, and the second Abney's observations on two other cases of total colour-blindness. The units of inten- sity in these tables are not comparable, but it will be observed that the maxima occur in the same part of the spectrum. " TicilvjU " Values of a Spectrum (Schaternikoff). Wave length. Intensity Value. Wave Length. Intensity Value. In milllonths of a In millionths of a mm. mm. 670-8 18-0 529-3 2736-0 651-8 36-5 522-3 2532-3 634-3 83 3 515-4 2219-3 618-1 216-9 508-7 1944-0 603-1 423-2 502-2 1475-8 589-3 881-7 490-0 1016-0 577-1 1424-9 478-6 6330 566-4 2110-7 468-0 364-5 556-0 2609-7 458-7 208-8 546-0 2899-0 451-1 111-2 537-2 3000-0 443-9 69-6 Since the direct evidence which we studied first suggested that adaptation was an affair of the extra foveal part of the retina, one might expect the fovea centralis in the totally colour-blind to be relatively insensitive. As a matter of fact, in seven out of eighteen cases which have been investigated, an absolute or relative central or para-central scotoma was VISUAL ADAPTATION 111 observed, and in most cases very imperfect fixation or even nystagmus was noticed. It may, however, be regarded as certain that the existence of such an insensitive area is not necessarily found in typical total colour-blindness ; as we shall see later, the point has been laboured chiefly for theoretical reasons. Luminosity Values of tivo Cases of Total Colour-blindness (Ahney). (No. 40 in Abney's scale is close to the B line.) Scale of Spectrum. K. B.'B Luminosity Value. P.'s Luminosity Value. 56 2-5 54 9-0 52 160 7-6 50 27-5 19-0 48 42-5 39 46 61-0 65-0 44 82-5 85-0 42 96-0 98-0 40 100-0 99-0 38 95-5 915 36 87-5 90-0 34 75-0 80-0 32 61-5 65-0 30 43-0 50-0 28 37-0 36-0 26 30-0 26-5 24 24-0 19-5 22 18-5 14-0 20 14-5 100 18 11-5 ... 16 9-0 5 '5 14 7-0 ... 12 5-0 In general terms, we may say that the analogy betAveen normal vision under conditions of dark adaptation and the vision of the totally colour-blind is quite close. In the next chapter some additional evidence will be considered, and the theory of the matter discussed. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY Sufficient indications to enable the reader to consult original sources of information will be found in the article Visual Adaptation, by M. Greenwood, Further Advances in Physiology, London, 1909 (Arnold), pp. 351-377. Of the literature there referred to, the memoir by Tschermak is the most exhaustive. CHAPTER XIII RECURRENT VISION-THEORIES OF ADAPTATION A SET of experiments of apparently little importance has thrown considerable light on the question of adaptation. These deal with the effects which follow the application of luminous stimuli for very short intervals of time. Such experiments can be carried out in at least two ways. By a contrivance similar to the shutter of a camera the eye can be stimulated for a very short time, or, the gaze being fixed, a source of light may be moved across the field of vision. In the latter method, a disc with a slit in it can be rotated in front of a lantern. If the length of the slit is I, and V is the velocity of movement, l/v measures the time during which each retinal element is exposed to the light, and we can make this time as short as we please. There is of course no difference in principle between the two methods, but the second is rather easier to employ, and the results obtained with it are of special interest. If a bright object is rotated in this manner on a screen with a slit in front of a light, the whole sensory effect com- prises the following phases : — (1) A primary image ; the immediate consequence of the stimulus, also its strongest effect. As compared with the image due to a stationary illuminant, it is more or less elon- gated into a streak of light. (2) Immediately following upon the primary image is a short, dark streak. (3) After the dark streak comes a second period of illu- mination which, if the stimulus be coloured, appears tinged with the complementary. In a successful experiment this effect may be so pronounced that it seems as if a second object were following in the track of the first, so that it has. 112 RECURRENT VISION 113 been termed the "satellite" or "ghost," and the whole phenomenon is described as that of "recurrent vision." (4) The end of the satellite is not sharply defined, and is followed by another interval of darkness. (5) The field once more brightens, but somewhat faintly, and an image of the same colour as the primary one appears. (6) Lastly, another dark interval is obtained. It would hardly be expected that there should be com- plete accord in the description of so complicated an effect as witnessed by different observers. Hess and v. Kries, to mention two highly trained and experienced sense physio- logists, differ materially in their accounts as to what can and what cannot be seen. The work of v. Kries has received, in all essential points, confirmatioijj^from the experiments of Macdougall, and I shall follow Jiis description as closely as possible. With respect to the experiment as a whole, we have three phases of illumination — the primary image, the satellite image, and the tertiary. In apparent brightness these are ranged in the order of their appearance. When the original stimulus is of low intensity, no tertiary image is obtainable ; with a still less intensity, the satellite also disappears. The lengths of the images can also be made to vary, and the dark intervals to vanish. Fixing our attention on the primary, the following characters have been found. The image is sometimes striped, but apart from this is uniform when viewed under conditions of light adaptation. As dark adaptation proceeds the image not only increases in ex- tension and brightness, but with chromatic stimuli ceases to be uniform. Thus with blue light, only the anterior border is deep blue, and is followed by a whitish stripe. Macdougall found that the white part begins at a distance corresponding in his experiments to a time interval of ^ second. With other colours, except red, the same result is obtained, but less distinctly. The satellite image begins J-i second after the com- mencement of the primary, and is generally complementary to it. This rule must, however, be modified in the following way. If the primary is pure white, the secondary is bluish ; H 114 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES in fact, the secondary is always modified in tlie direction of bluishness. Even -with a feeble blue primary, the secondary may still have a faint bluish tinge. As regards brightness, the result depends on the adaptive value of the stimulus, i.e. two lights of equal stimulus values for the " dark " eye give equally bright secondary images. Red, with its relatively low stimulus value for the " dark " eye, only gives a secondary when its physical brightness is great. As we should expect from its character, the satellite is largely dependent on the adaptation of the eye, increasing to a maximum as dark adaptation proceeds, and then diminish- ing, although it persists after prolonged dark adaptation (Macdougall). We associate, therefore, with the satellite the ordinary characters of peripheral vision, (1) relatively greater efficiency of the shorter waved lights, (2) increased intensity on dark adaptation ; it only remains to add failure on foveal stimulation. With respect to this failure, a somewhat heated discussion took place between v. Kries and Hess, the latter asserting that a distinct satellite could be obtained in central vision. V. Kries has, however, indicated certain possible fal- lacies in Hess' technique, and his assertion that no secondary can be produced at the fovea centralis has been confirmed by the work of Hamaker and of Macdougall. Passing to the tertiary image, the following characters may be noticed. The hue is best appreciated when red is chosen as the stimulus ; in such a case, the tertiary may be very distinct. With in- creasing dark adaptation, the tertiary gains in brightness but loses in chromatic value ; owing to the high adaptive values of green and blue when these lights are used at moderate in- tensities, the coloration of the tertiary image can only be seen at the beginning of the experiment. There is some difference of opinion as to whether this tertiary image can be seen in direct vision ; since red light is the most suitable stimulus for calling it up, it should be so perceived. Perhaps there are two factors in the production of the tertiary — a chromatic element unaffected by dark adaptation, and a brightness element which is so affected. The last group of facts to which I desire to refer in this connection are those which have to do with observations KECURRENT VISION 115 upon the pupillary reaction. Schirmer asserted that the pupil width and its reaction were related to the adaptive condition of the eye. With complete adaptation to a given grade of light, the pupil reaches after an initial widening or narrowing a physiological mean position. Garten found that momentary illumination produced in "light" eyes a weak sudden, in the " dark " eye a slow powerful contraction. Pro- ceeding further on these lines, Sachs discovered that the pupillo-motor response to coloured lights followed closely their adaptation values. Abelsdorff confirmed these results, using the apparatus sketched. Triplea: Zamp. ,SUt_ in Telescope. -Sqiccratbw Prismi. - CyHndior of Blacked- Paper 56 c.th. long. ■QmvesD L ens. Focal Length 10 c.tw. ^^ UncuxommodateA Eye. Fig. 13. — AbelsdorfE's apparatus for studying the pupillo-motor response. One of the two lights serves as a standard ; the subject, looking through the strong convex lens at the slit, sees a bright point surrounded by a diffusion circle. If the light falling on the eye be changed, the diffusion circle increases or diminishes in size. By rotation of a Nicol prism the new intensity can be increased or diminished, and the intensity is found for which the diffusion circle produced by the standard 116 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES light is not increased or diminished by changing to the tested colour. This method is more accurate, in Abelsdorff's opinion, than one might suppose, the mean experimental error being about 7 per cent. The next table gives some of Abelsdorff's results : — Pupillo-Motor Values. Wave Length. MM. Comparison Lighta. 600 ix^. Light Adaptation. 480 MM- Darlf Adaptation. 480 (ifi. 640 620 600 580 560 540 520 500 Mm. •5271 •8523 •9720 •9536 ■8303 •5518 •3333 •1181 Mm. •3920 •8376 •9822 •9090 •8739 •6141 •2936 •09141 Mm. •2666 •5670 •7260 •8065 •8865 •9200 •5755 •1612 Brightness Values. (Readings obtained when the same subject adjusted the intensities of the lights until they seemed to be equally bright.) Wave Length. Comparison Lights. Iili.. 600 ja/i. Light Adaptation. DarlJ Adaptation. 480 ^fi. 480 II.H.. Mm. Mm. Mm. 640 •5253 ■3518 •2529 620 •8204 •7230 •5515 600 •9431 •9090 •8536 580 •9431 •9090 •8535 560 •8811 •8613 •9540 • 540 •6259 •6354 •9540 520 •3700 •3189 •5750 500 •1168 •0944 •1612 The close agreement between the pupillo-motor values and those of apparent brightness justifies the method, and its importance lies in the fact that we can employ it in experi- EECURRENT VISION 117 ments on animals. We have no direct means of investiga- ting adaptive changes in any animal except man, but we can measure this pupillary response ; if we find it changing in the manner described, it is suggested (not, of course, demonstrated) that in such animals the visual responsiveness may be similarly affected. It has been found that the intensities of red and blue, which appeared equally bright to, and exerted the same pupillo-motor effect upon, a human " light " eye, did not produce identical changes in the pupils of the dove and the owl. For the former the red, for the latter the blue, was the stronger stimulus. Indeed, the pupillo-motor response to blue in the owl's eye was greater than in the case of a total-colour-blind (Abelsdorff). We can now appropriately examine the theories which have been propounded to describe these different classes of fact. In the first place, can we justly say that there is any functional difference between the spot of distinctest vision, the fovea centralis retinae, and the paracentral or peripheral regions of the retina in respect of colour vision ? In view of the long series of experiments bearing upon the Purkinje effect, the increase in brightness of the short-waved spectral lights at the expense of the long- waved vibrations, it seems clear that this effect is mainly if not entirely peripheral. Even those who claim to have observed foveal adaptation admit it to be much less marked than peripheral changes of responsiveness. We have also seen that these adaptive changes consist in a greatly increased responsive- ness to light of short wave length, such appearing more intense than under " light " conditions. We have to account theoretically for a localised change in responsiveness with respect to certain forms of stimulus. The difference in histological structure between the fovea centralis and the surrounding area caused Schultze, many years ago, to suggest a functional separation, a conclusion which he supported by evidence drawn from the study of comparative anatomy. When Schultze wrote, however, our knowledge of adaptive changes was little advanced, and his conception went unheeded. , The modern development of Schultze's hypothesis is due to the independent researches of 118 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES H. Parinaud and J. v. Kries, both of whom, together with their colleagues and pupils, have published numerous memoirs on the subject. The actual priority appears to belong to Parinaud, whose first paper was published in 1881. Essentially the theories of Parinaud and v. Kries are exceedingly simple. Two distinct visual mechanisms exist : of these, one is concerned in the elaboration of chromatic and achromatic stimuli, and is represented in the retina by the cones; the other mechanism deals with achromatic responses only, and is represented by the rods and visual purple. The former mechanism is the only one which can act in bright light, and its responsiveness is little, if at all, increased by resting in darkness ; the latter is brought into play when the eyp has been shielded from stimulation, being the sole or chief agency for twilight vision ; it is character- ised by special responsiveness to ethereal vibrations of short wave length. In view of the double nature of the mechanisms postulated, the theory has been named the " Duplicity Theory " (" Dtlplizitatstheorie "). Let us see how far the hypothesis covers the experimental observations I have enumerated. If the theory were true, we should expect (1) spectral maximal brightness to change in favour of the violet end when the physical intensity of the light is diminished ; (2) this change ought not, however, to occur in images formed at the fovea centralis retinse ; (3) no achromatic threshold ought to be obtained for any light at the fovea centralis or for red light anywhere. Each of these deductions has been shown to receive support from experiment and observation. Another way of testing the theory is to see whether we have any forms of vision in which, apparently, the basal mechanism is similar to that associated by the theory with twilight vision, and uncomplicated by any other type of vision. The subjects of total-colour-blindness appear to afford us a case in point. We found that the brightness judgment of these people agree well with those of normal men in a state of dark adaptation; that there is evidence in such cases of diminished or absent foveal responsiveness, bad fixation, inferior acuteness of vision, nystagmus and KECURRENT VISION 119 abnormally good vision in twilight. This might well be regarded as a case in which the twilight mechanism alone is operative. Conversely, one pathological condition is consistent with the activity of the hypothetical daylight mechanism existing by itself. This is the condition of "night-blindness," or hemeralopia, as it is badly called, which has been investigated by Parinaud, Nettleship, Messmer, and others. According to Parinaud, subjects of this disease have vision of the foveal type ; their colour sense is normal, but the spectrum is shortened at the violet end, and responsive- ness to feeble stimuli is abnormally poor. It is the latter condition which incapacitates a person who exhibits the peculiarity to a marked degree from working in twilight or poor artificial light. The investigations of Messmer have made it seem very improbable that night-blindness is a simple condition. In some cases, dark adaptation is very slowly induced, but after a sufficiently long time attains a normal degree of intensity. In other cases, a certain degree of adaptation is produced in the normal time, but the amount is far less than in ordinary persons. Owing to the complexity of the condition, we must admit that the light it throws on our problem is not so great as we could wish. Provisionally, we may perhaps say that some cases of night- blindness can be interpreted by supposing that the hypo- thetical daylight mechanism is alone functioning, while most of the examples of total-colour-blindness agree with the supposition that the twilight mechanism is chiefly at work. It must be pointed out that there cannot possibly be a com- plete identity between the normal peripheral mechanism and the visual system of a total colour-blind. Poor as is the latter's visual acuity, it is far superior to that of the peripheral parts of the normal retina. The complex results in sensation which are due to short or moving stimuli have perhaps confused some readers ; let us see whether our hypothesis is capable of arranging them in an orderly manner. The peculiar striping of the primary image has already been mentioned, how blue tails ofi' into white ; this suggests the interplay of two processes. I also emphasised the dependence of the secondary image on 120 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES adaptation, and its probable absence at the fovea centralis. We can perhaps sum up the effects in terms of our hypo- thesis thus : — The cone mechanism responds by two effects, the main part of the primary and the colour component in the tertiary. The rod apparatus responds in a threefold manner; it gives us the white tail of the primary, the whole of the secondary, and contributes, although slightly, to increasing the brightness of the tertiary. This description clears up some difficulties, but raises others. If we are to regard the twilight mechanism as solely responsible for the secondary image, it is clear that we derive sensations of colour as well as sensations of luminosity without hue from that mechanism, since the secondary image is often coloured. The mechanism cannot therefore be identical with that of a totally colour-blind eye ; we must either give up the view that total colour-blindness is a condition in which the rods and purple react as in a normal person, or regard the secondary as due to something beyond rod stimulation. The latter alternative is the one v. Kries is disposed to adopt, but we have then the difficulty of understanding why the secondary is entirely peripheral. Are we to suppose that there is a functional difference between the central aad peripheral cones ? If not, why in this case can the cones only respond if the rods are at the same time stimulated ? Probably this will be found the most serious difficulty in the way of a complete acceptance of the duplicity hypo- thesis. It is hoped, although not expected, that further experimental work will clear the matter up. A certain amount of confirmatory evidence has been brought forward from the side of comparative physiology. It has long been known that the relative numbers of rods and cones differ in various animals. Thus, the rods are very large and almost exclusively present in the retinae of nocturnal animals, such as owls, bats, and hedgehogs. In many other creatures, on the other hand, including most birds, cones predominate. It was, indeed, on the strength of this that Schultze advanced a theory essentially similar to that of Parinaud and v. Kries. Ktlhne subsequently RECURRENT VISION 121 showed that visual purple was present only in retinae con- taining rods, although he was not able to extract the pigment from all rod-containing eyes, an exception being the bat. Trendelenburg has recently extracted visual purple from the retinae of more than one species of bat. Experimentally, as we have seen, Abelsdorff found that, judging by the pupillo- motor response, the owl is specially sensitive to short-waved light and the dove insensitive. We all know that most nocturnal animals can see badly in broad daylight, while such birds as the pigeon exhibit a marked degree of night-blindness. In Parinaud's words : " It is a matter of common observation that hens and pigeons see very imperfectly in artificial light, and defend themselves with difficulty against the hand that tries to seize them ; that as soon as the sun goes down these animals seek their night shelter, the old adage, ' To go to bed with the hens,' meaning to go to bed early, evidently having its origin in this fact." ^ Biological investigation appears to show, therefore, a <;o-existence of rods and visual purple with vision of the twilight type and of cones with optimal vision in daylight. Reasoning from analogy is, however, proverbially dangerous, and the importance of the facts described can easily be ■exaggerated. We have, and can have, no direct knowledge of the actual type of vision possessed by the animals men- tioned; in some, notably the bat, it is a matter of doubt how far vision is the true directive sense under twilight conditions. The early roosting of diurnal birds may also be due to causes other than a state of night-blindness. The whole case can be summed up in the following way : — (1) There is a marked difference between central and peripheral vision in regard to the phenomenon of darkness adaptation, the former being little, if at all, affected in the process. (2) These differences may be provisionally interpreted on the hypothesis that visual sensations are bound up with two distinct mechanisms : (a) That of the cones, with which ' La Vision, by H. Parinaud, Paris, 1898, p. 66. 122 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES chromatic responsiveness and colourless sensori-reactions in bright light are associated ; (6) that of the rods, upon which depend achromatic reactions under conditions of darkness adaptation. The objections to this view are neither few nor unim- portant. It has not been proved that no central adaptation occurs. The equations (colour matches) of totally colour- blind persons and those of the normal " dark " eye agree well, but not completely. The interpretation of the secondary image of recurrent vision is not complete. The first of these objections, as well as the kindred one that a central scotoma does not exist in all cases of total- colour-blindness, may be parried by supposing that a trace of visual purple and a few scattered rods are present in the fovea. Recent measurements by Fritsch give an absolutely rod-free zone of only "2 mm., corresponding to an angular distance of less than a degree. We must not attach much weight to failures in the demonstration of such small non- adaptable areas, even supposing that they are completely rod-free. The difficulty regarding " total-colour-blindness " and normal "dark" equations is not formidable. Sufficient measurements have not been made to enable us to affirm that the differences are significant. The most serious diffi- culty as to the secondary image of recurrent vision has already been discussed; it may prove the crucial point of the theory. If one adopts the above view of the role of the visual purple and rods as elements in the physiological processes of vision with low intensities of light, one is tempted to speculate as to the nature of their activity. We have, however, only negative evidence. We can say with some confidence that retinal fluorescence is not an important factor in the process ; beyond this we cannot at present go. In conclusion, the reader must clearly understand that the cones of the peripheral retinas are not functionless, that peripheral vision in daylight is quite different from the type RECURRENT VISION 123 studied in this chapter. The following table shows this very distinctly : — Na line = 100. Wavelength. . 680 651 629 608 589 573 558 530 513 Peripheral value daylight . . 9-6 37-5 77'5 101 100 79-6 52-2 28o 14-6 Peripheral value twilight . . ? 3-4 14-0 35-5 100 256 351 321 198 KEOOMMBNDED FOR FURTHER STUDY The reader should consult the bibliography on pp. 376-7 of " Further Advances in Physiology." Of the papers mentioned, the most important are — Die Helldunkeladaptation des Auges und die Funktion der Stabchen und Zapfen, by A. von Tschermah, Ergebn. d. Physiol., 1st Jahrg. pt. h. pp. 695, etc. /. von Kries, Die Gesichtsempfindungen, Nagel's Handb., vol, iii. pp. 109-282. On Recurrent Vision, Macdoiigall, British Journ. of Psychol., i. 78 ; V. Kries, Z.P.P.S.O., xxix. 81 ; Hess, ibid., xxvii. 1. CHAPTEE XIV TRICHROMATIC VISION Up to the present we have considered visual sensations from the standpoint of pure comparison. Without analysing either the sensations themselves or their physical forerunners, we have been content to ascertain whether a physical agent of approximate constancy is or is not followed by the same phenomenon in consciousness irrespectively of the exact region stimulated or the general experimental conditions (light or dark adaptation). We have found that the end product does vary with the part of the retina examined and the latter's adaptive state. We now enter upon a wider and more difficult part of our inquiry, namely, a presentation and arrangement of the chief physiological facts of visual sensation. The difficulty of this task is apparent when one considers what is involved. We must attempt to describe the interplay of a complex of factors, physical, physiological, and psychological ; some of these are fairly well understood, others hardly at all, so that we can frequently do little more than echo the words of a great master of the subject — " The confession of actual doubt is better than the delusion of dogmatic certainty." ^ In the first place, are visual sensations solely dependent upon their physical forerunners ? Under like physiological conditions and with equal physical stimuli are the responses identical ? Reflection makes it clear that the answer is — no ; what we term the colour of an object is not a simple percept, but a construct. The difficulty has been admirably put by Hering, whose remarks deserve close attention. He writes : "The layman is convinced that external objects possess definite colours, that snow is white, soot black, and gold yellow. He attributes to these colours an existence inde- 1 Helmholtz, Eandh. d. Pliys. Opt., p. 379. 124 TRICHROMATIC VISION 125 pendent of the eye, and characterises them as the real {wirldiche) colours of the respective objects, distinguishing them from the accidental {zufdllige) colours which the same objects exhibit under unusual circumstances, e.g. with insuf- ficient illumination, or illumination differing very markedly from ordinary daylight. The red of the mountain peak in the Alpine glow ; the corpse-like pallor of a face illuminated by the sodium flame ; the bright spots on the floor of a room into which the sunlight streams through bright window- panes, are instances of such occasional colours which we attribute to a corresponding peculiarity of the illumination, and do not regard as characteristics of the objects in ques- tion. One who has learned that snow owes its whiteness and gold its yellowness to the particular rays of daylight reflected by each, easily forms the opinion that the " real " colour of external objects must be black, since he regards black, erroneously, as being due to a complete absence of light rays. Yet whenever he thinks of the snow, he always pictures it as white ; and so do all of us, whether we have thought much or little about the nature of colours. Thus-the mineralogist, to whom snow is a heaping together of little colourless transparent water crystals ; the chemist, for whom these crystals are composed of countless atoms and mole- cules: the physicist, looking beyond atoms and molecules to forms of energy — all perforce associate with the concep- tion of snow the white colour." ^ It amounts to this then, the colour sensations usually experienced when we are stimulated by certain objects are supposed by us to be necessarily connected with these objects. ''What the layman calls the real colour of an object is a colour of the object constantly present in his memory," and may be termed a " memory colour." The importance of such colours is obvious; they render it im- possible for us to describe visual sensations completely in terms of any physical measurements, since the sensation value of a stimulus is not solely dependent on that stimulus considered by itself. Two stimuli of equal value — physically ^ Hering, Grundziige der LeJire 1:0m Lichtsinn, Leipzig, 1905, p. 6. 126 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES speaking — will not necessarily elicit the same sensation even if compared under approximately the same physiological conditions. The different results which may follow the application of a constant stimulus are thought to be demon- strated by an experiment devised by Hering i (Fig. 14). If the apparatus be arranged as indicated in the diagram, with a suitably chosen artificial light, both papers appear brown, the yellow working rays reflected from the blue paper overpowering the blue rays. If the window be now shut and, without altering the artificial light, the two pieces of paper be removed from the photometer and examined, it ^^ D Fig. li.—A. Wooden Photometer Tube. B. Cylinrier with Wooden Prism having Blue and Brown (2 and 1) Papers on either side. D. Artificial Light. C. Mirror reflecting daylight. -will be seen that the "really blue" paper, although it is sending to the eye precisely the same mixture of rays as before, looks blue just as in daylight, only perhaps a little darker, while the brown paper still looks brown. In Hering's opinion, the constancy of colours in objects is one of the most striking phenomena in the field of visual sensations. Without it a piece of chalk on a dull day would have the same colour as a piece of coal on a sunny day, and, in the course of a single day, would pass through a remarkable series of gradations between white and black. The conse- quence would be that we should no more attribute whiteness to chalk or blackness to coal than hotness or coldness to iron. Interesting as are these reflections, it is permissible to ' Hering, op. eit, p. 15. TEICHROMATIC VISION 127 doubt whether the invocation of " memory colours " is neces- sary to account for the constancy of colour phenomena. It is equally reasonable to suppose that chalk never on the dullest day resembles coal, because relatively to coal it always reflects more white light. Hering's experiment ap- pears to me to involve too many factors for us to regard it as demonstrating his theses. Even, however, if we accept Hering's view, it by no means follows that we ought to dis- card experimental methods founded upon physical measure- ments. In quite popular language, the position may be stated as follows : We look at two scraps of paper, and say that in one case we experience a sensation of blueness and in the other one of redness; we then find that vibrations of one type are predominately reflected by the one and of another type by the other object. Although we must not say that this difference in vibration period is the only cause of the sensational difference, it may be convenient to use it as some criterion. This experimental convenience is some justification for saying that colour tone depends on wave , length, colour saturation on wave purity, and intensity on wave energy. I hope to show that, by adopting this con- vention, we can simplify our description of the facts without seriously prejudicing their theoretical interpretation. I shall therefore pursue the following order- — the empirical methods and results of colour-mixing and the phenomena of after- images will be described; then the theoretical deductions which have been made from these results, together with experiments specially designed to test these deductions ; and subsequently certain peculiar and special phenomena of colour vision will be considered. The physical basis of this kind of work is to be found in the conception of a simple or homogeneous light, and dates from Newton's researches on prismatic analysis. By homo- geneous light we are to understand ethereal vibrations, the wave lengths of which fall within certain narrow limits. Prisms and gratings enable us to filter such lights from a mixture, and to employ them for our experiments. A pure light is uniquely defined by its wave length, and any such light may possess any intensity. The physical unit of 128 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES intensity is not easily fixed, perhaps the best attempt being that of Krarup,^ who has applied the energy measurements of Angstrom. In any spectrum the intensities of the in- dividual lights depend on the source of illumination and the method of analysis, i.e. on the extent of surface over which light of a given wave length is spread. In an " in- terference " spectrum diffusion is uniform ; in a prismatic spectrum, on the other hand, it increases from red to violet, so that the short-waved light is relatively less intense than the long-waved. Spectra obtained by the two methods are accordingly not directly comparable. By colour or light mixing we understand an arrangement by which two or more homogeneous lights fall upon the same retinal area. That our experiments may be pure we Fig. 15. have to use pure light of constant intensity, which often necessitates the use of complex optical apparatus. Pigments cannot be used, because the light proceeding from a mixture of pigment is not equal to the sum of the lights emitted by the constituents, owing to selective absorption. A simple method, sufficient for many purposes, is the rotation of coloured sectors on a colour mixer such as is generally used for demonstrations. A more accurate method is that due to Helmholtz. Sunlight reflected from a heliostat enters a dark room by a slit, and then passes through a prism P (Fig. 15) and an achromatic lens Lj. The screen S^ is placed in the focal plane of the eyepiece, and a spectrum projected upon its anterior surface. Between lens and screen a diaphragm D is inserted, and the screen is furnished with ' H. Krarup, Physisch-opthaXmologisohe GreueproUeme, Leipzig, 1906. TRICHROMATIC VISION 129 two vertical slits which allow definite portions of the spectrum to pass through. A second achromatic lens Lg, of somewhat shorter focal length, projects an image of the diaphragm upon a second screen Sj. The aperture of the diaphragm must be so small that each of the pure lights is distributed over the whole of it. Under these circumstances the field projected on the second screen is made up of a uniform mixture of the two colours; colour tone and intensity are varied by adjusting accurately the position and breadth of the slits. Other more elaborate and exact methods can be used, but their description would be tedious. In most cases it is desirable to obtain a comparison as well as a mixed field, and this necessarily increases the difficulty of the arrange- ment. Examination of a spectrum teaches us that, apart from extremes of intensity, change in wave length goes with change in chromatic quality or colour tone, provided the alteration in colour tone exceeds a lower limit. At the ends of the spectrum, however, increase in wave length above or diminution below a certain value is not accom- panied by a corresponding sensation difference. The reason of this limitation is perhaps physiological, but its discussion must be omitted.^ Experiments carried out by one of the mixing processes just mentioned have enabled us to formulate certain general statements which were first clearly enunciated by Grassmann. These conclusions are as follows : If, in a mixture, one component be continuously varied, the appearance of the mixture will likewise vary (unequal lights mixed with equal lights produce unequal mixtures). If two lights look the same, then if each be mixed with a third light, the resultant mixtures will look equal. This can, of course, be generalised. A corollary is, that proportional increase of the intensity of each component in a mixture does not destroy a match. Passing to the actual observations, we at once note that the effect of mixing spectral extremes is the production of ^ See Krarup, op. cit., pp. 15-19. 130 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES a colour — purple — not present in the spectrum at all. It thus follows that any graphical representation of our results must take the form of a closed curve, since passing from red to violet we can either travel over the range of spectral colours or by way of purple. If we mix lights not belonging to the extreme ends of the range, our results are quite different. The simplest cases are those of mixing colours of wave length not less than 540 /t/t. For instance, a red (670 ytt/t) mixed with a yellow (580 /a/u,) gives a pure colour of in- termediate wave length. The greater the proportion of the long- waved component, the nearer will the position of the mixture approach the red end of the spectrum, and conversely. The mixing relations for this part of the spectrum are therefore quite straightforward ; but the result obtained — i.e. that two simple lights when mixed give a simple light the wave length of which is intermediate between those of its components — is only valid for a small part of the spectral range. If we mix a blue-green (510 fifj,) with a blue (460 fifi) the mixture, although resembling, perhaps closely, a pure intermediate — e.g. 490 fi/j, — does not match it perfectly. The mixed colour is paler, or, as we say, " less saturated," than the spectral one. This is still more evident when we choose our colours in such a way that the wave length of one is greater and that of the other less than 617 fifi. If one constituent is taken a little nearer the red than 560 /x/j,, and the other dimiirished in each ex- periment, then, with suitable proportions, the mixtures pass from greenish-yellow — becoming paler and paler — until we reach a combination which corresponds to a sensation of whiteness. As we tend to assign a unique position to white in our sensation-scale, it is customary to complete the mixing laws by the following statement: Any light mixture what- ever can be matched by a mixture of a definite homogeneous light (or a definite purple) and white light. That our re- sults may be as general as possible, it is well to note that there is no necessity for according a special place to white. " If we regard any homogeneous or compound light what- ever as fixed, and then mix it with the whole series of pure lights completed by purple, at the same time varying the TRICHROMATIC VISION 131 proportions in the mixture from the zero of the one to that of the other, we obtain all possible varieties of stimuli ; that is, any possible combination is matched by some member of the series." ^ It is, however, convenient in practice to separate white, and I shall continue to follow the usual classification. We have seen that our mixing experiments give us varia- tions in colour tone and variations in whiteness — that is, two variables — so that our results should be expressible graphically by some plane figure. We have also noticed that we can pass from red to violet, and then from violet again to red, without passing through the spectral range more than once, so that our graph should be a closed figure ; finally, in virtue of the fact that the position of any mixed colour depends directly upon the relative proportions of its components, we infer that the method of obtaining the position of the centre of inertia of masses is applicable. If three colours, A, B, C, none of which can be mixed from the other two, be repre- sented by three points in a plane, then, on assigning to them values in terms of any unit, the situations and quantitative values of their mixtures can be ascertained. Thus a colour mixed from a, parts of A and h parts of B will lie on the line A B at the point at which the centre of inertia of the two masses a and h (representing the proportions of the two colours in the mixture) would be situated. In order to establish the correctness of this method it is necessary to prove that, given the experimental laws of colour-mixing, this construction is valid in all possible cases, i.e. that the situa- tion of the mixed colour coincides with that of the mass centre of two equivalent masses (1) when the two constituents ■can be mixed from the three chosen colours, (2) when one can and the other cannot so be mixed, (3) when neither can so be mixed. The proof is too long for insertion in a book of this size, but it requires only an elementary knowledge of mathe- matics for its comprehension.^ We can show that the co-ordinates of the point at which the mixture must be situated according to the mixing laws are the co-ordinates of the 1 J. V. Kries, Nagel's Handl., vol. iii. p. Hi'. 2 See Helmholtz, Phyi. Optil.:, second edition, pp. 328-3cO. 132 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES mass centre of weights situated at the points where the components lie. It is clear that a diagram constructed on these principles will Tary in its actual form in accordance with our choice of units and fixed points. Thus Newton chose white as a fixed point, and arranged the simple lights at equal distances from it, so that the diagram was a circle ; we should obtain this result, except that the part of the curve passing from violet to red must be a straight line, since purple can only be mixed from violet and red, and its representation must be on the chord joining their representative points. If we recur to our experimental results, however, we shall see that the best diagram to adopt is that figured 16. From red to Greetv YMow \Vi6Ut Fig. Ifi.— Coloar Table. yellow the curve is a straight line, since we found that mixtures of colours between those limits matched pure spectral lights. We then get a sharp flexure in the part of the diagram beyond green, representing the low saturation of mixtures from this region, and finally we have the straight line through purple. Suppose now we select three colours in the spectral series, e.g. red, green, and blue, then in accordance with our previous deductions all mixtures of these are represented in the triangle R, G, B (Fig. 17) ; this includes a good deal of our com- plete diagram, but not all of it. If we choose red, green, and violet, the triangle R, C, V includes nearly the whole spectral diagram ; in other words, nearly all spectral colours can be matched by mixing three chosen lights in suitable proportions. TRICHROMATIC VISION 133 There is not indeed a complete coincidence, because of the bend in the spectral figure between green and blue, which means that mixtures of green and violet are less saturated than spectral cyan blue ; we can, however, generalise a little. If spectral greenish-blue be mixed with red in certain propor- tions, it matches a mixture of green and violet, or aGBl+/3R = 7Gr + 6V. G Fig. 17. Hence aGBl = 7Gr + eV — /3R, or we have the unmixable colour in terms of our three chosen colours. I do not, how- over, think this mode of expression desirable, since it is hardly capable of objective interpretation. It is to be remarked that colour equations, as they are termed, of the form R + Gr+V = R, seem to be justifiable ways of expressing experimental facts. Addition is uniform, the same result being always obtained when the same quantities are summed ; it is commutative, the order of operations does not affect the result; it is associative and homogeneous. These characters are found in our mixing process. Green + (mixed with) red + (mixed with) violet = (match) red + green + violet = (red + violet) + green. If we define subtraction, in terms of urithmetical quantity, as uniform, non-commutative, and non-associative, similar analogies can be observed ; but this is of little importance, since a justification of the use of the symbol of addition will suffice for our purposes. We can now consider some experimental points. Since, practically, all chromatic stimuli may be expressed in terms of three, researches are conducted in the following way : A definite spectrum — e.g. the prismatic spectrum of an Auer gas lamp and three lights — e.g. a red, a green, and a blue of arbitrary but constant intensity are selected. Each part of the 134 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES spectrum is then matched by mixing the three together until the mixed colour looks exactly like the homogeneous one. This process of spectral gauging has been applied by Koenig and Dieter ici ^ to the investigation of normal colour vision, and their results are generally regarded as correct. Experiments involving the manipulation and mixture of three pure lights are beset wiih technical difficulties, and the results are not entirely free from objection, so that I do not propose to give details, especially in view of the fact that we can, for many purposes, obtain almost as much information by another method. All I desire to emphasise is the general conclusion that the experimental facts of colour vision can be graphically represented by a plane figure, and the possible forms of stimuli reduced to terms of three independent vari- ables — that is to say, normal colour vision is trichromatic. It is important to notice that a choice of variables is, from the theoretical standpoint, arbitrary; indeed, if a table be constructed in terms of three primaries, A, B, C, a second can be deduced in terms of another three, A', B', C, because in view of what has gone before, it is plain that we can always define A' by an equation of the form A' = aA + &C + cC, and similarly for the other variables, the process merely involving a change of co-ordinate axes. Clearly, if we choose our primaries so near together that we cannot ex- perimentally reproduce all the spectral stimulus values, our table will involve negative directions, but this is of no theoretical importance. Although, as I have said, it is not necessary to give full details as to spectral gauging, I may note the results obtained with complementaries. The usual definition of comple- mentary colours — i.e. two lights which, when mixed together, give a colourless mixture — is ambiguous, depending on whether we define white subjectively or by reference to a mixture of known physical composition. Experimentally, the results obtained with and without a comparison field of known composition, i.e. by the second or the first method, do not difi'er widely. The table contains the values ascer- tained by Helmholtz (without a comparison), and those of ' Z.P.P.S.O., iv. 292. TRICHROMATIC VISION" Complementary Colours for Tioo Obitervern. 135 Observer V. KUIES. Obsbkver V. Trey. Helmholtz. Long-waved light. Short-waved Light. Long-waved Light. 656-2 Short-waved Light. Long-waved Light. Short- waved light. 656-2 492-4 485-2 656-2 492-1 626 492-2 626 484-6 607-7 489-7 612-3 489-6 612-3 483-6 585-3 485-4 599-5 487-8 599-5 481-8 573-7 482-1 587-6 484-7 587-6 478-9 567-1 464-5 579-7 478-7 586-7 478-7 564-4 461-8 577-6 473-9 577-7 473-9 563-5 From 433 575-5 469-3 572-8 469-3 downwards 572-9 464-8 570-7 464-8 571-1 460-4 569-0 460-4 571-0 452-9 568-1 452-1 570-4 440-4 566-3 440-4 670-1 429-5 566-4 429-5 D C D Frfi. 18. 136 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES V. Frey and v. Kries (with a comparison). The diagram is constructed from the second series. It will be seen that, at the red end, as the wave length changes, that of its com- plementary changes at first very slowly, then faster, until finally very great changes in wave length are associated with relatively little alterations in colour tone, while at the middle of the range the converse statement is true. It will have been noticed in the diagram that the two observers do not agree exactly in their values, and some of these personal variations are of interest. The best-known illustration is afforded by the individual differences found in matching a homogeneous yellow by a mixture of red and green ; a good match for one eye is too green for another, too red for a third. Maxwell, who first noticed this, suggested that the differences depend upon unequal quantities of yellow pigment in the macula, since yellow pigment absorbs very little long-waved light, only becoming markedly active for the yellow-green region of the spectrum, its powers diminish- ing again towards the violet. The correctness of this sugges- tion has been rendered highly probable by the researches of Hering, Sachs, v. Frey, and v. Kries. It has been shown that (1) the variations are consistent with the hypothesis that the eye contains an absorbing pigment ; (2) the macular pigment behaves as a yellow pigment from this point of view (Sachs) ; (3) the amount of macular pigment is variable ; (4) finally, a red-green mixture which matches a homogeneous yellow on direct fixation appears too green when examined para- centrally, and the central equation R + BIG = V appears dis- tinctly bluish-green in indirect vision. These differences are therefore of a physical order, and need not detain us. There are, however, in addition some cases which cannot be brought under the last category. Lord Rayleigh dis- covered that when testing the equation Homogeneous Yellow = Red + Green, certain persons required more red and others more green than the majority. These results have been confirmed by Donders, Hering, v. Kries, Koenig, and Dieterici, the type requiring an excess of green being the more frequent. These forms of trichromatic vision have been termed by v. Kries the anomalous greens and the TRICHROMATIC VISION 137 anomalous reds, and it is certain the peculiarities do not depend simply on physical diiferences. If the changes were due to differences in pigmentation, then, in matching a red- green mixed with homogeneous lights from 670 ;u./i to 570 nfx, these subjects would require throughout the same propor- tional increase in the green component of the mixture, which is not the case. Similar typical differences are observed in peripheral matches. These results prove that distinct varia- tions in trichromatic vision exist which are irrespective of differences in macular pigmentation, but perhaps no advan- tage is obtained by assigning them to separate classes. Hering in examining the colour sense of a series of persons found they could be divided roughly into two groups, one set making the brightness match. Spectral red (660 /u/u) : Spectral blue (447 (ifi) : : 1.15 : 1 ; the other for the same match requiring the discs in the ratio of 7 to 1. The spectral light judged to be pure green was not the same in the two classes, but nearer the red end in the first group. There were also typical differences in the composition of colourless red and bluish-green or greenish-yellow and violet mixtures ; in each case the first group required relatively large quan- tities of the short-waved component. The first group were said to be relatively yellow sighted, the second relatively blue sighted, in allusion to their special responsiveness to these colours ; v. Kries' red anomaly would be an extreme case of yellow sightedness, his green anomaly a marked example of blue sightedness. It is therefore not improbable that these abnormal trichromatics are extreme variants of a frequency system representing the whole range of visual types. The matter can only be settled when the quantitative mixing- ratios for a definite match have been determined on a large number of persons taken at random ; we may then find that the results are in accordance with some well-known frequency distribution, the normal equation representing a modal value. At present, the theoretical interpretation of these cases is extremely difficult. Summing up the results obtained in this chapter, we see : — (1) A complete sensational analysis of vision cannot be effected in terms of stimuli. 138 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES (2) Fixing our attention on stimuli only, mixing results are functions of two variables, and graphically expressible by means of a plane figure. (3) Colour differences can be expressed in terms of three chosen stimuli, the choice being theoretically restricted, but in practice certain definite stimuli are conveniently chosen. (4) Of the discrepancies observed between the matches of certain individuals, some depend on peculiarities of physical structure ; others are to be provisionally regarded as extreme variants from a modal type of vision. I hope to have made it clear that these statements are essentially descriptive, designed to resume experimental facts as simply as possible. Colour diagrams and the asser- tion that normal colour vision is (roughly) trichromatic are short ways of expressing experimental results, and contain no hidden theoretical meaning ; their truth, or falsehood, is a matter of observation. As we shall see later, the results have been the starting point of various hypotheses regarding the nature of visual processes, but the success, or failure, of these attempts is quite another story. In the next chapter we shall treat in the same objective fashion of those examples of partial colour-blindness which are physiologically interesting, and shall find that, just as normal vision may be called trichromatic, these types are dichromatic. EECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY Hermann v. Helmholtz, Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik., second edition, 1896, pp. 311-341. (The classical work, one of the greatest books of any age, contains full bibliography up to 1895.) Ewald Hering, op. cit. (Most suggestive and readable.) /. V. Kries, Nagel's Handb., vol. iii. 109-127. Sachs On Macular Pigment, Pfliig. Arch., vol. 1. p. 574. /. V. Kries, Beitr. z. Phys. d. Gesichtempf., Du Bois R. Arch. f. Phys., 1878, p. 503. Lord Eayleigh, Experiments on Colour, Nature, 1881, p. 264. Bonders, Du Bois R. Arch. f. Phys., 1884, p. 518. J. V. Kries, Z.P.P.S.O., vol. xiii. p. 241, and vol. xix. p. 63. IF. Nagel, Arch. f. Augenheilkunde, vol. xxxviii. p. 31. CHAPTEE XV DICHROMATIC VISION We have seen that, from the purely experimental point of view, the characteristics of normal colour vision admit of relatively simple arrangement, that, in fact, all the results of stimulation can be expressed in terms of three different stimuli; we must next consider some types of vision which, if themselves abnormal, throw light on the normal mechanism. The existence of abnormal visual systems is known to have been recognised for more than two centuries,^ but John Dalton, the great chemist, was the first to attract much attention to the subject.^ Goethe in his Farbenlehre, which appeared in 1812, gave the following description : — "We will here first advert to a very remarkable state in which the vision of many persons is found to be. As it presents a deviation from the ordinary mode of seeing colours, it may fairly be classed under morbid impressions ; but as it is consistent with itself, as it often occurs, may extend to several members of a family, and probably does not admit of cure, we may consider it as bordering only on the nosological cases, and therefore place it first. I was acquainted with two individuals not more than twenty years of age who were thus affected. . . . They agreed with the rest of the world in denominating white, black, and grey in the usual manner. . . . They appeared to see yellow, red- yellow, and yellow-red like others. . . . But now a striking difference presented itself. If the carmine was pressed thinly over the white saucer, they would compare the light colour 1 Turberville, Phil. Trans., 1684: Huddart, Phil. Trans., 1777; Whison, Phil. Trans., 1778. * J. Dalton, Literary and Philosophical Society of Manchester, 1794. (Cited in Koenig's bibliography.) 140 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES thus produced to the colour of the sky, and call it blue. If a rose were shown them beside it, they would in like manner call it blue ; and in all the trials that were made, it appeared that they could not distinguish light blue from rose colour. They confounded rose colour, blue, and violet on all occa- sions: these colours only appeared to them to be dis- tinguished from each other by delicate shades of lighter, darker, intenser, or fainter appearance. Again, they could not distinguish green from dark orange, nor, more especially, from a red brown. "If any one, accidentally conversing with these indi- viduals, happened to question them about surrounding ob- jects, their answers occasioned the greatest perplexity, and the interrogator began to fancy his own wits were out of order. With some method we may, however, approach to a nearer knowledge of the law of this deviation from the general law. " These persons, as may be gathered from what has been stated, saw fewer colours than other people; hence arose the confusion of different colours. . . ." ^ Since the time of Goethe a great deal of attention has been devoted to the subject, and its literature has attained formidable proportions. The most obvious distinction between the vision of the partially colour-blind and our own is an inability to perceive differences which are plain to us. As Goethe says, such persons see fewer colours than we do. This difficulty is of special interest, because it is something definite ; it is comparatively easy to find out whether a person can dis- tinguish between the effects produced on him by two stimuli which certainly affect us differently, while the actual physio- logical or psychological nature of the effect may remain obscure. How a certain green light affects a colour-blind eyoi by what sensation it is followed, we can only guess, but we know that the effect is indistinguishable from that pro- duced by a certain intensity of red light. We have, then, to deal with a condition in which the conscious responses ' Goethe's Theory of Colours, translated by Eastlake, pp. 45-7. London, 1840 (Murray). DICHROMATIC VISION 141 to varied physical stimuli are fewer than in normal persons. The question is, whether the experimental results can be summed up in the simple manner that was possible in the case of normal vision. It will be found that, with certain limitations, the results can be so described. We learned that for most experimental purposes normal colour is expressible in terms of three colours. The vision Proportions of Standard Red and Blue in a Mixture of apparently the same Tint as various Spectral Colours in four cases of Partial Colour-Blindness (v. Kries). Spectral Wave Lengths in millionths of a Deuteranope (1). Deuteranope (2), Protanope (1). Protanope (2). millimetre. Ked. Blue. Red. Blue. E«d. Blue. Bed. Blue. 670-8 33-0 34-4 5-3 4-9 656 48-0 56-4 9-1 8-4 642-0 79-0 ... 95-0 19-0 18-0 628-0 107-0 ... 126-0 38-0 38-5 615-0 147-0 ... 138-0 63-0 63-0 ... 603-0 151-0 155-0 90-0 84-0 591-0 137-0 ... 144-0 109-0 105-0 581-0 124-0 • . . 129-0 111-0 113-0 571-0 103-0 108-0 120-0 126-0 561-0 82-0 ... 89-0 108-0 106-0 552-0 64-0 ... 65-0 2-0 101-0 ... 544-0 52-0 56-0 78-0 85-0 ... 536-0 41-0 6-3 37-4 6-0 65-0 67-5 ... 525-0 26-0 12-0 21-0 10-3 38-3 11-0 46-8 515-0 15-0 28-0 13-7 21-6 20-6 34-0 32-8 13-0 505-0 7-7 36-0 7-5 32-2 9-8 35-0 17-2 29-0 496-0 3-7 48-0 4-1 46-3 4-8 47-0 8-4 330 488-0 1-6 62-0 1-9 58-0 2-2 57-0 5-3 49-0 480-0 0-9 64-0 0-9 67-0 0-9 66-0 2-9 71-0 469-0 0-3 70-0 0-3 65-6 0-3 67-0 1-0 69-0 460-8 ... 67-0 ... 68-6 ... 54-0 66-0 of partial colour-blinds is expressible in terms of two. If we choose as our fixed lights a red and a blue, these, mixed in certain proportions, will match every part of the spectrum as it appears to the partially colour-blind, and also unanalysed daylight. Normal colour vision is trichromatic, this is di- chromatic. But, just as the required proportions in the mixture differed in various types of trichromatic vision, we also find two types of dichromatics, distinguished by their 142 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES respective mixing ratios. The two classes are known by various names, of which I shall employ Protanope and Deuteranope as being quite non-committal from the theo- retical standpoint. Experimentally one proceeds just as in the examination of normal colour vision. A certain red and a certain blue are chosen, and the whole spectrum systemati- ze J \^ 1 \^ I 1 4 \> 1^ ^ \ / y \ |y> iv^ // \\. \ \ I / \, 'v^ y ^ / \v 'v^ . f N ^ ^ N t 1 \ !^ \N / / \ \ ^^ A, y i ^ \ \^ / \ ^ N,^ S N.. 1 ^ ^ s. ■^ s. B ■^ V ^ ■~-~ :i; — is 9 10 11 1Z 13 n IS 16 il 18 19 ZO Zl «3;iJ"^-S"T*e^«*<0 "9 14 LA ^ flO T^^p^JOt^yjlne^Cfa f^ ^ C3 m oft 12 3 4 5 6 7a lO Jo 5? Waxa Lengths cF Sptctnxjw. RetL YaMjes far FartiaL ColourBlinds (vSries). The two auvesA are' DetUeranopce, the' two £ Protanopes. cally matched against various mixtures of the chosen standard lights. The tables and diagrams embody the results of J. V. Kries.i An examination of the curves shows that the cases fall into two groups in respect of red values. The four blue value curves, on the other hand, are not very dissimilar ; in fact, remembering that the physical differences of macular 1 "Ueber Farbensysteme," Z.P.P.S.O,, vol. xiii. pp. 241-324. An abbre- viabed account, with the diagrams, by the same author in Nagel's Uandbuch d. Physiol, d. Menschen, Braunschweig, 1905, vol. iii. pp. 153-55. Some account of the experimental technic will be found in Z.P.P.S.O., vol. xii. pp. i, etc. DICHROMATIC VISION 143 absorption would be most influential in the part of the spectrum where the blue curves agree worst, we may regard the similarity as fairly close. Referring to the tables, we see that for matching spectral colours of wave length greater than 530 fifi, no blue at all is required, so that the forms of the red value curves beyond this point are especially instructive. In one group the curve rises fairly sharply to a maximum about 571 /u./a and then falls steeply, suggesting so 10 / ■ 2:: i? :;^ ?— ' -X eo ,. ■■-r^ •y- '" — N N y. 7 \ t 50 y '/ ■> / // "S § 20 S ^"^ / / / / ■^ / i 21 za 2^1 Blue Values of Partial Colour-Blinds. a relatively low stimulus value for the long-waved light; in the other group the rise is more gradual, attaining a maximum at 603 fifi, while the curve does not fall to so low a point at the red end. Hence, within the region under examination, we may say, with fair accuracy, that the four subjects are grouped into one pair relatively more sensitive to short-waved light and one pair relatively more sensitive to long-waved light. To the former group we apply the name Protanopes, to the latter that of Deuteranopes. But, it may be objected, we have only examined four cases. It 144 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES is necessary to see whether the results attained are confirmed when experiments are carried out on a larger scale. If the conclusions just stated be valid, in matching yellow some partial colour-blinds (protanopes) will require enormously more red than others (deuteranopes). This is a test which can be quickly applied, while the above examination is too elaborate to be often repeated. V. Kries tested twenty cases, using red (lithium line) and a fixed yellow (sodium line). The results are contained in the table below, and it will be seen that one set (protanopes) required on the average five times as much red as the other group (deuteranopes) to match rrotanopes. Deuteranopes. 214-0 36-5' 213-0 36-3' 211-0 36-3 » 205-0 36-5' 196-0 38-4 198-0 37-3 210-0 37-0 200-0 37-0 210-0 37-8 203-0 37-0 225-0 36-9 ... 38-0 the standaid yellow. There is, accordingly, some evidence that the classification is a definite one. The relative insensitiveness to long-waved light explains the observation that in protanopes, who correspond to the class (badly) named red blinds, the red end of the spectrum appears shortened, although the shortening is of very little importance from our point of view. It is to be remarked that within each class slight individual variations are found. In the two protanopic eases, from 552 /i/x onwards to violet, one subject constantly demanded more red in his mixture than the other. An interpretation is easy : in the observer requiring less red, the homogeneous light was probably weakened by macular absorption, since the diminution in red values ao-rees ' One subject. DICHROMATIC VISION 145 with the known increments of absorption as we pass towards the violet. This difference is of some importance in con- nection with an interesting peculiarity of dichromatic vision. Since daylight can be matched, for the dichromatic eye, by a mixture of red-blue, and since all spectral colours can be matched by mixing the same two standards in varying pro- portions, we should expect some spectral colour to match, more or less closely, unanalysed daylight, and to appear to the dichromatic the same as what we call white or grey. Such a point of the spectrum is called " the neutral point." As for protanopes, the stimulus value of the light falls off rapidly towards the red ; their " neutral point " should be nearer the violet than that of the deuteranopes ; this is, in fact, generally the case, but if there is a good deal of pigment in the macula lutea, the mixed light undergoes selective absorption, and the homogeneous match is nearer the red end of the spectrum. Owing to this fact, the typical differ- ence in the situation of the neutral point may not be found at all. The table on p. 146 gives results obtained on a deuteranope with a moderately pigmented macula, on a protanope with much and a protanope with little pigment in the macula region. Evidently simple determination of the neutral point would not enable us to distinguish the two forms. A study of the neutral point, however, immediately brings to our notice that characteristic which has attracted the attention of practical men. Spectral light between 490- 500 fifji. induces normally the sensation of green; for the dichromatic it has the same effect as unanalysed daylight or a particular mixture of red and blue. But this mixture con- tains much red and little blue, so that a normal person asked to name the simple colour which it most resembled would say red. In other words, both the dichromatic classes con- fuse red and green. Even here we observe a class difference. In matching a given bluish-green the protanope, being relatively insensitive to long waves, requires much red in his blue-red mixture ; the deuteranope takes about the same amount of blue, but much less red. Accordingly, a protanope confuses a light bluish-red (physically speaking) with a green that appears to us much darker, e.g. a scarlet with an olive- K 146 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES green ; a deuteranope matches a much bluer-red with green, which we should take to be about equally bright. Although both groups confuse certain reds with certain greens, " the red that appears equal to a given green differs markedly both in colour tone and intensity in protanopes and deu- teranopes " (v. Kries). The fact that both forms of common colour-blindness are associated with a confusion of red and green is of great practical importance, since these colours are universally employed for signalling in connection with railways and ships. In most countries persons desirous of acting as engine-drivers or navigating officers are submitted to examination with a view to the detection of colour-blind- Neutral Points {v. Kriex) Mixed Light Used. Wave Length (in fifi) of the homogeneous Match. Deuteranope. 1st Protanope. 2nd Protanope. Magnesium oxide } in daylight ^ 499 498 490 Mirrored cloud ') weakened by ^ ground glass ) 499 497 489 Do. do. weakened ) by smoked glass | 495 494 486 ness. While it is not the function of a theoretical writer to discuss at length a matter of this kind, a few remarks on the utilitarian aspect of the matter are not, perhaps, out of place. A test which can be easily applied, and is consequently dear to the mind of lay officials, is that associated with the name of Holmgren. The subject is given a large number of skeins of wool, and is told to separate out the reds and greens. This test is, in my opinion, altogether inefficient. Under the conditions of the test, the marked differences in luminosity of the wools often enables persons who suffer from typical colour-blindness to sort out the skeins correctly, especially if they have previously practised the experiment. Such DICHROMATIC VISION 147 persons, however, would be highly dangerous navigators or engine-drivers, because at times when the differentiation of a red from a green light is vital, e.g. in a slight haze or in snowy weather, such adventitious aids as differences of intensity, apart from colour tone, will be absent. Edridge Oreen, to whom we owe many important observations on these points, has perfected a lantern test which is in every way superior to that of Holmgren, and nobody who takes on himself the responsibility of testing colour vision for administrative purposes can safely rely on the other and less efficient method. For further details, special works must be ■consulted. Returning to the physiological characters of the two common types of colour vision, we have to consider what relations their visual systems bear to that of normal persons. The mere fact that the systems are 'dichromatic tells us com- paratively little ; we should indeed conclude that sensations of colour are less numerous for dichromatics than for us, but they might be quite different. What we seek is a relation between stimuli. As early as 1837 Seebeck expressed the opinion that two lights or mixtures of lights which appeared equal to the normal eye never appear unequal to colour- blind eyes, that equations valid for the normal eye are always valid for the colour-blind eye. If this be true, it is important, because in that event a dichromatic lacks some- thing the normal person has, but possesses nothing the normal man has not got ; in other words, dichromatism is an error of defect. Experimental evidence, particularly that furnished by v. Kries, on the whole bears out Seebeck's view. In the first place, mixtures of red (670-8 t^jj-) and yellowish-green (550 ^/i) which match a homogeneous yellow when the proportions are chosen to give a good match for a normal eye, give also equality for either a deuteranopic or protanopic subject. Similarly, any match which is found to be valid for both types is described as a good match by a normal person. " One employs the frequently cited equations between a homogeneous yellow and a mixture of red and yellowish- green (670-8 /*/* and 550 Myu). As all lights in this region are of 148 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES equal stimulus value for the colour-blind whatever the ratio of red to yellowish-green, one can always give the homogeneous yellow such an intensity that the match is good either for a protanope or a deuteranope, but in general the matches of the one are not valid for the other. As we should expect, a strongly red mixture is for the protanope equivalent to a yellow of relatively feeble intensity ; a deuteranope finds in a match arranged by the protanope the mixture too bright and the pure yellow too dark. The relation is reversed for strongly green mixtures. With extraordinary accuracy, however, we find that for the ratio of red to yellowish-green that has for the trichromatic an equal colour tone with the homogeneous yellow, both groups agree ; trichromatic equa- tions are valid for both protanopes and deuteranopes. Conversely, if we try to find an equation valid for both groups, we arrive precisely at the one valid for a normal person." i Still stronger evidence is afforded by the following con- siderations : — V. Kries prepared a table giving the mixing ratios for a normal person, matching spectral colours between 670"5 and 550 /j-jj-. The following are his results : — Spectral Keglon, Proportions of Standard Colours. Wave length. 670-8 n/i. 662 nu. 670-8 88-5 6280 251-0 10 6150 276-0 27 603-0 270-0 49 591-0 202-0 67 581-0 123-0 76 671-0 73-0 91 661-0 21-0 80 552-0 71 He also found that a certain deuteranope required 33 units of the standard red to match the spectral 670-8, and 64 units of the standard 552 to match the spectral 552 /n/x. If deuteranopic vision is a reduction form of the normal ' V. Kries, Nagel's Handh., iii. 160. DICHROMATIC VISION 149 system, it should be possible from these data to calculate the stimulus values of intervening lights. Take as an illustra- tion 591 iJ-iJ., which is matched by 202 units of 670-8 /x/i plus 67 units ot 552 /^/x. We first change from the arbitrary in- tensity units of the normal subject by dividing by 88-5 and 71 respectively, and then express in the intensity scale of the deuteranope by multiplying by 33 and 64 respectively. The stimulus value should therefore be 202 67 gg:^ X 33 + yY X 64 = 135 (approx.) units. This, then, is the stimulus value of 591 fj,/j, expressed in the intensity values of 670-8 fi/j. and 552 /x/x for the given spectrum, on the hypothesis that a match good for a trichromatic eye is valid for a dichromatic; the observed value was 137. In this way the following table was obtained, and the agreement is very good : — Stimulus Values for Deuteranope and Protanope. Wave Length. Deuteranope. Protanope. Calculated. Observed. Calculated. Observed. 670 33 33 4-9 4-9 628 106 107 28-8 38-5 615 126 145 54-2 63-0 603 145 151 86-0 84-0 591 135 137 108-0 105-0 581 114 124 117-0 1130 571 110 103 " 137-0 126-0 561 76 82 111-0 106-0 552 64 64 101-0 101-0 A further, but less satisfactory, verification is afforded by the construction of a normal colour table from observations made upon dichromatics. Bearing in mind the principles upon which the normal colour " triangle " depends, it is clear that in applying the method to dichromatic systems all those colours which appear alike to the colour-blind eye must lie on a straight line, since the line joining two points in the colour " triangle " contains all points corresponding to 150 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES colours which can be mixed from the colours represented by the first pair. It is also clear that the mixtures of confusion colours with any other colours lies on a series of straight lines. Hence it can be shown ^ that (1) all such lines meet in a point or are parallel, (2) the point of intersection corresponds to a colour which has no stimulus value for the colour-blind eye. This point is usually termed the " Fehlpunkt " of the system.^ Bearing these preliminaries in mind, the method of constructing a colour "triangle" for normal vision from dichromatic observations is simple. Let ABC be an equilateral triangle, then it is assumed that the protanopic red value, the deuteranopic red value, and the common blue value are independent one of another, and their representative points are the apices of the triangle. The positions of the various lights are then determined in the following manner : Take, for instance, 603 jxji. The pro- tanopic red value is 90, the deuteranopic red value 155, Fig. 19. the blue value 0. Divide AB in C'so that AC':C'B = 90 : 155. C marks the situation of 603 /xju in our "triangle." Take again 505 /i/^. The protanopic red value is 7"5, the deuteranopic red value 9'8, the blue value — — i — = 33-7o. Divide AB in C" so that AC" : C"B = 7-5 : 9-8. Join C"C ; divide C"C in C" so that Q"C"' : CC"'= 33-75 : 9-8 + 7-5. The diagram above shows a colour "triangle" constructed in this way by v. Kries. The only modifications are that (1) the unit for blue values has been increased fivefold, i.e. the tabled values (p. 149) have been divided by five for con- venience of reproduction; (2) "the very small W (red) values for lights of wave lengths less than 505 /^jit are not sufiSciently exact to permit of the evaluation of the ratio * See proof at the end of the chapter. DICHROMATIC VISION 151 (protanopic red value : deuteranopic red value) with certainty. On this account, that part of the boundary line extending from blue to green would be correspondingly irregular, whereas observations on normal sighted persons show that it must present a steady curvature, mixtures of the two lights appearing a little less saturated than the spectral lights. I have for this reason drawn the boundary line with a constant curvature, so that the ratio gradually passes from that corresponding to lights between 515 j^/^ and 525 /i/i to that observed for lights from 469 /xju, to 480 [j-iJ'. These two values are indicated in the drawing by the dotted lines from C ; accordingly a curve drawn in exact correspondence with the observations would run irregularly between these lines. It should also be remarked that a construction of ao-8 Fig. 20. this sort is not entirely free from objection, as it combines the results of several observers with undoubtedly diiferent amounts of macular pigmentation." ^ That the process is not so satisfactory as the one already considered will be readily admitted. But we notice again that the deduced " triangle " agrees very fairly with that given in the chapter on trichromatic systems, so that it affords some confirmation of the surmise that dichromatic systems are in reality reduction forms of the trichromatic type. The reader is specially cautioned that the conclusions hinted at in the last few pages are not to be pushed too far ; for reasons, some of which will be considered in the chapters on theories, all the prominent workers of to-day recog- 1 v. Kries, Nagel's Handb., iii. 162. 152 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES nise that partial colour-blindness cannot be completely or adequately described in terms of a hypothesis postulating so simple a relation as that just suggested between normal and abnormal kinds of vision. The experimental results are, however, by no means without interest, and I have for that reason devoted some space to the matter. Summing up the rather difficult topics dealt with in this chapter, we may say that — (1) The two common forms of partial colour-blindness are distinguishable one from another by varying responsive- ness to long-waved and medium-waved lights. Protanopes are relatively insensitive to long waves, deuteranopes to short waves. (2) Each is an example of dichromatic vision, using that term in a strictly experimental sense. (3) Each is, very approximately, a reduction form of normal trichromatic vision. The forms of partial colour-blindness just described are of everyday occurrence, and their recognition of obvious practical importance ; another type, less common, and there- fore less completely studied, is that known as Blue or Yellow- Blue Blindness. This condition, unlike the last, is not always congenital, and frequently one-sided. Definite pathological changes in the retina have also been observed in several examples of the defect, and the whole visual field may not be involved. Koenig's observations make it probable that this type also is dichromatic, two suitably chosen simple lights matching the whole spectrum. Blue blindness may be a reduction form, since all normal matches appear to be valid for such an eye. The neutral point is in the yellow between 566 /x/x and 570 ij-ij- ; the " Fehlpunkt " is close to the violet. Our knowledge of this condition is not sufficiently exact for it to be profitably discussed in a text-book.^ The experimental production of a form of colour-blind- ness by Burch has so direct a bearing on the theories of * The curious reader will find sufficient references to the literature of Blue Blindness in my article on " Theories of Colour Vision " in Further Advances in Physiology, edited by Leonard Hill, London, 1909 (Arnold). DICHEOMATIC VISION 153 colour Tision, that I shall consider his work in the chapter devoted to the theories of the subject. We shall next turn our attention to the phenomena of successive induction or after-images. APPENDIX TO CHAPTER XV. Proof of the existence of a " Fehlpmikt." ' Suppose the quantity r o£ a colour situated at R in the diagram matches the quantity jr of a colour at Gr, r = nr + (l — n)r. But the quantity nr of 'R=ng of G. Therefore if m be a proper fraction, r of R matches {l — n)r of lS, + ng of G. The situation of this mixture is S if RS_ ng ,j> SG {l-n)r ■ ■ ■ ^ ' and the quantity s of the mixed colour obtained = Mjf + { 1 - ra))-, and this is independent of n. If, now, we mix b parts of some other colour B with s of S, we obtain a mixed colour whose appearance is independent of n. Let its measure be t and its situation T, TS b J = 6 + s = 6 + mo + (l-ra)r-, and, as before, =— = p: r- . . . (la) ""■''' jjX ng + {i -n)r Draw BH perpendicular to RG and TL perpendicular to BH. Call LH, X, BH, h, TL, y, HG a, and RG c. |=gg = — (by similar triangles) = ^ , „„ , /i — ^„. ■ ■ ■ (1)6 ^ V _TL_SH_ SG-tt h-x BL BH h ' But, from (1) SG = c (!-"> ng + {\-n)r ■ V ^ jc-aXl-ny-ang .^, 'h-x hlng + {l-?i)r] Eliminating n between (16) and (Ic), we have = ybh(g - r) - x[crg + br{c-a) + abg'] + bh[{c-a)r + ag] . . . (Id) This is a linear relation between the S'lG- 21. co-ordinates x and y otT with respect to an origin H. Therefore the locus of T is a straight line. 1 Helmholtz, op. cit., pp. 363, etc. 154 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES Let TQ be the locus intersecting RG in Q. QH. = y when x = Q, . „ _ (c-a)'- + ag ■ • y^ F=-^' i.e. is independent of b, the amount mixed from B. Therefore all lines of equal mixtures from RGB intersect at the same point Q, or they are parallel, in which case r=g, and i/q is infinite. The distance of Q from R is given by 3/o-c + a=-^ = QR . . . (1/) r-g If we mix q of the colour Q with g of the colour G, so that R results g=|butRG = c, r-g q ^ ■> Since, by hypothesis, r=g and q = r — g is not necessarily = 0, the colour Q has no stimulus value tor a colour-blind eye. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY The literature of Colour Blindness is enormous, and most of the papers mingle a description of experimental or clinical observations with theoretical discussions. I should advise the student first to read : — /. von Kries, Ueber Farbensysteme, Z.P.P.S.O., vol. xiii. pp. 241- 324. Having mastered this, he might read the account in Helmholtz's Handbuch, and then the more recent controversial papers. A complete bibliography of the literature up to 1896 is appended to the second edition of Helmholtz's Handbuch. Dr. Edridge Green's work on Colour Blindness and Colour Perception, in the International Scientific Series (Kegan Paul), contains many ingenious speculations and matter of practical interest, but should be read in a critical spirit. CHAPTER XVI AFTER-IMAGES OE SUCCESSIVE INDUCTION That after the withdrawal of a stimuhis some sensation process persists has long been known. In 1634 Peiresc described the positive and negative after-images of the window, and a few years later Bonacursius maintained in opposition to the learned Jesuit Athanasius Kircher that one could see as well in the dark as by daylight, convincing his opponent by the following experiment. Kircher was taken into a room and made to look fixedly at a drawing covering a window slit ; the room was then completely darkened, and Kircher once more perceived the drawing on looking at a piece of white paper in his hand. The Jesuit's own explana- tion of the result was that light absorbed by the eye streamed out in the dark and illuminated the paper. Similar experi- ments were described by Mariotte in 1668, and Newton was familiar with the phenomena, which he held to be of psycho- logical origin. In the eighteenth century many workers observed these after-images or " accidental colours," particu- larly the negative or complementary images, which, as we shall find, are more easily studied. Writing in 1734, Jurin observed that the "contrary sensation is apt to arise in us, sometimes of itself, and sometimes from such causes as at another time would not produce the sensation at all, or at least not to the same degree." ^ Scherfer (1761) investigated complementary images, and in copying a picture painted it with a green face shaded with yellow, white hair and eyebrows, black eyeballs with white pupils, and green lips, so that the after-image had the colours of the original. Kobert Waring Darwin (1786) studied both positive and negative after-images, explaining clearly enough the experimental precautions essential for their demonstra- 1 Quoted by Burch, Proc. Roy. Soc, 1900, Ixvi. 204. 165 156 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES tion, e.g. the necessity of excluding any extraneous light if the "direct spectrum" (positive after-image) is to be seen. Goethe in his Farbenlehre, which appeared in 1810, records several observations of negative after-images, of which the following is an amusing example : — "I had entered an inn towards evening, and as a well- favoured girl with a brilliantly fair complexion, black hair, and a scarlet bodice came into the room, I looked attentively at her as she stood before me at some distance in half shadow. As she presently afterwards turned away, I saw on the white wall, which was now before me, a black face surrounded with a bright light, while the dress of the perfectly distinct figure appeared of a beautiful sea-green." ^ During the nineteenth century the subject has been closely examined by an array of workers, amongst whom Brewster, Fechner, Briicke, Helmholtz, and Hering are, perhaps, the most prominent. I shall give a short account of the facts, avoiding, so far as possible, any theoretical pre- suppositions. No special apparatus is required for the demonstration of some fundamental results. If one looks fixedly for 20 to 40 seconds at a moderately illuminated white spot or source of light and then shuts the eyes or looks towards a sheet of white paper, a black spot usually surrounded by a bright halo is visible. If the object be coloured, then performing a similar experiment we see a spot tinged with the comple- mentary of the original impression. These are the negative or complementary after-images with which all are familiar. To obtain an after-sensation identical in character with that experienced during the original stimulation, a positive after-image, somewhat greater care is necessary, since eye or body movements rapidly cause its disappearance or inhibit its production. " After waiting a sufficient time with care- fully covered eyes, one turns the eyes (still covered by the hands) toward the object, taking care to remain quite still, then takes the hands away rapidly, and with the same speed brings them back again. This movement of the hands must 1 Goethe's Theory of Colours, Eastlake's trans., 1840, p. 22. AFTER-IMAGES OF SUCCESSIVE INDUCTION 157 be executed gently, without violent exertion or shaking of the body. If the experiment has been properly carried out, the positive after-image seen behind the covering hands is sometimes so sharply defined and bright, that it seems as though the hands were transparent and one still saw the real object. One has sufficient time to note details in the after- image which there was no time to study during the actual exposure." ^ The first thing to grasp is that no discontinuity appears to exist between positive and negative after-images. Suppose one develops an after-image by looking at a bright object, and that on closing the eyes one obtains a bright image on a dark ground, i.e. a positive after-image ; then if instead of closing the eyes after performing such an experiment one looks at a moderately illuminated field, a dark image on a bright ground is seen, i.e. a negative after-image. Between these termini we can obtain a continuous series of intermediates ; for instance, we can choose a field of brightness such that, on fixating it, no after-image is obtained at all. This amounts to saying that the nature of an after-image is in the main determined by two factors: (1) The quality and intensity of the first or primarily exciting stimulus ; (2) the character of the secondary or, as we may call it, modifying stimulus. In any after-image sensation, both factors are necessarily involved ; even when the retina is shielded from external stimuli, a stimulation process is active, bound up with the sensation called by some the light chaos, by others the autocthonous retinal light. Naturally, this is not an ex- haustive enumeration of the modifying factors; the con- ditions noted are necessary, but not sufficient. Individual peculiarities, both psychological and physiological, are in- volved, but our first study must be directed to the constant factors which give us a definite point of departure. Let us divide the experiments into two chief groups : (1) The eye is exposed to one extrinsic stimulus only; (2) two extrinsic stimuli are successively employed. In (1) we have the primary stimulus compounded with 1 Helmholtz, Handb. d. Phys. Optik., second edition, p. 504. 15 8 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES the intrinsic retinal factor alone, in (2) the state of affairs is more complex. Before applying a stimulus, the entoptic sensation or "light chaos" is indeterminate in character, varying like most subjective phenomena with the observer. Helmholtz speaks of " manifold light points in motion often like convoluted vessels or scattered moss fibres and leaves." The appearance is often like that of waves passing slowly across the field of vision. If we regard fixedly a moderately bright object for 40 seconds and then darken the eyes, we see in the light chaos a dark patch surrounded by a bright halo ; that is to say, the effect is much the same as noticed if one looks at a moderately bright surface, the " light chaos " behaves like such a surface. Turning to the more important case (2), we find a com- plex of results. Let us call the first extrinsic stimulus the Ketuning Light, the second extrinsic stimulus applied to the same area the Reacting Light. The effect produced by the reacting light will depend on the change induced by the retuner, and can be measured by comparing its effect Avith that obtained on stimulating regions not exposed to the retuning light; the stimulus applied to such an area may be termed the Comparison Light. The whole process is conveniently named Retuning. I have already made it seem probable that stimulus values are not the same in different parts of the retina, and that light-dark adaptation is selective ; hence, that our results may be pure, we must work with small directly fixated fields and under conditions of constant adaptation, preferably light adaptation. The first question that arises is as to the validity of the mixing laws. We can match a homogeneous yellow with a red-green mixture; if we retune with a homogeneous yellow, a reacting yellow does not, of course, match a com- parison yellow, and similarly for a retuning and reacting mixture. The question is whether the responses to the two originally equal stimuli are changed to a like extent, whether the simple and mixed colours still match. Appa- rently the answer must be affirmative. " On small directly fixated fields optical equations suffer AFTER-IMAGES OF SUCCESSIVE INDUCTION" 159 no alteration if the tuning of this part of the retina be altered by means of any light stimulus whatever. For instance, a homogeneous yellow and a centrally equivalent red-green mixture appear after previous yellow illumination both paler, after previous blue illumination both more saturated yellow, but they still match perfectly. In the same way, the equality of unanalysed daylight and a white formed by a complemen- tary mixture is not destroyed if both fields, by means of previous colour stimulation, are strongly tinged by the com- plementary of the retuning light." ^ This very important result has not escaped criticism; we are all agreed that it cannot be obtained by stimulating the peripheral region of the retina. The correctness of the experiments so far as the fovea centralis is concerned is testified to by Btihler, who has re-investigated the matter and can be provisionally accepted. Accepting the persistence of optical equations as an experimental fact, conclusions may be drawn which enable us to deduce some interesting formuliie. Thus we can obtain a relation between the change in appearance of different reacting lights and any specified retuning. Put briefly, we may say that the change in appearance of a light as a result of retuning consists in a lowered or heightened responsiveness of the eye to that light. If R be the stimulus measure, the sensation result will depend on a product of the form a ■ R, where a is a measure of the retinal excitability for R, a value dependent on the tuning ; in determining the effect produced in any given case of re- tuning, we have only to consider the value of a. Suppose we have two retinal points A and B differently tuned, and R^ falling on A matches Rg at B, S^ at A similarly matching Sg at B. Hence (adopting the ordinary convention as to equality), Rj^ + Sj = R2 + Sj. Now let A and B be retuned by exposure to light of stimulus value T, which alters the excitability for S^ and S^ respectively. We now have at A, R^ + aS]^, and for B, Rg + aSj, i.e. R^ + S^ — (1 — a.)Si, and R2 + S2-(l-(x)S2, which must match, since by hypothesis S^ at A is equal to S2 at B. This is the 1 V. Ki-ies, Nagel's Handb., iii. p. 210. 160 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES essence of tlie so-called " Law of Coefficients," and is a short way of stating the experimental facts mentioned above. The law obviously fails in the case of feeble stimuli, because then the autocthonous stimulus (the light chaos) becomes measurable relatively to Sj, Sg, and T, and can no longer be neglected, as in the above expressions. Let us next ex- amine some matters of detail. First, what happens when the retuning and reacting lights are identical ? This merely requires for answer a study of the sensation changes accompanying prolonged fixation of any object, and we all know the answer. Brightness differ- ences grow smaller and smaller, being at length effaced, which suggests a relationship between the degree of re- tuning and the strength of the stimulus. Next, suppose we retune as before with white, but use a chromatic reactor. We find (under conditions of light adaptation) that the reactor must have the same qualitative composition as the comparison, but greater intensity. V. Kries in his experiments obtained a good match if the amounts of coloured light in reactor and comparison were approximately in the ratio of 3 or 4 to 1. With a colour mixer, the blue sectors were 270° : 97° ; the red, 270° : 84° ; the yellow, 270° : 97°. If the coloured sectors were made equal, and the white so chosen that the discs were approximately equal in brightness, the sensation excited in the retuned area was always that of far too low saturation. White retuning, therefore, seems to change chromatic stimulus values ; but the experiments upon which this con- clusion is based have been adversely criticised by Hering, whose objections will be considered in a later chapter, when we come to deal with the theoretical side of the question. The next case is that of retuning with a colour, and here also the simplest example is when retuner and reactor are the same. Without a comparison, loss in brightness and saturation are apparent; with a comparison, a difference in colour tone may also be perceptible. Voeste's results are as follows : A yellow (560 fxfj), a green (500 jj-ij.), and a blue (460 fj-n) undergo no change in hue on pro- AFTER-IMAGES OF SUCCESSIVE INDUCTION 161 longed fixation; intermediates pass away from the green towards the red or the blue. Thus a light between 500 and 460 ixfji, will match a comparison of less wave length than itself; one between 500 and 560 i^ti, a light of greater wave length. If we use a grey field as reactor it produces a sensation closely allied to that normally associated with the retuner's complementary, and this is not a mere blend of the sen- sation obtained in the darkened eye after retuning plus the whiteness normally excited by a grey field. If this were so, after retuning with a given yellow the subjective blue could always be compensated by mixing the same amount of yellow with the reacting field so that it might appear colourless. As a matter of fact, however, the more white there is in the reacting field, the more yellow must be mixed with it — as we shouid expect from the coeflScient rule. If the reactor be grey, the complementary of the retuner may be very well marked; if the reactor be the complementary itself, a sensation of colour is produced far exceeding in purity that associated with any region of the spectrum; this result is universally admitted to be of con- siderable theoretical importance, as we shall find later. Finally, if some colour not complementary to the retuner be used as reactor various sensations are experienced, although in general the complementary of the retuner predominates. After red retuning, v. Kries matched a re- acting yellow (589 fjufx) with a green-yellow (556 yu/i); after green retuning, it matched an orange (605 /*/i). Hess, after blue retuning, matched a reacting 517 ^j" with a comparison 565 /^/-t. These experiments are interesting, but a systematic study of the eftects of retuning when each spectral colour is used in turn as a reactor, with a com^ parison field, has yet to be effected, and we cannot at present lay down any general laws. The time relations of the process of retuning are of some interest. Many researches have been published, the technique of some being simple, of others elaborate. One plan is easy; a white object on a dark ground is fixated, and one ascertains, after a definite interval, what comparison L 162 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES light placed near the object matches it at the moment of observation. The table contains v. Kries' results. By a similar method, we can measure the rate at which the effect passes off with a definite amount of retuning. Un- fortunately, the diflSculty of avoiding two important sources of error — adaptation and extra-foveal stimulation — is great, and we can lay but little stress on the results. For instance, it is not known whether the effect of retuning passes off smoothly, as Fechner and Helmholtz believed, or in pulses, Changes in White Value {v.K'i 'ies). strength erf Stimulus (Arbitrary Units). Apparent Diminution After Seconds. - 3 6 • 10 20 40 80 160 1 •91 •81 ■66 •58 •43 •23 ' .,5 1-9.5 . . •8G •74 •62 •52 •32 •18 •09 3-9 •82 •71 •62 •34 •21 •14 •08 34-7 ■74 •57 •42 •25 ■16 •08 •03 as held by Aubert. It is likely that some of Aubert's evidence is vitiated by imperfect technique, but his hypo- thesis cannot be disproved. Exner attempted to measure the change in sensation from the moment at which the stimulus is applied by an ingenious method, which has been used in a modified form by Burch. This method depends on the fact that with a given stimulus the sensation increases to a maximum, then, in consequence of retuning, diminishes. At a definite instant a stimulus, eg. & bright semicircle in a dark field, is applied ; then after a known short interval a bright field is exhibited; finally, after another known interval a dark field appears. In this way two neighbouring points have the same stimulus applied to them, but on one the stimulus begins to act a fraction of a second earlier than on the other. If the first stimulus AFTER-IMAGES OF SUCCESSIVE INDUCTION 163 effect has reached its maximum but the second has not, the resultant after-image will be positive; if both have passed their maxima, the after-image of the semicircle will be negative, because the first excitation process will have fallen to a lower level than the second. Between these extremes we have a point at which no differentiation appears, the field seeming to be uniform. This latter result will be obtained when one stimulus has just passed, the second just fallen short of its maximum, and accordingly gives a measure of the time necessary to attain the first maximum; Ingenious as are these experiments, their value as mea- sures of the time relations in retuning is not, perhaps, very great. Evidently they depend on the assumption that sensational intensity changes after the stimulus has been withdrawn, just as it does while the stimulus is still acting. Our study of recurrent vision, particularly the brightness relations of the tertiary image, has taught us that this assumption is of doubtful truth. We thus know but little of the excitation changes quantitatively, and even from the qualitative stand- point our information is not complete. The colour changes undergone by the after-image of a colourless object gene- rally form a white-red-green-red-blue cycle. The image of a coloured object passes from positive to negative (com- plementary) through a colourless or reddish-white phase. It is quite certain that the duration of after-image effects is much longer than has been generally thought. Burch passed from an ordinary into a completely darkened room and noted the sensations experienced with the following results : — " 0*5 mins.^ Confused after-images of portions of objects recently looked at. These images gradually fade and give place to a luminous fog, made up of what I have called dazzle tints, i.e. coloured impressions of luminosity without form. " 10-15 mins. Fog no longer uniform but ' spotty,' with patches of a brownish or greenish-bronze colour. " 18-21 mins. A group of yellow or green luminous points. > G. J. Burch, Proc. Roy. Soc, B., 1905, pp. 212-213. 164 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES " 23-28 mins. Green dazzle tints predominate, beginning to break up, in some cases, into patches of green showing a fainter blue between. . . . " 106-120 mins. Gradually a luminous fog seems to iill the surrounding space. For a minute or two it increases in brightness, and then breaks up near the middle in two or three places and seems to roll away on all sides. Then it returns, and again breaks up, generally leaving an island about the size of the yellow spot, which lasts two or three seconds longer than the rest. The colour of these clouds is a rich pure violet, like that of the calcium lines H and K in the arc spectrum. This phenomenon may go on from ten minutes to half-an-hour, the violet patches getting smaller and appearing at longer intervals till they die away." To sit patiently in a dark room studying the time relations of after-images may seem crude and uninteresting to those, who associate experimental research with expensive apparatus, and cannot be made to realise that some of the most brilliant achievements of physiology have been effected with the help of little except ability. It is probable that much-needed light will be thrown on the process of retuning by an exten- sion of work, such as that of Burch. A series of experiments must be carried out, the eye being exposed for a definite time to a given light before each period of observation and the resultant changes are compared. In some such fashion we may be able to unravel the complicated tangle of results already mentioned. Summing up the definitely ascertained conclusions, we find:— (1) The distinction between positive and negative after- images is not absolute but relative, depending on the nature of the reacting stimulus. (2) An image-producing or returning stimulus changes the stimulus value of a reacting (subsequently applied) light, but only in such a way that the sensation-response following exposure to the reacting light is increased or diminished quantitatively. Colour equations do not lose their validity. (3) The latter statement, as expressed in the " Coefficient AFTER-IMAGES OF SUCCESSIVE INDUCTION 165 Law," is true in the case of foveal vision, but not for peripheral stimulation. (4) We do not know the time relations of the retuning process, nor whether the latter proceeds uniformly or in pulses. I have now reviewed briefly the chief experimental data upon which a theory of visual sensations (exclusive of spatial perceptions) must rest. The question of simultaneous contrast has indeed been omitted, but owing to certain peculiarities of this branch of the subject it is best postponed until the more generally interesting theories of colour vision have been examined. We shall therefore now pass on to the theoretical side of our investigation, and endeavour to co-ordinate the apparently heterogeneous facts in some logical manner. We shall see that, wide as are the apparent differences of opinion, and many as are the difficulties to be encountered, both the main theories embody something of interest and importance. It will add to the clearness of our discussion if a preliminary chapter is devoted to some ancient theories of vision, and these will be examined in the following pages. BOOKS AND PAPERS BBCOMMENDBD FOR FURTHER STUDY Helmlwltti, Hand. d. Physiol. Optik., second edition, pp. 501, etc. V. Kries, Nagel's Handb., vol. iii. pp. 205, etc. Tschermak, Ueber das Verhaltnis von Gegenfarbe, Kompensationsfarbe und Kontrastfarbe (Pfliig. Arch., 1907, cxvii. 204). Goethe's Farbenlehre, Eastlake's trans. F. Klein, Nachbilder, Uebersicht und Nomenklatur (Engelmann's Arch. f. Physiol., 1908, Supp. Bd. p. 219). G. J. Burch, Proc. Roy. Soc, 1900, Ixvi. 204. CHAPTEE XVII HISTORICAL THEORIES OF VISION In attempting to estimate the scientific value of ancient theories of vision, it is necessary to bear in mind certain limitations which were imposed by incomplete means of investigation. Even with modern apparatus it is not easy to obtain a precise knowledge of the elaborate structures con- tained in the eye, hence workers unprovided with the simplest microscope knew almost nothing of facts which form part of the mental equipment of every educated man in our own time. Roughly, we may say that all the early theories agree in regarding the " pupil " of the eye and the " image " within it as of primary importance. Again, the flash of light seen on pressing or rapidly moving the eye was held to prove the existence of an inherent or native " fire," also of great signi- ficance. Thirdly, the presence of a watery substance within the eye required some explanation. The problem, as it pre- sented itself to the earliest writers, was to assign their proper shares in the visual act to the " fire," the " image," and the "water." As knowledge of psychology or physiological psychology progressed, the theories became more intricate, and it is possible to distinguish between theories of physio- logical optics, theories of colour vision, and more general inquiries as to the nature of perception in general and spatial perception in particular. Work under the last heading is much the most subtle, and has proved of enduring interest and value for modern thinkers. The speculations as to anatomy and physiological optics are, for the reasons above recited, of little importance ; while the theories of colour vision occupy an intermediate place, having in some respects proved fruitful, in others barren. As any one who is familiar with the subject will recognise, it is not in all cases easy to separate out an author's theory of vision into the sections enumerated, i«t HISTORICAL THEORIES OF VISION U7 and my account of the matter will not be so clear as I could wish. I shall try in this chapter to outline the main theories of vision, especially in regard to colour vision ; purely ana- tomical or physical theories will not be examined. One of the earliest Greek writers on this subject was Alcmseon of Cretona (fl. 500 B.C.), but our knowledge of his opinions is fragmentary. He appears to have thought that seeing is accomplished by the passage of rays from the ocular " fire " to the object, and that these returning to the eye, altered in some way, are reflected in the diaphanous "water." The fire is therefore the active element in vision. It is not clear how Alcmason harmonised the conception of a visual ray from the " fire " with that of mirroring in the "water." Empedocles (circa 450 B.C.) evolved a more subtle theory, but it is not easy to reconcile different statements attributed to him. According to the first doctrine enunciated by Empedocles, like perceives like. All bodies are characterised by the following properties : (1) All are made up of four elements — earth, air, fire, and water. (2) All are permeated by minute passages or pores, and all give off emanations which enter the pores. Thus, in perception, emanations from the object pass into the pores of the percipient organ. But that this passage may be effected it is necessary that the emanations and the pores should correspond; if the former be too large or too small for the latter, no percep- tion can occur. Hence with the eye alone can we perceive emanations of colour, because these are " symmetrical " with the pores of the eye alone. This correspondence is the basis of sense specificity. Further, there is a symmetrical arrange- ment within the eye itself with respect to the different forms of stimulation. By means of the intra-ocular fire we per- ceive the emanations of fire — i.e. white ; with the water we see water — i.e. black ; and so on. " With earth we see earth, with water we see water, with air we see the bright air ; just as with love we (perceive) love, and with hate, baleful hate." ^ Empedocles is said to have regarded four colours — white, black, red, and green — as primaries (Stobseus), but only 1 Beare, Greeh Theories of Elementary Cognition from Ahmceon to Aristotle, p. 18. 168 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES examines black and white in detail. He also taught that rays issued from the visual "fire," but how he associated this with his general doctrine of pores and emanations is not clear. It is probable, judging from some passages in Lucretius' great poem,i that a somewhat similar doctrine was held by Epicurus. Democritus (? 460-357 B.C.) agreed with Empedocles in pos- tulating the entrance of particles from an object into pores contained in the perceiving structure, and in the dictum that " like is perceived by like." But he denied that there were four qualitatively distinct elements, believing that all things were made up of homogeneous atoms moving in a vacuum and infinitely numerous. Vision is due to the mirroring of an object in the eye, the latter's character being somehow determined by its moist and porous nature. This part of Democritus' theory was sharply criticised by Aristotle, who remarked, " It is absurd that it should not have occurred to him to doubt why the eye alone sees, but nothing else in which energies are apparent. That the sight is aqueous is true; yet it does not happen that it sees because it is aqueous, but because it is diaphanous, which is also common to air." 2 Democritus seems to have been the first writer to attempt a detailed theory of colours, the simple ones being white, black, red, and green. White is the smooth, because any- thing which is not rough, does not throw shadows, and is not difficult to penetrate, is bright. Black consists of atomic figures of an opposite kind, viz. those which are rough, uneven, and dissimilar ; on this account they cast shadows, and their pores are neither straight nor easily permeable. Red is formed of the same kind of atomic figures as the hot, but the figures of red are larger. A proof that red is com- posed of such atoms as those forming hot is, that " we our- selves are red when heated, just as other things are when ignited, as long as they continue to have the character of ' the igneous ' ; but ignited things are redder in proportion as they are formed of large figures, such as flame, coals, or » Lucr., De Rer. Nat., ii. 833. ' Aristotle, De Sensu et SensUibus, Taylor's trans., vol. iii. 133. HISTORICAL THEORIES OF VISION 169 wood, whether green or dry, and also iron and other metals which are subject to ignition."^ Green is formed of the solid and the void, the tint varying with their position and arrangement. Each colour is purer the more the figures of which it is essentially composed are free from admixture with others. All other colours are mixtures of these four. For instance, purple is mixed from white, black, and red, red being in largest and black in smallest amount. Owing to white coming midway, the colour is pleasant to the sense. It is hardly possible for us to criticise this theory with understanding. Here, as so often in considering ancient science, one is conscious of the gulf which separates us from the world which has passed away. Much which seems meaningless or nonsensical probably only does so because not only has the author's work come down to us in a fragmentary state, but it is impossible to see things from his point of view, or even to find out what that point of view was. In ancient times, Theophrastus, who no doubt under- stood it better than we do, criticised Democritus' theory in so far as it deals with colour. He says that Democritus made a difficulty by suggesting four primaries rather than two — black and white — and objects that the position of the atoms rather than their shape or figure should be the cause of colour. The matter is, however, so obscure, that it can hardly be pursued in this place. The views of Anaxagoras (499-428 B.C.) and of Diogenes of Appolonia (fifth century B.C.) can, for similar reasons, be passed over rapidly. Anaxagoras held, in opposition to his contemporaries, although the opposition is more formal than real, that perception is the result, not of like operating on like, but of the reaction between contrary and contrary. The "image" is not reflected upon a part of like colour to the object, but upon a different colour. Diogenes, who believed that an all-pervading air was the ultimate agency in nature, has left no distinct theory of colour vision. We now come to one of the great names in scientific history, that of Plato, whose views, although far less ' Beare, op. cit., p. 32. 170 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES suggestive than those of Aristotle, are well worth con- sideration. Plato has considered the problem from two different points of view. His account of the physical side of the matter, as one may term it, is contained in a passage in the I'imcBus, of which the following quotation gives an idea : — " And of the organs they first contrived the eyes to give light, fixing them by a cause on this wise. They contrived that as much of fire as would not have the power of burning, but would only give a gentle light, the light of everyday life, should be formed into a body ; and the pure fire which is within us and akin to this they made to flow through the eyes in a single entire and smooth substance, at the same time compressing the centre of the eye so as to retain all the grosser element, and only to allow this to be sifted through pure. When therefore the light of day surrounds the stream of vision, then like falls upon like, and there is a union, and one body is formed by natural affinity according to the direction of the eyes, wherever the light that falls from within meets that which comes from an external body. And everything being affected by likeness, whatever touches or is touched by the stream of vision, their motions are diffused over the whole body and reach the soul, producing that per- ception which we call sight." ^ . In the genesis of colour, particles are discharged froni external things and impinge upon the eye, some being larger, some smaller, and some equal in magnitude to the parts of the eye. All colours are compounded of four — white, black, bright, and red. Bright when mixed with red and white becomes golden-yellow; red blended with black and white yields violet. From statements in the TiTuceus and Republic it would seem that Plato, unlike Democritus, believed in the objective existence of colour, but, as Helmholtz pointed out, his views seem to have varied. In the Thecetetus, colour is considered from a different standpoint, as will be clear from the next quotation : " We shall see that every colour, white, black, and every other colour, arises out of the eye meeting 1 Plato, Timceus, Jowett's trans., ii. 375. HISTORICAL THEORIES OF VISION 171 the appropriate motion, and that what we term the substance of each colour is neither the active nor the passive element, but something which passes between them and is peculiar to each percipient. . . , "When the eye and the appropriate object meet together and give birth to whiteness, and the sensation of whiteness which could not have been given by either of them going to any other object ; while the sight is flowing from the eye, and whiteness from the colour-producing element, the eye becomes fulfilled with sight and sees, and becomes not sight, but a seeing eye ; the object which combines in forming the colour is fulfilled with whiteness and becomes not whiteness, but white." ^ It may be said that Plato's views are interesting, but do not embody any complete theory of colour vision. Aristotle (384-322 B.C.) elaborated a theory which is not the least important of his contributions to natural know- ledge and, clothed in modern notation, still survives. According to Aristotle, the object of sight is colour. Colour is at the surface of all visible bodies, but in order to be seen requires the presence of light, which is the medium of vision. This basal proposition formed part of the teaching of Epi- curus, to judge from a notable passage in the work of his greatest exponent,^ but in developing the conception Aristotle far surpassed Epicurus. Light, says Aristotle, presupposes a diaphanous substrate, which in its turn is the medium of light. Examples of this " diaphanous " are air, water, and many solids. The realisa- tion or actualisation of this potential quality of being dia- phanous is light, its absence darkness. When the former condition of actual light is established in the diaphanous medium, any coloured body sets up a movement in it be- tween object and eye ; this is the essential process in colour perception. The diaphanous substrate upon which depends the existence of light and, a fortiori, colour is not peculiar to the bodies called transparent or diaphanous, but is a species of universally difi'fised natural power ; it is not indeed ^ Plato, Thecetetus, Jowett's trans., vol. iii. 538-9. 2 Lucretius, De Ra-wn Nat., ii. 730-833, especially 795-8. 172 PHYSIOLOGY OP THE SPECIAL SENSES capable of existence independently of " body," but subsists in varying degrees in all things. The colour of a body either forms its surface or is upon its surface, the latter opinion being the more exact since the indeterminate " diaphanous " of air and water exhibits colour, -which, however, owing to the indeterminate boundary, is variable. This explains the changing hues of sea and sky. Bodies with a definite boundary have a fixed colour, so that one might again define colour as the surface limit of the diaphanous in determinately bounded body. This definition is consistent with the first given, viz. that which stimulates the actualised " diaphanous " (light) between the object and the eye, but the latter is a definition in terms of vision and the medium of vision, the former in terras of the object as it exists apart from vision. Colour is a genus comprising seven species ; it is a quality, and cannot therefore exist without a substrate. The seven species are white, black, golden-yellow, crimson, violet, leek- green, and deep blue. The colour genus (like all other genera of sensible qualities) consif^ts of species lying between extremes ; outside these extremes there can be no colours, between them are specific boundaries. By subdividing the scale limited by the extremes, we cannot obtain an infinite number of distinct colours, because a sensible quality is discrete, not continuous. By dividing the substrate we do not arrive at any new colour ; the halves of a white object are white. It is true that by sufficiently fine division no colour whatever may be perceptible, but on reuniting these portions we again obtain white. The two limits are black and white; when one is actually existent, the other is potentially existent. The transition from white to black is effected through the successive degrees, which are the species of colour. The substratum, of which these are the qualities, is one, and is in strictness that which is changed ; the colours alternate. Colour is not purely subjective ; it is true that it depends upon the eye, but it also depends upon the object. Actual colour depends upon the possibilities of these two being realised together, but the coloured object existed in nature HISTORICAL THEORIES OF VISION 173 as a potential colour before the act of vision, and apart from it. " It is light that at once transforms the potential colour to actuality, and the potentially seeing to an actually seeing eye."^ In the colour scale (as among the elements) there is a sort of opposition of positive and negative. White is the positive, black, the negative. This is Aristotle's general doctrine of colours ; he also treats certain of them in detail. The presence of some fire-like element is the cause of light in the diaphanous, and in its absence we have darkness. In all determinately bounded bodies we may assume some- thing analogous with the presence and absence of this fiery element. Its absence means blackness, its presence white- ness. Therefore, in determinately bounded bodies, blackness is privation of whiteness. Blackness and whiteness are con- traries within one sensory province, that of colour, and from them all the other colours are to be explained. " The transition from white to black is possible through continuous degrees of privation: that from white to black is likewise possible by an ascending scale in the opposite direction. The various colours are species which fall between the two contraries, and' are generated of certain combinations of these." ^ For instance, in passing from white to black we first come to crimson. As the intervening stages in the pas- sage mark relative extremes, change can start from any point. With regard to the actual mode of origin of the inter- mediate colours, what is actually effected in the above- mentioned process ? Aristotle's views on this point are not quite consistent in his different writings, but he appears to condemn the doctrine of atomic juxtaposition and that of superposition, taking the view that a complete blending occurs: by blending is meant a compound or mixture of so intimate a nature that no individual part retains its original qualities unmodified. The colour spoken of as grey is sometimes described as standing at the mid-point of the , scale between black and white, sometimes as a sort of relative black. Golden- yellow is also nearly akin to white. Red is produced by ' Beare, op. cit., p. 64. " Ibid., p. 69. 174 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES light streaming through black, as when the sun shines through a fog. Purple is akin to crimson, but differs from it in having more of the dark constituent. " Sometimes the light of a lamp shows not white but purple, the ray that is sent from it being feeble, and being reflected from a dark colour. This increasing feebleness of the ray brings us from purple to leek-green and violet successively. The stronger ray yields crimson against the dark ground (or when mixed with dark); the next in strength gives leeJe- green; the weakest, violet."^ In this connection, Aristotle refers to positive and nega- tive after-images. After looking at the sun and closing the eyes we see the object first of the same colour as before ; this changes to crimson, then to purple, then to black, and finally vanishes. The order illustrates the genesis of colours from the blending of white and black. Simultaneous con- trast receives an explanation along these lines — the brightest rainbow is seen in the darkest cloud, white wool has its colour intensified when placed next black wool, etc. Aris- totle rejected altogether the theory of emanations and pores, while his conception of a vibratile movement imparted to the actualised "diaphanous" may, perhaps, be regarded as a partial anticipation of the modern doctrine of a lumini- ferous aether. We cannot, however, push the comparison far, since he maintained, in opposition to Empedocles, that light does not travel. The eye itself is like other organs to be defined in terms of its function, and thus is an eye only so long as it can see. The eye of a corpse is so called only in a somewhat incorrect way. The eye is the organ which is stimulated by colour, but this process of stimulation must in some way be transmitted to the " soul." It seems to follow that the diaphanous medium which acts objectively is also functioning within the eye itself in order to transmit the stimulation inwards. The eye consists of heterogeneous parts; that part which is specially concerned in vision is the part generally translated pupil, but probably, at least after the time of Empedocles, meaning rather the crystalline ' Beare, op. cit., p. 75. HISTORICAL THEORIES OF VISION 175 lens. Surrounding this internal moist part comes what Aristotle terms the black (? iris), and outside of this again is the white (? sclerotic). The pupil and vision are to the eye what body and soul are respectively in the whole living creature.^ For perfect vision there must be a proper amount of moisture in the eye. Those creatures which have too little see well by night but ill by day, because owing to the deficiency the eyes are over-stimulated by daylight. The other group, with too much moisture, can see well by day- light but badly in the night, because there is not relatively to the water enough fire in the eye. The membrane which covers the "pupil" must be transparent, white, thin, and smooth. Aristotle rejects the theory that the eye consists of fire, a theory which, as we have noticed, rests on the obser- vation of phosphenes (flashes of light seen when the eye is rapidly moved or when it is pressed). If, asks Aristotle, the eye is igneous, why do we not always see these phos- phenes, instead of only under exceptional circumstances ? Further, why does not the eye see itself always ? Besides, if the visual part of the eye were really fire, we ought to be able to see in the dark, and Plato's explanation of the circumstance that we cannot see in the dark, viz. an ex- tinction of the visual ray in the darkness, is inadmissible. Such fire as is made with coals may indeed be extinguished by cold and moisture, but not light. The previous pages will be sufiicient to give the reader some idea of the nature of Greek thought respecting the visual processes. A few general remarks may fittingly con- clude the chapter. For the reasons mentioned at the beginning, the state- ments respecting the anatomical and physical side of the problem of sight are of little interest. It cannot be said that even Aristotle's view of the matter is particularly helpful. It will have been noticed how the dogma of like producing like hampered work on this side, already suffi- ciently difficult, and the value of Miiller's doctrine {vide supra, ^ See Beare, op. cit., p. 80. 176 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES Chapter I.) receives a practical illustration. Since the external medium is transparent, there must be some internal trans- parency ; since " fire " is visible, there must be some internal "fire" by which it is perceived, and so on. Anaxagoras might appear to be an exception, but a little thought con- vinces one that the exception is only formal. In adopting the converse of the dogma, like produces like, he too stands committed to the belief that there is some necessary con- nection in kind between the processes occurring within us and those conceptually existent outside of us. The theories of vision as a problem in physiological psychology, on the other hand, stand on quite a different plane. As we shall see in the chapter on Hering's theory of visual sensations, Aristotle's teaching was the basis of Goethe's theory of colours, and the doctrine of Goethe, freed from certain mathematico-physical absurdities, partly a result of imperfect physical knowledge, has been transformed by Hering into a hypothesis of rare ingenuity and intellectual value. To pursue the history of visual theories from tho Greek period to the modern epoch would be of little more than antiquarian interest. In another chapter I shall refer to the work of Berkeley in connection with the theory of space ; his contribution to the theory of colour vision in the strict sense is not of much importance from the physiological standpoint. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY There ia a considerable literature dealing with the sense physiology and psychology of the ancients, but it is mainly written from the stand- point of the psychologist or that of the philologist. The reader will find ample material for extending his knowledge in the work of Professor Beare, to which I have frequently called attention. Jowett's trans- lation of Plato's Dialogues can be profitably consulted for the earlier speculations. The standard work on Aristotle is Aristotles ueber die Farben, von 0. Prant, Miinchen, 1849. CHAPTER XVIII THE YOUNG-HELMHOLTZ THEOKY OF COLOUE VISION As I attempted to show in tlie chapter on normal vision, Newton's researches demonstrating the (conceptually) com- plex nature of white light and the physical substratum of chromatic stimuli — as co-ordinated by the undulatory hypo- thesis — enable us to form a coherent, but not necessarily, or even probably final, account of the physical elements in- volved in retinal stimulation. On the other hand, the specificity of physiological response, which finds a not quite accurate expression in Miiller's law, releases us from many of the difficulties attendant upon the primitive theories of vision, the propounders of which were hampered by the dogma of physico-physiological identity. The acceptance of .these facts has left wide scope for ingenuity in the con- struction of theories as to the psycho-physiological mechanism which links together the subjective and objective visual worlds; but the liberty of choice which is thus accorded to the scientific imagination has its own disadvantages. Perhaps the most obvious is the impossibility in which we stand of " proving " — in the more popular sense of that much abused word — any theory of colour vision to be true or false. So long as it was held that a conceptual system of physics was a valid physiological and psychological system, that our physiological categories were to be tested against our physical classifications, it was comparatively easy to judge and condemn a theory of vision. We now hold that the writ of the physical court does not run in the kingdom of physiology, and we do not know to what jurisdiction a physiological theory of colour vision is amenable. This is one of the reasons, in fact the chief reason, why an enormous number of rival theories are in the field, and why their study has seemed to 1" M 178 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES many highly educated men as unprofitable as the mediseval disputes of the schoolmen. To those who have the leisure, and the particular form of intellectual curiosity which is attracted to the study of human ingenuity divorced from the immediate phenomena of nature, a complete history of modern theories of vision would be instructive and valuable ; but by those Avhose leisure is less ample, and whose curiosity does not take this form, a different path must be followed. One must consider the theories of vision which, it may be, rather from the energy of their propounders than from any overwhelming speculative merit in the theories them- selves, have borne the most fruit in respect of experimental observations and methods. Adopting this criterion, two theories stand out above the rest, those of Helmholtz and Hering. If, therefore, these theories only are discussed in the present work, it is not because I hold them to be necessarily' superior as theories to others — for instance, those of Ladd-Franklin and Schenck — but because they have borne so much more experimental fruit. In a famous passage, R. L. Stevenson describes a conversation. " What I advance," said one, " is true." " Yes," replied the other, " but not the whole truth." " Sir," was the retort, " there is no such thing as the whole truth." In what follows, the reader should have that aphorism constantly in his mind. In attempting to arrive at an adequate interpretation of any class of experimental facts, it is permissible to advance by more than one route, just as many problems in geometry can be solved by analytical as by pure geometrical methods. As a rule, it does not much matter which path we choose, so long as we keep count of any assumptions made and avoid the introduction of unnecessary steps. We shall now start from the experimental laws of colour mixing and see whither we arrive by following the most obvious route. We found that for most experimental purposes, we could say that a sensation " produced by " a colour stimulus could be matched by a sensation due to a stimulus obtained by mixing not more than three lights together. We saw that these three lights did not coincide accurately with any spectral colours, but that if we admitted negative values into our colour YOUNG-HELMHOLTZ ON COLOUR VISION 179 equations, our vision could be regarded as definitely tri- chromatic even in terms of known stimuli. In order, how- ever, to avoid this, let us so choose our stimuli that only positive values of each are employed. This means that they must be so chosen that the colour " triangle " is circumscribed by the lines joining their representative points in a plane, e.g. Red/i- iWolet' Fia. 22. This is our first assumption, and is little more than a generalisation of experimental facts. Thus far, we have merely asserted that a stimulus R', say, can be expressed by the equation R' = a;.R+2/.G-)-2:.V, where x, y, z are real positive quantities. But our only measure of equality of stimulation being corresponding equality of sensations, we imply (and this is our second and most important assumption) that there is a definite relation- ship between the physiological excitatory processes which lead up to sensations and the stimulus magnitudes. This assumption is justified if we can show that it leads to the formulation of a useful working hypothesis, and if we assume the simplest relationship consistent with the ex- perimental facts. What, then, is the simplest relation we can suppose to subsist between the stimulatory and ex- citatory processes ? Clearly that just as stimuli may be reduced to terms of three independent variables, excitatory processes are represented by three independent variables. Thus, taking our previous example, any stimulus R' = a;.R + 2/.G + zN, then A =f^{x, y,z),B =f^(x, y,z),C =f^{x, y, z), and conversely, x = Y^[K B, C), y = ^lK B, C), z^Y^{K B, C). 180 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES These latter expressions may be taken as " elements " or unit excitatory processes, or any linear function of them may be so taken.^ The conception is merely that three independent physio- logical processes exist, each of which is defined by a func- tional equation connecting it with the three independent- stimulus values which, as we have seen, measure the effect of any given stimulus. This statement contains the fundamental part of the hypothesis first sketched by Thomas Young and elaborated by Helmholtz. It is important to distinguish the essentials of the theory from its subsidiary parts. The effect produced by any chromatic stimulus is sup- posed to depend upon changes set up in three independent " substances," nothing being postulated with respect to them except that the magnitude of change in each is a function, i.e. depends on the proportions of three independently variable stimuli in terms of which the given stimulus can be ex- pressed. Conversely, any given stimulus value is a function of the independent activities of three visual "substances." The nature of the " substances '' from a physiological point of view, also the exact relations between them and the stimuli, are left undiscussed. For convenience of illustration, Young and Helmholtz assumed that the activity of each substance was associated with a single colour sensation, and chose red, green, and violet as " elementary sensations " from the present stand- point, " Substance " A, when stimulated, was supposed to give rise to the sensation of red ; B, under similar conditions, to that of green ; and C, to that of violet or violet-blue ; in this way the well-known "valency curves" of the text-books were obtained. The advantage of this method is that the theory seems more definite, but the disadvantage is entailed that if the illustration proves irreconcilable with facts of experiment, the reader omits to notice that what is found wanting is merely an illustration, not the basal theory. It is especially important to remember that the colour operations discussed in Chapter XIV. do not pretend to describe any ■ For a proof of this statement, see Helmholtz, Handh. d. Physiol, Opt., second edition, pp. 342-3. The section beginning on p. 341 should be closely studied. YOUNG-HELMHOLTZ ON COLOUR VISION 181 direct relation between the hypothetical "substances" and stimulus magnitudes. In precisely the same way, for the sake of illustration, Helmholtz suggested the existence somewhere in the retino- cerebral apparatus of three sets of fibres, each corresponding to one of the hypothetical "substances." This suggestion was less happy because its utility was not so great as, and the chance of misunderstanding greater than, in the pre- vious case. The red, green, and violet fibres have been persistently misunderstood; it is time to remember that they are pure abstractions, no more essential to the theory just sketched than is the employment in algebra of the letter x to denote an unknown quantity an essential pro- cedure in that science. To avoid this fertile source of misunderstanding I shall use Professor v. Kries' term, Components, and speak of the theory as the " Three Com- ponents Hypothesis." Another mistake is illustrated by the following quotation from a paper by Miss Calkins : i — "From the point of view of a psychological analysis of our conscious sensations of colour, the postulates of this theory are not in accordance with the facts of observation ; for, even granting that violet is a simple fundamental colour sensation (which many observers regard as complex), it can hardly be denied that yellow is just as well characterised and definite a sensation as red or green. Yellow looks yellow, and does not seem at all like a mixture of red and green, or indeed any other colour mixture." Objections of this type are, I think, irrelevant. The theory is only an attempt to express physiological processes in terms of experimental facts, i.e. of stimulus values; it has nothing to do with the psychological analysis of sensa- tions. Whether such an analysis can be performed is a question which must be decided by psychologists, it is no part of our inquiry. We are only concerned with sensations in so far as they are the signs of the existence of physio- logical changes. Having reduced the hypothesis to its simplest terms, let ' Arcli. f. Pkysiologie, Supp. Vol., 1902, pp. 244 et seq. 182 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES us apply it to the facts resumed in earlier chapters. That it describes normal visual systems is, of course, apparent, since they form its starting point. Abnormal trichromatic systems are satisfactorily described if we suppose one of the visual components to be defined in a peculiar way. Thus, if a component A is normally defined as f[(x, y, z), in these cases it is some other function, /[(as, y, z), say, of the stimulus values. Turning to dichromatic systems, we have seen that, from the experimental standpoint, they are reduction forms of a normal system. Theoretically, the simplest reduction we could imagine would be the absence or ineffectiveness of one of the three normal components. Thus if we take R, G, V (for the sake of clearness) as the normal components, in the absence of R all sensations (sensation being used with the meaning above defined) are functions of G and V only> a condition approximcding to that of protanopes. If G be absent and R present, we get a form resembling deuteranopia. Under such conditions, not only would the relation between dichromatics and trichromatics be easily intelligible, but we could determine from observations on dichromatics the components of a normal system, or, more precisely, the stimuli which act exclusively upon such components. A stimulus inoperative upon a dichromatic eye must act exclusively upon the missing component. We have learned (Chapter XV.) how to find the position of such a stimulus in the colour triangle (the " Fehlpunkt "), and the two such points obtained for the two systems of dichromatic vision deter- mine the stimulus relations of two visual components in the normal eye. All this depends, however, on the assumption that one component is absent and everything else unchanged in a dichromatic eye. Modern work renders it more than doubt- ful whether we may make this assumption. In 1885, when the second edition of his classical treatise was being pre- pared, Helmholtz wrote as follows to Lord Rayleigh: "I have never doubted that our colour system depended on three variables, and no more. In regard to colour- YOUNG-HELMHOLTZ ON COLOUR VISION 183 blindness, the recent observations of Donders and of my assistant, Dr. A. Koenig, show that this defect cannot be referred simply to the lack of one of the fundamental colours, but that two of the primaries (red and green) appear to acquire a more even distribution in the spectrum, so that now one and now the other makes a more vigorous im- pression; in other words, the resulting curve approximates now more to the red and now to the green sensation. In addition to this we have every shade of lessened power of discrimination. Consequently different individuals re- quire very different mixtures of lithium and thallium light in order to make up sodium light." ^ But if we cannot regard dichromatic vision as differing from the normal merely in the absence of a component, we can, in terms of the fundamental hypothesis, assert that it de- pends upon a visual system made up of two variables defined by such expressions as A' = Fj (x, y, z) and B' = F2 {x, y, z). This way of looking at the matter, although consistent and logical enough, is not free from objection. We could not deduce from such expressions any definite statements re- specting the components of normal systems ; the description is too general to admit of detailed verification. To put the matter in a nutshell, the older conception of partial colour-blindness as due to the absence of one normal com- ponent is simple and, if true, practical, but is not, in fact, quite adequate ; the more general statement is adequate, but not very helpful. Next, what has the Three Components Hypothesis to say with regard to the phenomena of after- images ? A noteworthy feature of after-image experiments is, of course, that stimulation with a given light increases respon- siveness to its complementary. It would appear, therefore, easy to imagine that activity of the three components, or any one or two of them, in a certain way diminishes their responsiveness in one direction, increasing it in another direction. This amounts to supposing that we have a condition comparable with the state of the reflex arcs, so brilliantly described by Sherrington; the nervous path is - 1 Eolingsberger's Life of Hdmholtz, English trans., Oxford, 1907, p. 436. 184 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES occupied by one form of motor discharge, and this very occupancy paves the way for a discharge different, and even opposite, in kind. To so highly general a statement as this no objection will be found, but if we investigate details, difficulties arise. Eor instance, the apparent saturation of spectral colours is greatly enhanced by previously stimulating the eye with their complementaries. Helmholtz accordingly supposed that all the spectral colours act on each visual component. But if this be true, the simpler interpretation of colour-blindness once more fails. Observations of dichromatics suggest that lights having wave lengths greater than 550 /^/^ do not affect the third component (the " blue " or " violet ") at all, because no standard blue had to be mixed with the standard red in order to effect a good match with colours in this part of the spectrum. To a normal eye, however, the satu- ration of spectral yellow (589 /^/i) is unquestionably en- hanced by previous exposure to blue. Either spectral colours do not affect all three components, in which case the theory does not cover after-image efifects, or the simpler explanation of partial colour-blindness must be abandoned. In view of what has already been said, the reader will perhaps agree that the second alternative is the more plausible, and con- clude that after-images are adequately described at the cost of strengthening our suspicion that dichromatic and trichro- matic systems cannot be co-ordinated in any simple manner. So far we have found that the Hypothesis of Three Components describes with sufficient clearness the facts of normal colour vision, including the phenomena of after- images; that it also describes the facts of partial colour- blindness, but in very general terms, the earlier direct explanation being insufficient. We have next to see whether any experimental evidence can be found pointing to the existence of independent visual components satisfying the conditions of the theory or conceivably capable of so satis- fying them. Evidence of this kind has been afforded by the experi- ments of G. J. Burch. This observer exposed his eye to direct sunlight in the focus of a 2-inch lens behind coloured YOUNG-HELMHOLTZ ON COLOUR VISION 185 glasses. A gelatine film stained with magenta and com- bined with a medium ruby glass was found to transmit a fairly pure red, three thicknesses of green glass were used for green, and a tank of cupric ammonia-sulphate for violet. Similar arrangements were made for the other hues, and in some experiments a large spectroscope was employed. Two minutes' exposure was sufficient to produce the maximal effect in the case of red. After exposure to red light, the following effects were noticed. Scarlet geraniums appeared black, calceolarias and sunflowers green, purple flowers, such as clematis, violet. Pink roses were sky-blue. Fatiguing with violet light caused objects reflecting violet light to appear black, purples and reds seemed crimson. Green stimulation made the foliage appear reddish or bluish-grey. After these exposures, "the colour by which the eye has been dazzled, and to which it is now blind, tint all those objects which naturally reflect none of this." This state- ment is illustrated by a simple experiment of Burch's. Suppose the eye to be somewhat fatigued by green, as during a long summer walk in the country, if the eye be directed to a small red spot on a black surface, e.g. a geranium petal on the black cover of a book, and one walks quickly with it into a dark shed or barn, the colour of the petal changes from red through orange and yellow, becoming eventually, perhaps, whitish. On coming into the light again the red reappears. These interesting experiments suggest that stimulation with red, green, and violet alters responsiveness with respect to these stimuli alone, and the same is the case with blue. Orange stimulation, on the other hand, affects not only the appearance of the orange, but that of the red and the green as well. Both positive and negative effects pass off rapidly in the case of artificial red-blindness (in ten minutes), more slowly after violet fatigue (in two hours). It is, I think, obvious that the state of affairs presented by these experiments is highly complex. We are dealing with a change in responsiveness analogous to the retuning effects of Chapter XVI., but of greater magnitude and in less simple form. Take the experiment quoted as to the 186 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES apparent hue of a geranium petal after green stimulation ; this is an ordinary after-image effect, and differs in no way from the results obtained by other workers ; in the case of exposure to stronger green light, the effect is similar. How does this, we may ask, differ from the experiment with intense orange light ? Simply in the fact that " retuning " with orange affects responsiveness to red and green as well, so that they, like orange itself, will cause the production of negative after-images. The conclusion that in this case the mechanism involved in the production of orange is com- pounded of a mechanism yielding a sensation of redness and a mechanism responding with a sensation of greenness is reason3,ble, and finds confirmation in a recent experiment performed by Burch. We know that responsiveness to green is increased, re^ latively to that for red stimulation, by resting the eye in darkness; hence if orange or yellow depends upon a fusion of two physiological processes, one concerned with green, the other with red, then, under conditions of feeble illumina- tion and dark adaptation, the yellow should appear greenish, because the mechanism responding by a sensation of green^ ness is more active under these conditions than that asso- ciated in the same way with redness. One speaks of a physiological process responding by the production of a sen- sation purely for brevity ; it is not psychologically accurate, but the psychological reader is not likely to be misled. Burch found the result to be as expected — "the sodium lines appeared pale green when of the minimum visible intensity." These results do, therefore, support a contention that components in the sense of our theory may possibly have a physiological counterpart. I do not think, however, that the view that four components — a red, a green, a violet, and a blue — exist is proved by Burch's experiments, valuable though these are. To prove that any light acts upon only one component it would be necessary to show that after dazzling with, e.g., blue, any mixed light was altered by the subtrac- tion of blue, and that any light not containing blue had that colour added to it. The facts that, the condition is YOUNG-HELMHOLTZ ON COLOUE VISION 187 transitory is perhaps attended with some risk, and almost certainly involves psychological complications, render exact observations difficult. Under the circumstances it is per- haps best to say that although the experiments are perfectly consistent with a component hypothesis of the type dis- cussed, it would be rash, on the strength of them, to make any general statement as to the nature of the components from the physiological standpoint. Theoretically, it does not matter whether we adopt three or four components ; the algebraical form of our theory would not be changed, but we should lose the practical advantages of considering normal colour vision to be, experimentally, trichromatic, which would be a serious objection. Before finally summarising the case presented, two matters need attention. First, as to monochromatic vision or total colour-blindness. It has been asserted that such a condition cannot be described in terms of the Young-Helmholtz theory. As a matter of fact, the assertion is inaccurate ; symbolically we could cover the facts by supposing that the functions defining the variables are identical, thus : A = /^(x, y, z) = B = f^[x, y, z) = G = f^{x, y, z), or graphically we can put it that, the three valency curves coincide. In any case, the reader will have probably seen reason to think that mono- chromatic vision depends upon a mechanism entirely dis- tinct from the precursors of normal foveal vision, and that its treatment should be kept separate from that of the phenomena with which we are here concerned. In the second place, no reference has been made to Simul- taneous Contrast. The reason is, that it seems doubtful whether the phenomena of simultaneous contrast are not of quite a special kind. It is true that the original hypothesis of Helmholtz, which assumed that contrastive effects are dependent upon factors purely psychological in nature, can hardly be maintained without some modification, but I shall reserve a discussion of the point for a later chapter. It must be said, however, that if subsequent work should compel us to assign a purely physiological basis to the facts of simul- taneous contrast, it will probably be necessary to modify the theory of components in such a way that it will become some- 188 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES what more complicated than it is at present. Leaving this matter for further consideration, we can say that the com- ponent hypothesis associated with the names of Young and Helmholtz supposes (1) that colour sensations depend upon the activity of three independent physiological substances of unknown nature and situation ; (2) that the relationship be- tween these components and the complex of stimuli is ex- pressible quantitatively by saying that the responsiveness of each component is measured by a real linear function of three standard stimuli; (3) the results of stimulating these components are unit sensations in a purely physio- logical sense, not units of consciousness; (4) no spectral light acts upon only one of the components. The theory describes with sufficient accuracy the main facts, and there is some direct experimental evidence — that of Burch — which is consistent with its truth. The main objection to the hypothesis in its modern form is its highly general nature and want of direct applicability to the im- mediate data of physiological and physical research. How far this is a real objection may be a matter of discussion, but it at least inclines one to examine those theories which are, in the colloquial phrase, less up in the clouds. Such an examination will be the object of our next chapter. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY Helmholtz's treatise should, of course, be the first work consulted. The reader should then turn to v. Kries' article in Nagel's Handb. The following papers of Burch describe his experiments : — (1) Phil. Trans., B., vol. cxci. pp. 1-34. (2) Proc. Roy. Soc, vol. Ixvi. pp. 216-219. (3) Proc. Roy. Soc, B., 1905, p. 214. CHAPTEE XIX HERING'S THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS In the last chapter I attempted to trace out the theoretical consequences developed by Young and Helmholtz from the experimental facts of colour-mixing. It will have been clear that the whole web of deductions depended from the fact that, in general, the eifect of a given stimulus or combination of stimuli was constant, so that we might attribute to the stimulus a causal value. In other words, we have regarded the sensations of colour as signs or differentice of processes initiated by the stimuli ; the resulting theory was accordingly not in any real sense a theory of visual sensations, but one of visual stimuli. If the estimate I formed of the value of this process be in any measure just, it would seem to follow that its weakness lay rather in what was left unsaid than in any positive error. One cannot but notice in the theory as at present advanced a certain dryness, a detachment from one's experience of colour on the subjective side, which is repellent. All this possibly amounts to saying that the theory is not very simple nor very direct, objections which can be urged against many theories of wider intellectual value than that of Young and Helmholtz in other fields of thought, objections which are not by any means fatal. Recognising the existence of these difficulties, however, it is well to see whether we can obtain a more satisfactory solution of our problem by approaching it in a somewhat different way. This chapter will be devoted to the study of another pro- posed solution of the matter, that of Ewald Hering. This other line of investigation practically resumes the pro- blem at the point at which the Greeks left it, and dates from the publication in 1810 of Goethe's Farbenlehre. The com- paratively slight direct influence of this work on the develop- 190 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES ment of modern physiological thought respecting the nature of physiological processes is due to causes well worthy of attention. The physical analysis of white light into mono- chromatic constituents by Newton had naturally attracted the chief, almost the exclusive, attention of those who occupied themselves with the study of colour vision. Goethe, however, with characteristic intellectual insight saw that the difficulties of the problem were not to be overcome by vague references to physical experiments. He saw that the problem was one of sensations, and he approached it from the sensational standpoint. Had he contented himself with this, with an analysis of sensations of colour, his work must have had an enormous influence; but he went further. The prevailing tendency to over-estimate the significance of the physical side of the question led Goethe into the opposite error. He sustained the thesis that the Newtonian analysis was physically incorrect, and that the alleged decomposition of white light was not in general possible. Consequently, much of his work is devoted to an attack upon the Newtonian doctrine from the physical side, an attack which signally failed. Unfortunately the failure of this attack involved more valuable parts of Goethe's work in discredit, and his book is not well known even to professional physiologists. I can best give an idea of the valuable parts by quoting a few passages which are specially relevant for us. "With regard to the German terminology, it has the advantage of possessing four monosyllabic names no longer to be traced to their origin, viz. yellow (Gelb), blue, red, green. They represent the most general idea of colour to the imagination, without reference to any very specific modi- fication. If we were to add two other qualifying terms to each of these four, as thus, red-yellow and yellow-red, red- blue and blue-red, yellow-green and green-yellow, blue- green and green-blue, we should express the gradations of the chromatic circle with sufficient distinctness; and if we were to add' the designations of light and dark, and again define, in some measure, the degree of purity or its opposite by the monosyllables black, white, grey, brown, we should have a tolerably sufficient range of expressions to HEEING'S THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS 191 describe the ordinary appearances presented to us, without troubling ourselves whether they were produced dynamically or atomically." ^ " Considered in a general point of view, colour is deter- mined towards one of two sides. It thus presents a contrast which we call a polarity, and which we may fitly designate by the expressions plus and minus. Plus. Minus. Yellow. Blue. Action. Negation. Light. Shadow. Brightness. Darkness. Force. Weakness. Warmth. Coldness. Proximity. Distance, Repulsion. Attraction. Affinity with acids. Affinity with alkalis." ^ We see here formulated the conception of certain colour sensations as occupying unique places in our sensational field, and presenting also, as it were, a species of sensational contrast one with another. Of course in many respects this idea is an old one, and has perhaps always been realised by painters. In a dialogue on colours by Ludovico Dolce, published in 1565, the fol- lowing passage occurs : " He who wishes to combine colours that are agreeable to the eye will put grey next dusky orange, yellow-green next rose colour, blue next orange, dark purple, black, next dark green, white next black, and white next flesh colour." ^ Titian, according to his biographer Ridolfi, Avas fond of opposing red and blue to his flesh tints, and Rubens con- trasted a bright red with his "still cooler flesh colour" (Eastlake). Here, as elsewhere, the greatest art is to conceal the art. A study of the works of Rembrandt, whose skill in con- trastive effects has never been equalled, reveals the fact that 1 Goethe's Theory of Colours, Eastlake's translation, London, 1840, pp. 243-4. ' Goethe, op. oil., p. 276. ' Ibid., Note C. 192 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES the most striking effects are produced by light-dark contrasts rather than specific colour oppositions. The remarkable specific brightness {vide infra) of yellow is, however, well seen in the (so-called) " Nachtwache " ; the extraordinarily vivid effect produced by the yellow dress of one of the central figures is balanced by general shadow, without, so far as I could judge, any obvious use of the " Gegenfarbe." These remarks seem to me to apply also to the " Staalmeesters." I hope that some day a physiologist with a competent know- ledge of art will undertake a study of the master works of art from the standpoint of the physiology of vision. The point to mark in the preceding passages is the general agreement that certain of our sensations of colour are really singled out from the whole group as presenting sharply defined, special characters. All sensational theories are primarily concerned with the definition of these characters, and secondarily with an attempt to describe the data in terms of a physiological hypothesis. Of such attempts the views developed by Professor Ewald Hering of Leipzig and his pupils during the last five-and-thirty years are the most valuable results. Whatever may be our ultimate conclusion as to the validity of these theories, no one can doubt that they have greatly advanced our knowledge of visual physi- ology, and their study cannot be neglected by any one desirous of acquiring even a superficial idea of modern conceptions. Whenever I come upon a new writer dealing with colour vision, I turn to his account of Hering's theories ; if he dis- misses them as " obviously " incorrect or absurd, I am con- fident that his own contribution to the subject is a very small one. According to Hering's method of analysis, our whole visual world can be resolved into six elementary qualities of sensation ; white, black, the toneless, and blue, yellow, green, red, the toned or bright (hunte) colours. If one considers the tone-free qualities, they can be arranged to form a series of shades or gradations passing from the intensest white to the deepest black. If one attends to the toned colours, they can be arranged in a circle with four divisions. " If we choose in such a colour circle any colour as starting HEEING'S THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS 193 point — for instance, a red similar to that with which a spectrum usually begins at the long- waved end — we see the red colours arranged in one direction gradually becoming more yellowish, while the redness of the colours correspondingly, diminishes, until finally, passing through orange and golden-yellow, we arrive at a yellow which contains no trace of the red which is still so apparent in the orange. To this yellow succeed other yellow colours which play more and more into the green (sulphur-yellow, canary-yellow) ; further on (as in sap- green) the yellowishness recedes more and more behind the steadily increasing greenishness, until we finally reach a green which seems to be entirely free from yellow. To this succeed green colours which already play into blue (water- green); further on the bluishness of the colours becomes increasingly stronger, the greenishness weaker, until we finall}' reach a blue exhibiting no more greenishness at all. To this blue succeed blue colours of increasing reddishness and correspondingly diminishing bluishness (blue-violet, red- violet, purple-red), until the last trace of bluishness vanishes in a definite red." ^ If we define a pure green as a sensation free from ad- mixture with that of blue and yellow, and the other three sensation qualities in the same manner, we see that our pure colours (from this point of view) can be arranged in two pairs, yellow and blue forming one, and red and green the other. The members of each pair can be placed oppo- site one another in a diagram, because we can only pass from yellow to blue or from red to green by traversing the province of a member of the other pair, as just explained. There is no pure yellowish-blue or reddish-green sensation quality. But there is yet another contrast, from the standpoint of sensation quality, between yellow and red on the one hand, and blue and green on the other. Somehow, in a manner diffi- cult to express in words, yet of universal experience, the two former colours are associated with a certain heightening and increased vividness of sensation tone. This finds its ex- ^ Ewald Hering, Griindziige der Lehre vom Zichtsinn, Leipzig', 19C5, p. 41. 194 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES pression in the classification by artists of colour into warm and cold. Goethe has emphasised these points : — "We find from experience again that yellow excites a warm and agreeable impression. Hence in painting it be- longs to the illumined and emphatic side. "This impression of warmth may be experienced in a Yery lively and emphatic manner if we look at a landscape through a yellow glass, particularly on a grey winter's day. The eye is gladdened, the heart expanded and cheered, a glow seems at once to breathe towards us." ^ Of blue he says : " This colour has a peculiar and almost indescribable effect upon the eye. As a hue it is powerful, but it is on the negative side, and in its highest purity is, as it were, a stimulating negation. Its appearance, then, is a kind of contradiction between excitement and repose. " Rooms which are hung with pure blue appear in some degree larger, but at the same time empty and cold. "The appearance of objects seen through a blue glass is gloomy and melancholy." ^ In the opinion of Hering, the facts are most satisfactorily described by saying that in the sensation-complex blue and green produce a darkening and yellow or red a brightening effect ; the toneless colours, black and white, also contribute respectively in a negative or positive sense to the sum-total of effects. We have, therefore, the brightness of a colour defined in strictly sensational terms. We have now reached the conception of six primary sensation qualities arranged in three pairs — white-refl, red- green, yellow-blue. The first member in each case increases, the second diminishes the subjective intensity or brightness of a sensation complex of which it forms part.^ "The brightness or darkness of a toned (bunte) colour is, according to this view, the result of the inherent bright- ness or darkness (Eigenhell vmd Eigendunlcel) of its consti- tuent pure colours, which as the pure constituents of that colour agreeably to their respective distinctness determine 1 Goethe, op. cit., p. 307. ^ Ibid., p. 310. ^ Seusation-eomplex is a term used merely to indicate the supposed multi- plicity of infra-conscious representatives, not in reference to perception. HERING'S THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS 195 the quality of the colour. In any colour really existent for us is a definite inherent degree of brightness and darkness, and in accordance with whether the brightness or the dark- ness be the more distinct, we call the colour bright or dark. " A toned colour may generally be regarded as made up of four primary components, two toned and two tone-free •(black and white). Only in colours of the tone of a pure colour is one toned constituent present by itself. In any red-yellow colour, e.g. orange, we have accordingly to dis- tinguish three bright, pure components (red, yellow, white), and one dark (black); but in any green-blue, three dark {green, blue, black) and one bright. The red-green and green- yellow colours would contain, however, two bright and two dark pure components. "From what has been said, the following rules can be deduced : — " If two colours of equal tone and equal purity differ in brightness, this is due to a difference in their black-white components. " Two colours differing in tone may, notwithstanding equal degrees of purity and equality as regards their black-white components, differ in brightness. " With equality of conditions as to the black- white com- ponents, a yellow, a red, or a yellow-red colour is so much the brighter, a blue, a green, or a blue green so much the darker the more distinct the colour tone in comparison with the black- white constituent." ^ This quotation describes what is often called the theory of the " Specific Brightness of Colours." The whole is, or claims to be, a faithful analysis of our visual sensations with- out reference to any hypothesis whatever. That this is a perfectly legitimate process I have already attempted to show ; the next step is to translate these facts, or supposed facts, into terms of a physiological hypothesis. Such a translation can be readily effected. It is supposed that somewhere in the retino-cerebral a,pparatus, in the infra-conscious sphere, four distinct sub- ^ Hering, op. cit., p. 61. 196 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES stances exist. Each of these substances can undergo a building up, or anabolic, and a breaking down, or katabolic, change. External stimuli will, depending on their natures, induce either an anabolic or a katabolic change in these substances, and are associated with definite sensations of colour. The building up of the black-white substance corre- sponds to a sensation of blackness, its breaking down to a sensation of whiteness ; anabolism of the red-green sub- stance is associated with green colouration, its katabolism with red colouration ; similarly in the third substance yellow is katabolic in origin, blue anabolic. Before discussing these views in detail, I must warn the reader against some popular misconceptions. Certain opponents have asserted or suggested that the facts upon which Hering founded his theory differ in some perverse way from those data which are ordinarily called facts of experiment. This is not the case. The facts the hypothesis attempts to describe are as legitimately objects of inquiry as any others within the purview of physiological science. It is further to be noted that the four physiological " sub- stances" have just as much and just as little real existence as the three components of our other theory. It is idle to say that the postulated anabolic and katabolic processes are essentially unlike any chemical mechanisms with which we are acquainted. It is equally vain to object that stimulation processes in animate nature are to all appearance bound up with katabolic changes; this would only be a valid objection if we attempted to identify the hypothetical substances with any known retino-cerebral constituent. No such identifi- cation is attempted; the suggestion that Hering and his- school identify the black- white substance with visual purple is entirely unjustified. The fact is, that this theory can only be judged on the grounds of scientific expediency. Does. Hering's method give us a better account of the phenomena of colour vision than that based upon stimulus relations ? This is the only question worth the physiologist's while to answer. One notices directly that the hypothesis, in the form in. which it has just been presented, offers two considerable^ BERING'S THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS 197 advantages. Firstly, it deals with the immediate data of vision, the sensations of colour, directly, not merely in terms of stimulation magnitudes. In the second place, it is essenti- ally easy to comprehend, which cannot, perhaps, be said of the components' hypothesis. Let us first of all attempt to express the facts of successive contrast, after-images, in terms of this hypothesis. After resting the eye on a white object we should expect, under certain conditions, a positive, under others a negative after-effect, and experiment agrees with theory. For example, if the eye be stimulated with green light, anabolism will occur in the red-green substance, an anabolism which will lead to the formation of a large quantity of "material." If, now, red light stimulates the retina it produces not only katabolism of the normal quantity of substance, but the new formed material also falls to pieces, and the correlative sensation is greatly enhanced. That a positive after-image will sometimes be produced might be expected. After the stimulus is withdrawn, the anabolic (or katabolic) change induced by it will continue for some short space of time owing to a species of chemical inertia in the substance. In fact, all the obvious phenomena of after-images are well enough described in terms of Bering's theory ; it is when we come to the details that trouble arises. It was noticed by v. Kries that the responsiveness of the eye to monochromatic light was markedly altered by previous exposure to white. He found that the stimulus value of the red he employed was diminished in the ratio of about one to four by previously retuning the eye with white. The validity of v. Kries' conclusions has been contested by Bering, who objected that the colour used by v. Kries as a reactor (180° blue, 180° white on a disc) was not sufficiently saturated, and brings forward the following experiment : Two discs, A and B, are arranged. A consists of a black centre surrounded by a white ring, B of a centre composed of 120° blue and 240° black, encircled by a ring containing 356° blue and 4° white. A point upon the internal margin of the white ring in A is fixated for a given time, and the ex- perimenter then turns his eye to an exactly corresponding point in B. Bering always found that the outer ring of 198 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES B, under these conditions, looked more saturated than the centre. As the term saturated is not used by Hering in the physical sense, the exact bearing of his objection to v. Kries' experiment is not clear. Presumably he holds tha,t the " blueness " of v. Kries' disc was not distinctly separable from. its " whiteness," with the result that the apparent change in the latter on retuning with white was mistaken for a change in the former. If this objection is admissible, it is to be observed that in Bering's experiment the difference between the amounts of blue in reacting and comparison fields was so great that an enormous diminution in responsiveness over the retuned area would be necessary for the production of a good match between the centre and the periphery of B. It is contended that after white retuning a reacting blue and white can be made to match a pure blue in brightness by adding more white ; adding more blue will always make the reactor too saturated. Hering's experiment actually demon- strates that a diminution of some 66| per cent, in apparent intensity cannot be attained by retuning with the white he employed. That the apparent brightness was correspondingly reduced is, of course, evidence in his favour, but the difiiculties of specific brightness comparison are under such conditions not small. The conclusion of Hering's memoir is so important from the theoretical standpoint, that I quote it verbatim. " To the change of state experienced by an element of the somatic visual field when acted upon by, e.g., blue light, with which the blue sensation is associated, the whole somatic visual field reacts by a change in the opposite direction which corresponds to the oppositely coloured or yellow sensation, and any light that now falls on the retina acts, in consequence of this chromatic retuning of the visual field, as if its yellow valency were increased and its blue valency correspondingly diminished. This retuning is maximal in the immediate vicinity of the element acted upon by the blue light, and diminishes with its distance from the same. ... A white light falling on the neighbourhood of the region which has been stimulated with blue seems therefore more HERING'S THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS 199 or less yellowish, but a white light which. falls together with blue on the spot that has been stimulated with blue, see- ing that it behaves as a more or less yellow-valent light, neutralises the blue valency of the blue light so much the more the greater be the quantity of it mixed with the latter. This explains the striking fact that the chromatic quality of a saturated colour is so extremely quickly extinguished by increasing the amount of white mixed with it. . . . " When V. Kries, therefore, supposed that, according to the theory of opposite colours (Gegenfarben), the same result would be obtained from a fatigued and an unfatigued area if the same quantity of blue were allowed to fall on both, but in addition on the fatigued area a suitably chosen quantity of white light, he was in .error. In such a case the blue valency of the blue light at the unfatigued area is unaltered, since no other light is mixed with it ; over the fatigued area the blue valency of the blue light is partly neutralised by the admixture of white. Accordingly the blue at this latter area must appear less saturated than at the former. In fact, a transitory equality in brightness and saturation between the two areas is only obtained when white is indeed mixed with blue for the fatigued area, but, on the other hand, the blue light for the unfatigued area is suitably diminished. In general, for reasons already given, an equality in colour tone for blue can only be obtained under exceptional cir- cumstances when the character of the daylight is specially favourable, and the tone of the blue just right." ^ It is clear, therefore, that while white tuning is held by Hering not to affect colour valency, chromatic retuning does markedly affect white valency. In other words, what is, sensationally, a pure white has a definite colour valency ; pure Avhite, in terms of the physiological process in the white-black " substance," is hypothetical. We see again in Hering's theory the weak point we noticed in the hypothesis of Young and Helmholtz, viz. the necessity of complicating the theory in order to make it cover the facts, a necessity which brings the theory out of touch with the immediate 1 Hering, "Ueber die von der Farbenempfindlichkeit unabhangige Aen- derung der Weissempfindlichkeit," Pflilg. Arch., xolv. (1903), 533. 200 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES data of experience. It is rather interesting to notice tliat Hering's theory becomes more difiicult to follow by develop- ing in a direction opposite to that followed by the other theory ; the latter became unsatisfactory, to some, by tending to be too general, the former by multiplying its detailed sub-hypothesis. I cannot too strongly urge on the reader the considera- tion that such difficulties will always arise; they are bound up with the progress of knowledge. An explanation which was satisfactory at the date of its publication ceases to be so as the volume of scientific output in the subject of which it treats widens. In this sense all theories are creatures of a day. Hering's account of partial colour-blindness is subject to similar, but perhaps stronger, objections. He originally taught that in partial colour-blindness the red-green sub- stance was absent. In view of the fact that there are two forms of partial colour-blindness further explanation was necessary, and a most ingenious conception, a conception which in all probability contains at least some truth, was evolved. We have seen that trichromatic systems exist which differ appreciably from the normal in responsiveness to yellow and blue light. We have ako seen that pigmenta- tion of the retina and the lens influence appreciably colour matches. Hering suggested, and supported his suggestion by some observations on subjects of the two forms of trichromatic anomaly, that the two forms of partial colour- blindness were, in a sense, extreme forms of yellow and blue sightedness combined with extremely marked or extremely slight pigmentation. The protanopes would centre round the blue anomaly (relatively blue sighted), and the deuteranopes round the yellow anomaly (relatively yellow sighted). That the conception of continuous variation in colour sense is antecedently probable must be admitted; there is much reason to think that the occurrence of discontinuous varia- tion in biological characters is far less common than certain enthusiasts would have us believe. It must, however, bo said that the observations of v. Kries and others, which were alluded to in an earlier chapter, have made it difficult for us to believe that the difference between protanopia BERING'S THEORY OF VISUAL SENSATIONS 201 and deuteranopia is not more definite than this explana- tion would suggest. Tschermak/ a prominent supporter of Bering's views, seems more or less definitely to abandon this attempt to describe dichromatic vision, and, so far as I know, an adequate expression of the facts in terms of Hering's hypothesis has not yet been found. Our general conclusions, therefore, may perhaps be ex- pressed in the following way : — The theory associated with the name of Hering is an attempt to arrive at a general conception of the visual processes by an analysis and comparison of sensations of colour. In this way prominence is given to many important facts neither specially nor adequately resumed in the theory founded on stimulus relations. If, however, we attempt to build up upon such data an hypothesis adequate to the task of describing all the experimental facts, difficulties are encountered not less formidable than those associated with the Young-Helmholtz theory. In attempting to meet their respective difficulties the two theories became unsatisfactory in different ways. The stimulus hypothesis becomes too general, the sensational hypothesis too detailed. Examples have been seen in the former's treatment of dichromatic vision, and in the latter's account of after-images. The most tempting reconciliation of the modes of analysis is to suppose that the theories are in some sense comple- mentary — a view akin to that of Donders ^ — that they both contain some measure of truth, surveying the vast complex of phenomena from different points of views. Neither theory is wholly true nor yet wholly false, nor does adhesion to the one imply total rejection of its ostensible rival. Of the numerous other theories of colour vision I do not, for reasons already given, intend to speak ; many, especi- ally perhaps those of Schenck and Ladd-Franklin,^ are highly ^ Tschermak, Ergebn. d. Physiologic, 1st Jahrg. (1902), second part, p. 795. * Donders, Archif. f. Opthalm., 1881, xxvli. part 1, p. 55; ibid., 1884, xxx. part 1, p. 15. ' Schenck, Pflilg. Arch., cxviii. (1907), p. 161. Ladd-Franklin, Z.P.P.S.O., iv. (1893), 211. 202 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES ingenious ; all contain some points of importance, but none can be compared in interest and educational value with those we have examined in the last two chapters. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY The best account in English, and written with judicial impartiality, is that of Dr. Rivers, Schafer's Text-book of Physiology, Edinburgh, 1900, vol. ii. p. 1026. In Support of The Yodng-Helmholtz Theory — Helmholtz, Handb. d. Physiol. Optik., second edition. V. Kries, Nagel's Handb., vol. iii. p. 109. In SnppoET of Hering's Theory — Hering, Griindziige der Lehre vom Lichtsinn (Sonderabdruck a. d. Handbuch d. Augenheilkunde, 1 Teil, xii. Kap.). Leipzig, 1905-7, Engelmann. (This is an admirably cleax account so far as it has gone, but the separate publication appears to be delayed. ) CHAPTER XX SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST It may be regarded as a very general truth of sense physiology that the sum of the effects produced by two simultaneous stimuli is not equal to the total effect produced by two stimuli applied successively. The mutual effect exerted in this way by two synchronous excitations is revealed to us subjectively as contrast. Although this phenomenon of contrast is observed in each department of sensation, its study is most conveniently effected in the case of vision, and a rich harvest of results has been garnered by workers in this field. For this reason I shall confine myself to a study of visual contrast ; the generalisation of the results, together with the modifications necessary in special cases, is an exercise which may profitably be left to the reader. Although it is perfectly true that the fundamental ex- periments in simultaneous contrast are readily performed, the subject in its detailed examination is not free from difficulty, and a satisfactory theory has not yet been pro- pounded. In summarising the experimental methods and results I shall follow the order adopted by Tschermak,i whose memoir is both lucid and exhaustive. The theoretical considerations which I shall finally submit, although not in any true sense novel, will be found to differ slightly from the usual interpretations. The first class of experiments comprises Surface or Areal Contrast, in which the alteration is appreciable over a comparatively large surface. These ex- periments can be subdivided again into brightness or black- white contrasts and colour contrasts. The simplest instance in the first group is the varying brightness of a scrap of grey paper in accordance with the whiteness or blackness of the background against which it is viewed. Bering has demonstrated this in his elegant " Doppel-Zimmer " experiments. A circular aperture in an ' A. V. Tsohermak, " Ueber Kontrast und Irradiation," Ergebnisse d. Fhysiol., 1903, second part, pp. 726-798, with exhaustive bibliography. 203 204 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES opaque partition is kept constantly illuminated ; its apparent brightness is, however, seen to vary with changes in its environment. Generally the two contrasting surfaces are concentric, but this is merely for convenience of experiment, as Aubert pointed out. Areal colour contrast has long been known, the simple fact that a colour tends to produce an apparent com- plementary tinging of a neighbouring field having been pointed out by Leonardo da Vinci. The ordinary laboratory method of demonstration is known as Meyer's experiment, although the principle is re?Jly due to Johannes Mtiller. A sheet of green (for example) paper is spread out and a scrap of grey paper placed in the middle, the whole being covered with tissue paper. The grey scrap appears of a distinct rose hue. Another method depends on the colouring of an objectively colourless mirrored light or shadow which is caused by a coloured background. Goethe noticed that the image of window bars reflected from the upper surface of a piece of green glass was purple. The best form of the experiment is due to Ragona Scina, and has been slightly modified by Hering. The observer looks through a sheet of coloured glass inclined at an angle of 45° to a horizontal sheet of paper with black and white figures on it — for instance, rings of about 1 cm. in breadth ; a second sheet of white paper is placed vertically opposite the inclined plate.^ The black rings on the horizontal sheet, upon which colourless light is mirrored by the vertical sheet, are brightly tinged by contrast. The white rings appear in the saturated and the ground in the unsaturated colour qi the glass. Cobalt glass gives, I think, the best effect, but ordinary red glass does very well. Probably the most striking effect of all is that of coloured shadows, the existence of which was recognised by Leonardo da Vinci and Otto von Guericke. For demonstration pur- poses no special apparatus is necessary, and I find the foUow- ^ This sheet, which should be at right angles to that seen through the glass, ought to have black and white rings on it alternating with those on the other sheet, so that the colour of the white rings seen through the glass is not weakened by mirroring of white light from corresponding rings on the vertical sheet. SIMULTANEOUS CONTEAST 205 ing arrangement quite good. Set up vertically on a table a large white screen, and lean against it a lecturer's pointer. Darken the room except for one window slit, which gives a sharp shadow on the screen. On the side of the table away from the window stand an ordinary electric table lamp, so that a second shadow is formed on the screen. The window shadow is diffusely illuminated by the yellow electric light, and looks yellow ; the lamp shadow, which is diffusely illuminated by the window light and should appear grey, is strongly blue. The contrastive nature of the appearance is demonstrated by switching off the electric light, when the shadow at once loses its colour. The most exact application of the method is due to Hering, and is, in prin- ciple, as follows: Two parallel slits are made in a vertical shutter; one is covered with ground glass, the other with coloured glass, the double shadow being focussed on a white screen. Another good method is that of Helmholtz. A small black disc is brought upon the dividing line of a half- white, half-green field and looked at through a fragment of Iceland spar. The extraordinary image of polarisation of the green falling on the ordinary white image forms an unsaturated green middle stripe. Internal to this the ordinary image of the right side of the black disc loses its whiteness and appears saturated green; the extraordinary image of the left half of the disc also appears internal, but is illuminated with white, and appears in the contrast colour. Another form of contrast observations relates to the changes observed at the margin of a field. For instance, a grey disc on a black ground appears brighter at the periphery than at the centre, and a white trellis work on a dark ground seems darker at the points of crossing than at other parts. This emphasis, as it were, of the margin is responsible for some of the beauties of mountain scenery, the graduation of grey values in mountain peaks with intensi- fication of the contour lines. We must now consider some details of the contrastive action. (1) The Spatial Extent of the Effect. The intensity of the contrast diminishes rapidly as we pass 206 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES out from the excited area, and a very slight separation of the contrasting fields makes a considerable difference in the result. A small black line separating two fields markedly diminishes the contrast, a fact to which Helmholtz attributes much importance. A somewhat important observation is that of Charpentier, that the liminal stimulus value for a given retinal area is the same whether light be completely excluded from that area or contrast blackness induced by stimulating a neighbouring point. If true, this opinion tells strongly against the theory of contrast advanced by Hering (vide infra), but many observations are difficult to reconcile with it, especially those of Brewster, Meyer, and Aubert. (2) The temporal Succession of Contrast Effects. The optimum contrast efi'ect is attained after a measur- able but very short period of time (Exner), and it then falls off rapidly. Hering showed " that any true contrastive effect is most marked at the beginning of the observation (apart from the very short period of development), and diminishes rapidly, only remaining distinct for a very short interval." He attributes contradictory results to the occurrence of after- images associated with movements of the eye. With long continued fixation the contrast colour fades, giving place to its complementary (Hering's Simultaneous Coloured or Colourless Induction). By employing a colourless disc in a coloured field, when the contrast has faded, the complementary colour gradually appears, increasing in saturation until finally disc and field fuse together. Accordingly during fixation of the object it- self the negative after-image may result, a sudden diminu- tion in the amount of light being a specially favouring circumstance. It is, therefore, a difficult matter to observe accurately the time relations of simultaneous contrast with the exclusion of after-images. Absolutely steady fixation, utilisation of the first moment of appearance of the induced colour, and short duration of the whole experiment are essential conditions. The relations between simultaneous and successive con- trast were exhaustively studied in Hering's laboratory by Kuhnt. He used colourless discs on a moderately illuminated SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST 207 background of coloured glass or paper. With red and green the simultaneous contrastive effect endures for some seconds, with yellow or blue the effect is but momentary. In the end, in all cases, disc and background become indistinguishable. It is generally held that contrastive effects are better marked in indirect than in foveal vision, which naturally suggests that light-dark adaptation would be of importance in this connection. Some experiments support this con- clusion, but no systematic inquiries have, I think, been published. Quantitative Laws of Simultaneous Contrast (1) White-Black Contrast. It has been generally held that the subjective bright- ness of a measurably illuminated surface varies when seen against a background of varying illumination. Lehmann, as the result of numerous experiments, concluded that the maximum contrastive effect was attained when the illumina- tions of background and disc were in a constant ratio (about 4"76) to one another. Ebbinghaus' results were somewhat complicated. He asserted that contrastive whiteness was proportional to the difference between the intensities of illumination of the contrasting fields and independent of their absolute magnitudes. Contrast darkening, on the other hand, was said to be proportional to the difference of the illuminations multi- plied by their quotient, and accordingly depended on absolute intensity. Hess and Pretori, who repeated the work with special precautions, agree with Ebbinghaus in respect of contrastive whiteness, but disagree with his rule for contrast darkening, which, they hold, follows the same law as was found in the opposite case. (2) Colov/r Contrast. This has been studied somewhat exhaustively by Pretori and Sachs in Hering's laboratory. In their first series of experiments they worked in the following way. In the contrast-exciting field they used a coloured paper and a grey paper, which looked exactly like it to a dark adapted eye. 208 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES In the contrast-sufFering field (for brevity, I shall write c.e. and c.s. fields) a definite grey was used, formed by mixing black and white, while the initial contrastive tinging was sought to be eliminated by the introduction of a coloured sector. In the experiments the coloured sector in the c.e. field was continuously increased, that in the c.s. field kept constant, the contrastive tinging being obliterated by using a larger and larger black sector. They found that with a constant white valency in the c.e. field, combined with in- creasing colour valency, the same magnitude in colour contrastive effect is reached with a simply proportional diminution of white valency in the c.s. field. If the c.e. light were kept constant, and the amount of white in the c.s. field increased, the amount of contrast effect rises from zero to a certain optimum amount. In the second series of experiments the coloured sector of the c.e. field was kept constant and the grey varied. The coloured sector of the c.s. field was maintained con- stant and the white sector varied until the contrastive tinging disappeared. Under these conditions the optimum contrastive effect was attained with a proportionally higher white valency of the c.s. field. In the third series of observations, both coloured and colourless components of the c.e. field were varied, but in such ratio that the white valency increased at the same rate as the colour valency. In the c.s. field the white sector was varied. In general, they found no increase in the contrastive effect when both white and coloured components of the c.e. field were thus varied {i.e. with a constant saturation of the colour), but some experiments indicated an increase in con- trastive effect with the intensity of the c.e. field up to a definite maximum. The Colour of the Stimulus and that of the CONTEASTING FlELD It has long been known that, under ordinary conditions, the contrast colour is not exactly complementary to the exciting colour, and a similar, more striking, discrepancy has been observed in ordinary after-images. The fact is SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST 209 undoubted, but the explanation is obscure. Since, in general, the variation takes the form of an apparent addition of blue-red to the true complementary, and is either slight or absent after prolonged dark adaptation, Hering has supposed that ordinary daylight possesses a certain (yellowish) colour valency, so that the eye is not, in his terminology, in a condition of neutral tuning. The explanation is undoubtedly ingenious, but its discussion would be out of place in an elementary work, a remark which also applies to individual and pathological anomalies in contrast effect. Binocular Contrast The earliest experiments on this subject were due to Smith, Brewster, Fechner, and Meyer, A white object against a dark background was binocularly viewed, and one eye was illuminated through the sclerotic with a beam of yellowish-red light. To that eye the object appeared blue- green, to the other, of the same tinge as the objective side light. The most striking demonstration of binocular con- trast is probably that devised by Hering. One eye looks through a red, the other through a blue glass of not very different brightness, both glasses being sloped obliquely from the nasal to the temporal side to allow saturation to be suitably regulated by mirroring white light from side screens. A black stripe on a white ground is doubled by increasing or diminishing the ocular convergence. Although the observed background is here and there patchy, now red, now blue, owing to " retinal rivalry," or sometimes a uniform whitish-violet, the stripe seen through the red glass looks green, and the image for the eye looking through the blue glass appears yellow. The Theories of Simultaneous Contrast In early times much difference of opinion prevailed as to whether contrast colour was objective in nature or a subjective phenomenon, but the problem cannot be said to have attained much importance in the theory of sensory processes before the time of Helmholtz. The theory ac- 210 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES cepted by Brticke, and elaborated by Helmboltz, denies that the basis of contrast action is a physiological alteration in any of the percipient structures, and has consequently been termed a psychological theory. It must, however, be thoroughly grasped from the outset that consciousness was not thought by Helmholtz to have any part in the genesis of contrast. It is the more needful to realise this, because certain passages in the treatise of Helmholtz are so worded that it is very difficult to avoid the inference that he is invoking a process of conscious judgment to account for the facts. Some of the more superficial students of Helm- holtz have undoubtedly fallen into this error. As to this, it is sufficient to remark that the experiment in Binocular Contrast just described is alone sufficient to disprove the possibility of consciousness intervening in the matter at all, since, if we make such an assumption, we should have to believe in the possibility of two incorrect judgments re- specting the same objects, and inconsistent one with the other, being simultaneously entertained, which is evidently absurd. With this caution, I shall enter upon a description of the BrUcke-Helmholtz theory. The basis of pure simultaneous contrast, especially of marginal contrast, is not a change in the mechanism of sensation, but a change in our infra-conscious interpretation of the sensation. Through the continuous and predominat- ing influence of one colour, the standard of what we call white undergoes a change. Great errors in judgment as to colour are obviated by the existence of retinal light (the sensation experienced when the eyes are closed), but if an object is viewed under circumstances which render com- parison with an objective standard difficult or impossible, then errors arise. Helmholtz performed the experiment of Ragona Scina, already described in a slightly different way. Vertical and horizontal sheets of paper were white, on the vertical plate is a black, and on the horizontal a white and a black scrap of paper. The black spot is seen in the con- trast colour, rose-red, when the glass plate is green. Helm- holtz explains this in the following terms : — " One judges that the black spot on the lower horizontal SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST 2U sheet is rose-red, but also judges that one sees this spot with its rose-red colour like the whole sheet through the green glass, and that the green colour given by the glass extends itself without a break over the whole underlying surface, including the dark spot. One believes, therefore, that at this place one sees simultaneously two colours, viz. green, which one ascribes to the glass plate, and rose-red, which one ascribes ' to the paper, and both together give, as a matter of fact, the true colour of this part, namely white. In fact, an object which, when viewed through green glass, sends white light to the eye, like this spot, must be rose-red. If we now bring above the glass plate a distinctly perceived white object, there is no more reason for splitting the colour of the object in two, and it looks to us white." ^ He describes Meyer's experiment in almost identical language : — " It is the same when coloured surfaces are covered with tissue paper. If the underlying surface is green, the paper appears itself to be of a greenish colour. If, now, the sub- stance of the paper extends without any visible interruption over the grey placed underneath, one thinks one sees an object shining through green paper, and such an object must necessarily be rose-red in order to give white light. But if the white part is definitely marked out, continuity with the greenish part of the surface is destroyed, and one regards it as a white object lying on that surface." ^ Helmholtz also laid some stress on the observation of Osann, that the coloured shadow effect (see above) persisted when the shadow was looked at through a tube so that the contrasting field was not seen. This theory and the arguments adduced in its favour have long been the object of Hering's attacks, attacks which must, I think, be regarded as successful, at any rate in the sense that the theory as outlined in the preceding quotations can hardly be maintained. Hering demonstrated that recognition of the grey disc (in Meyer's experiment) as not forming a part of the green field does not destroy the contrast. He also pointed out that 1 Helmholtz, Phys. OptiJc., p. 560. ^ ji^_ 212 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES marking off the grey disc with a black pencil line, or using glazed paper (either of which, according to Helmholtz, diminished the contrast effect) also diminish after-images, which Helmholtz agreed to be of physiological origin. With regard to Kagona Scina's experiment, he took even stronger ground. He showed that in this experiment, as modified by himself, the effect is just as marked when the glass plate is not seen at all, or even its existence known to the observer. By rotating the glass plate and the horizontal sheet the rings can be seen at different distances, and even in reversed positions, without change in the contrast effect. Osann's observation is attributed by Hering to the disturbing influence of after-image formation. Hering accounts for simultaneous contrast on the general lines of the theory explained in a previous chapter, anabolism or katabolism in a visual substance leading to a reciprocal activity in the immediate vicinity of the excited substance. In what region this reciprocal effect is produced, Hering does not decide. It cannot be in the retina itself, because simultaneous contrast has been obtained on a complete central scotoma due to retinitis of the papulo-macular bundle (Tschermak), and many workers have obtained both colourless and colour contrast at the blind spot. It would be a somewhat thankless task to examine this controversy in detail, but I venture to put forward for the reader's consideration the following attempt at reconciling the two theories. No afferent impulse when it passes beyond the physio- logical level into what one may term the infra-conscious region is simplex, its entrance implies the simultaneous entrance of something not itself. The basis of a colour sensation, red, say, may be, perhaps must be, a unique chemico-physical change in the receiving organ, but there is at the next stage no such thing as red per se, only red in contrast to what is not red. Supposing, therefore, the products of, for instance, stimulation with white light and stimulation with red light pass on into the infra-conscious region we shall really have four things — red, not-red, white, uot-white. Since the not-red and the not-white are, as it SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST 213 were, solely infra-conscious products, they will tend to pass away from each other towards the immediate products of activity in the physiological field. There will therefore be an intensification of the opposition between the " real " red and the " real " white. The red and the white will have their mutual difference exaggerated. But if we remember Hering's analysis of the colour field — an analysis which, as the reader will remember, depends in no way upon any physiological hypothesis, but is the starting point of such an hypothesis — the opposite to white is black and to red is green. Hence we have presented in the infra-conscious sphere a (darkened) red and a green. In this way one can account for the facts of simultaneous contrast. Evidently the introduction of some third element, which may serve as a standard of reference, will destroy this simple opposition, and this is what appears to happen. I think this way of looking at the facts better than that indicated in the quotations from Helmholtz, because it is hard to avoid the intervention of true consciousness when we use what looks like a rather complicated process of inference, as in his account of Meyer's experiment. Strictly speaking, all that is done is to transfer Hering's theory of reciprocal action to the infra-conscious sphere — I do not speak of sub-consciousness, because that term has a fairly precise psychological meaning, not corresponding to my idea — from the physiological level. This transference is, I think> necessary. In the first place, it is very difficult to form a clear 'conception of the physiological mechanism by which the reciprocal action can be supposed to be effected. In the second place, the idea of a simple colour sensation without any existent opposite sensation is — to me at least — unattain- able. I can conceive of a single physiological change pre- ceding the existence of the sensation of redness, but I cannot isolate that sensation when it comes into existence above the physiological level. The existence of redness seems to me to imply necessarily the simultaneous existence of not-redness. I claim no originality for this attempted reconciliation of the rival theories, although I am not consciously borrowing 214 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES it from another writer. The objections to it are numerous. It is psychologically crude and very incomplete. I suspect, however, that investigation along these lines might well enable a competent psychologist to reconcile the apparently opposite theories of Hering and Helmholtz, although it may reasonably be doubted whether the end would justify the trouble involved. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY The reader will find a clear account of all the important researches and a full bibliography of the subject in A. Tsc/iermak's Ueber Kontrast und Irradiation (Ergebnisse d. Physiol., 1903, II. Abth., pp. 726-798). CHAPTER XXI THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" A SCIENTIFIC poet in the noble language which never failed him has set forth the conception of space as an objective reality which was once accepted by philosophers, and is still believed by those who have neither opportunity nor inclina- tion to consider the subject with attention.^ If, he teaches, we consider the nature of things, we find that there are bodies and empty space. Were there no empty space, bodies could have neither position nor movement. This space must likewise be intangible — " Cui si tactus erit quamvis levis exiguusque, Augmine vel grandi vel parvo deuique, dum sit, Corporis augebit numerum summamque sequetur." Beyond this space and the bodies moving in it nothing has existence — " Nam quae cumque cluent, aut his conjuncta duabus Rebus ea invenies aut horum eventa videbis." The speculations of many of the greatest philosophers of past time, pre-eminently perhaps those of Berkeley, have moulded thought into a very different form, and from the scientific side — apart from speculative metaphysics — we may be content to say that " Space and Time are not realities of the phenomenal world, but the modes under which we see things apart. They are not infinitely large nor infinitely divisible, but are essentially limited by the contents of our perception." ^ It is, however, no part of a physiologist's business to discuss these obscure riddles. We have to recognise the existence of a faculty of separating simultaneous sense ' Lucretius, De Rer. Nat., Bk. I., 418 et seq. - The Grammar of Science, by Karl Pearson, second edition, 1900, p. 191. 216 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES impressions or, to speak rather unscientifically, of perceiv- ing spatial relations between stimuli, and we are to inquire which of the sense organs are specially concerned in this process. It has always been, at least in modern times, a moot point whether this power or faculty is innate or acquired, and, if the latter, which sense mechanism is to be deemed of primary importance. Without expressing any opinion 'at all as to which school is abstractly correct, it must be admitted that, physiologically, the work of the empiricists is of much greater importance, and the doctor of this school whose work deserves special attention is undoubtedly Berkeley. I shall, therefore, examine his views at some length. Berkeley's contribution to the subject is contained in An Essay towards a New Theory of Vision, which was first published in 1709, when its author was only twenty-four years of age, being perhaps the most important contribution to modern philosophy which has been made by so young a man. Berkeley's doctrine can be summarised in a few words. Distance and magnitude are not directly determined by the nature of the images formed on the retina, nor by changes in the inclination of the optic axes. They are ideas which we form as the result of experience, and are not, strictly speaking, sensations at all. " I know it is a received opinion that, by altering the disposition of the eyes, the mind perceives whether the angle of the optic axes, or the lateral angle comprehended between the interval of the eyes and the optic axes, are made greater or lesser ; and thus, accordingly, by a kind of naural geometry, it judges the point of their intersection to be nearer or farther ofT. But that this is not true I am convinced by my own experience, since I am not conscious that I make any such use of the perception I have by the turn of my eyes. And for me to make these judgments, and draw these con- clusions from it, without knowing that I do so, seems altogether imcomprehensible." ^ The position is defined in the following admirable passage : " From what we have shown, it is a manifest consequence 1 Berkeley's Works, vol. i. p. 83 (Sampson's Edition, London, Bell, 1897). THE PHYSIOLOGY OF. "SPACE" 217 that the ideas of space, outness, and things placed at a distance are not, strictly speaking, the objects of sight ; they are not otherwise perceived by the eye than by the ear. Sitting in my study I hear a coach drive along the street; I look through the casement and see it ; I walk out and enter it. Thus, common speech would incline one to think I heard, saw, and touched the same thing, to wit, the coach. It is nevertheless certain the ideas intromitted by each sense are widely different and distinct from each other; but having been observed constantly to go together, they are spoken of a,s one and the same thing. By the variation of the noise I perceive the different distances of the coach, and know that it approaches before I look out. Thus, by the ear I perceive distance j ust after the same manner as I do by the eye. I do not nevertheless say I hear distance in like manner as I say that I see it — the ideas perceived by hearing not being so apt to be confounded with the ideas of touch as those of sight are." ^ The concluding sentence of this passage will suggest to the reader the weak point of Berkeley's reasoning. Although he is careful to point out that there is no necessary but only a habitual connection between visible and tangible magni- tude,2 yet in his detailed applications of the theory he is constantly accounting for visual sensations of magnitude by their connection with tangible magnitudes. Thus he con- cludes a discussion of units of length with the words: " From all which it is manifest that the judgments we make of the magnitudes of objects by sight are altogether in re- ference to their tangible extension." But there is, in theory, no reason why visual space should be subordinate to tangible space ; the contrary proposition could equally well be main- tained. We might, for instance, imagine that our unit of length is not a tangible impression involving some number of touch areas, but a retinal impression affecting a certain number of conoidal areas. I take it the reason why tangible magnitude is taken to be primitive is that cases of congenital blindness with normal tactile sensibility are common, while cases of total cutaneous anaesthesia of congenital origin com- 1 Op. eit., p. 98. ^ Ibid., p. 102. 218 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES bined with normal vision either never occur or have never been described. It would, therefore, seem that Berkeley's treatment of the subject, admirable though it is, does not give an adequate account of spatial perception in general ; its physiological importance is its insistence on a study of the empirical facts. A somewhat similar objection has been urged by Professor James against the speculations of an even greater thinker on the subject, Helmholtz. I shall now, without further re- ference to the theory of the matter, enumerate the physio- logical data which have to be taken into consideration, beginning with the information derived from the cutaneous mechanisms. It is a matter of common knowledge that we possess the power of localising the point of application of a tactile stimulus, and that the fineness of this power varies in dif- ferent persons and in different skin areas in the same subject. Experimentally, the matter can be investigated by the use of some form of sesthesiometer. The most useful form is an instrument constructed on the principle of a compass, the points being covered in such a way that pain is not produced by their application, and that the surface of contact with the skin is identical in both limbs. Since if two stimuli are applied together, we can when they are more than a certain distant apart recognise that two points are stimulated, and since when two different points are successively touched, we can recognise that the second stimulus was not applied to the same region as the first, two sets of experiments are to be planned. In the first set we apply the stimuli to- gether and determine the shortest distance which corresponds- to (a) a duplicate sensation, (b) a single sensation having lineal extension. In the second set the stimuli are successively employed, and we ascertain the shortest distance enabling the subject to perceive (a) that the second stimulated point is not Identical with the first (b) exactly where the second poiny is situated. The general conclusion to be drawn from such experiments is, that the liminal distance for simultaneous is far greater than for successive stimulation, V. Frey concludes that if two neighbouring touch spots are THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE"- 219 stimulated successively, one can always, under favourable experimental conditions, recognise that the second stimulus was not applied to the same point as the first. If the spots are touched at the same time, then a duplicate sensation does not arise, the latter being only produced if one un- stimulated touch area lies between the points of application. (The actual liminal distance for simultaneous stimulation varies from about 1*1 mm. on the tip of the tongue to over 65 on the upper arm or thigh. In the case of the limbs the direction of the line joining the two points is not in- different, the liminal distance being greater for stimulation parallel with the long axis of the limb than for transverse application. The actual values obtained depend very greatly on the conditions of the experiment, the influence of fatigue being particularly appareny Local vascular conditions also play a part, although different workers are not agreed as to their exact influence. (^It has also been generally asserted that practice diminishes the liminal distance, but this statement has not, I think, been entirely substantiated/ That any change in the usual relations of the part stimulated greatly affects one's power of localisation is well seen in any of the modifications of Aristotle's experiment, the best being that of Henri. When the middle and ring fingers are crossed and the subject is given a diagram of the fingers in their normal relation on which he is to mark the point touched, stimuli applied to the radial side of the crossed finger tend to be localised towards the ulnar margin. Without discussing the theoretical interpretation of these results, we can pass to the visual data for space perception. The physiological basis of spatial perception, in so far as it depends on visual sensations, is to be sought in move- ments of the eyes. The musculature of the eye and the exact shape of the eyeball, together with the direction and force with which each muscle pulls, have been elaborately studied. Of the methods adopted, two are of special interest. The Method of After-images. A coloured ■ piece of paper is affixed to a sheet of grey paper ruled in squares of known size. The after-image of 220 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES the coloured scrap appears on different parts of the ruled surface in accordance with the way in which the eye has been moved; the ruled squares enable the position to be accurately defined. The Substitution Method of Hering Probably the best form of this method is that adopted by Bonders in his Isoscope (Fig. 23). RR is a fixed framework on which a movable parallelogram is fixed. D is a thread attached to the frame EE, two threads attached to the parallelogram and moving with it. The parallelogram is mov- FiG. 23. able round an antero-posterior axis, and supplied with an index pointer F reading on a scale G. With this instrument one can measure the inclination of EE and D to one another which gives parallel double images when both eyes fixate a given point. In this way one can study the apparent situation of images when the relation of the two retinse is altered. It is usual to call the arrangement of the eyes which obtains when the head is vertical and the visual axes parallel to the plan of the ground the " primary . position." To change from this position either directly up or down or THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" 221 directly to the right or the left, the eyes never rotate round an antero-posterior axis, like, for instance, the steering-wheel of a ship. The last sentence contains the teaching of Listing on this point, and is usually termed Listing's " Law." Another "Law," first propounded by Bonders, is that for identical directions of the eyes the orientation of the retinae is identical, however the eyes may have been rotated into the final position. Both these statements only apply to cases in which the visual axes are approximately parallel and movements are effected without undue muscular strain. It is of mechanical and physiological advantage for rotation to be effected in accordance with Listing's principle; the mechanical advantage is that the movement follows the shortest path and, other things being equal, can be effected in the shortest time; the physiological advantage (Hering) is that apparent rotation of an external object, owing to wheel-like movements of the eye, does not occur, and that the number of corresponding retinal points (vide infra) is maximal. Numerous researches, mainly by the method of after- images, seem to demonstrate that even for parallel, axes the law of Listing is not strictly true, but the deviations, at least for normal eyes, are not large. MoNocuLAE Vision The first question which arises is with regard to visual acuity, or the power of distinguishing between the images formed by different objects or by different parts of one object. The simplest case is an answer to the question. How far apart must two points be in order to be distinguished? The answer depends upon several circumstances, of which the most important is the amount of contrast between the points and the background (e.g. black points on a white surface) against which they are viewed, so that the minimum distance varies enormously in accordance with the experi- mental conditions. I cite a few examples on the following page. 222 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES Object. Visual Angle. Observer. Parallel threads . . ... 50 sees. Hirschniann. Black points (on a white ground) 1 min. 4 sees. Hueck. White squares (on a blackground) 55 sees. Aubert. Parallel black lines alternating with equally broad white ones . 52 secs.-lmin. 15 sees. Bergmann, Jupiter's satellites More than 2 mins. A. Humboldt. With good conditions of contrast and moderate illumina- tion, Snellen's assumption that a normal eye can distinguish points separated by an angular distance of 1 minute is probably accurate, enough for most purposes. Calculations made on the basis of such experiments as to the actual size of the retinal images suggest that, as in the case of touch, one unstimulated retinal element must always intervene for stimuli affecting two other elements to be separately sensed. It is generally held that, in the case of foveal vision, the retinal elements in question are the cones; whether the inner or the outer limbs of the cones are involved is a moot point, but Hensen has advanced somewhat cogent evidence in favour of the outer limbs. Eor practical purposes, visual acuity at the fovea is tested by the method invented by Jager and improved by Snellen, which is too well known to need description. The hypo- thesis of Snellen that the legibility of his test letters is pro- portional to the visual angle in every direction of the letter has not escaped question. According to Guillery, there is no uniform relation between power to recognise an object and the size of its retinal image in simple objects, and, a fortiori, none in the case of such complicated figures as letters. The ease with which Snellen's types can be recog- nised is greater than one's power of isolating the squares of a chess-board pattern seen under a visual angle of 1 minute for each square and with effective contrast. The fact is, that in reading letters of a familiar alphabet we fill in the letter mentally from seeing a small part of it, j ust as in a Persian THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" 223 manuscript the letters are not written out in full, only the (to a native) characteristic portions of each. Snellen's method is, therefore, a test of the oninimwm of visual acuity. Snellen's method has been modified by Cohn, who sub- stitutes combinations of lines for the letters so that it can be used for illiterate persons. Cohn, who has investigated the normal acuity of uncivilised peoples (Beduins, Egyptians, etc.), is of opinion that the general belief that civilised peoples have lower visual acuity than uncivilised races is unfounded. His data have not, however, been submitted to rigorous statistical analysis. Cohn reports some cases of visual acute- ness far transcending the normal limits; thus an Egyptian boy of sixteen had an acuity eight times the normal value, and a girl of eleven, six times the normal. It is improbable in view of Pearson and Barrington's work that unfavourable environment — for instance, too close application to books in early childhood — is of such importance in deteriorating eyesight as various popular authors have alleged, but the subject must still be considered as of a con- troversial nature. Hering has pointed out that methods such as those described, which rest on the determination of the just-notice- able distance between two objects, do not reveal the fineness of the visual "Raumsinn" for the determination of "the slightest difference in situation or magnitude which the eye can appreciate." This is analogous to the skin sense which is able to distinguish two points successively touched when closer together than two points simultaneously stimulated — i.e. contiguous elements of the retinal surface give rise to different sensations or have " local sign." So far we have considered foveal acuity; as we pass towards the periphery of the retina, the acuteness of vision diminishes, under ordinary circumstances, fast, but the change is not spatially symmetrical. The falling off is more rapid in the vertical than in the horizontal meridian, and in the latter is more rapid externally than internally. Important researches on the comparative acuteness of vision in light and dark adapted eyes have been carried out by v. Kries and his pupils. V. Kries found that in dark adaptation visual 224 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES acuity between tlie centre of the fovea and the blind spot, at a distance of 4 to 12 degrees from the fovea, was practically constant, while at the fovea itself the test objects were imper- ceptible. From the blind spot outwards, acuity is the same for light and dark adapted eyes. Koester taking visual acuity as zero at the fovea ( " dark " eye), found it rapidly rising at an eccentricity of 5 to 10 degrees, and from thence fairly constant towards the periphery. At an eccentricity of 30 to 40 degrees, " dark " visual acuity is greater than that of the " light " eye. Bloom and Garten obtained somewhat different results; according to them,neither centrum nor periphery of the "dark" eye attains the degree of visual acuity possessed in light adaptation. Central and peripheral acuity are affected in the same way by dark adaptation, but not to the same ex- tent ; only when the illumination is very feeble does the dark adapted retina give the better response. Perception of Depth with the help of one Eye Strictly speaking, we can with the help of one eye only perceive differences of directions in lines along which points are situated, but accumulated experience enables us to dis- tinguish depth and form with moderate accuracy. How large a part is played by past experience will be clear when we recall the numerous illusions of form which beset us when we examine distant objects for the first time. Factors which influence our judgment of relief (either in the case of mono- cular or binocular vision) are (1) the distribution of shadows, as seen in the fine sculpturipg of snow-clad hills and valleys when the sun is low on the horizon; (2) the state of the atmosphere, aerial perspective, which causes the citizen to misjudge distances greatly when in clear mountain air. For the judgment of the distances of near objects the state of accommodation, although of importance, does not give so much help as might be expected. Wundt has ex- perimented on this point. The subject looks at a black thread, which can be moved nearer to or farther from a white screen, through an opening. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" 225 Although the absolute distance of the thread could not be gauged, yet, within limits, it could be ascertained in which direction the thread had been moved. Distance of Thread. Liminal Movement. Wlien moved Forwards. When moved Backwards. Metres. 2-5 2-2 2 1-8 1 •8 ■5 ■4 Centimetres. 12 10 8 8 8 5 4-5 4-5 Centimetres. 12 12 12 12 11 7 6-5 4-5 These experiments show sufficiently well how inexact are the data afforded by monocular vision for judging distance with the help of accommodation, and they are really too favourable, for information is afforded by changes in the brightness and distinctness of the threads. Hillebrand and others, by methods in which these latter sources of infor- mation were excluded, have found that the data afibrded by changes in accommodation (or, to speak more precisely, changes in accommodation and convergence) are even less complete than Wundt's work suggests. We all know that changes in the position of the eye — or head — with respect to the object improve our judg- ment of solidity. One reason why this should be so is plain. When the eye is moved the rapidity with which a retinal image changes its position depends on the distance of the object, and is inversely proportional to it. If there be two objects in the field of vision, and one moves faster than the other when the head is moved, we conclude the former to be nearer to us. Some experiments carried out by Bourdon are of interest in this connection. Two points, separated in one experiment 6 degrees, in the other 1 degree apart, were placed at about 6 metres from the eye, one point being higher than the other. The observer was allowed to move his head but not his body. In the p 226 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES case of the higher point being the farther off, a correct answer was given for both distances; when the lower point was the farther off, the results were not so good but generally better for the smaller angular distances. Judgment of Shape This part of the subject, which includes a study of the numerous geometrical illusions, such as that of Zollner and Poggendorf, can only be dealt with in a few words. Many experiments have been performed to determine how much two straight lines must differ in order that we may per- ceive the difference, but the numerical values of different observers are discordant. Horizontal lines can be more accurately compared than vertical ones, and a comparison of horizontal and vertical distances leads to still greater errors.' If one attempts to draw a square on a plane at right angles to the line of sight, the vertical sides may be as much as one-fifth shorter than the horizontal ones, the least error being one-sixtieth to one-thirtieth. According to Feilchenfeld, when a rectangular cross is approximated to the eye one increasingly over-estimates the nasal limb, a result which he attributes to the greater extent of the temporal visual field. If one freely chooses the fixation point in monocular vision, the temporal half of a line to be bisected is over-estimated because (Feilchenfeld) the point which lies along the fixation line, and is too much towards the temporal side, is erroneously taken to be the centre. These explanations appear to me to leave the experimental facts pretty much where they were before. The various ways in which the eye can be deceived with regard to form and direction are too well known through the medium of popular magazines to need reproduction, and I confine myself to a diagram of the Mliller-Lyer illusion (Fig. 24). The part of the horizontal line towards which the short lines approach appears shorter than the exactly equal portion from which the lines recede. Painstaking attempts have been made to interpret this and similar illusions, with indifferent success. Some regard the basis THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" 227 as directly sensational, others as secondary or dependent upon a process of subconscious or unconscious inference. Fie. 24. Helmholtz has interpreted many of these illusions on the basis of a " law of contrast," according to which clearly perceived differences are judged to be greater than indis- tinctly perceived ones, a reversal of the old dictum, omne ignotum pro nnagnifico. He explains in this way the well- known illusion that a distance divided by dots appears greater than the same space not divided, the former being more distinctly seen, and therefore judged to be greater. In other cases, irradiation and ocular movements are thought to be influential. Hering has suggested that the apparent distance between two points depends on the linear distance of their retinal image points. Hence, small distances are more accurately judged than large ones, since the length of the chord of a small arc approximates to that of the arc. Einthoven attri- butes certain illusions to differences of distinctness between images formed at the fovea and on the peripheral retina. It may, perhaps, be said that since our knowledge of all the factors which are involved in judgment of size is, even at the physiological level, imperfect, the explanations put forward are rather of interest as intellectual exercises than as com- plete interpretations of the phenomena. One interesting problem must be mentioned, viz. the reason why the moon on the horizon looks larger than when she is at the zenith, a question which has been touched on by such proficients as Berkeley, Gauss, and Helmholtz. The experiments of Zoth appear to prove that the influence of aerial perspective, of the comparison with objects of known size, and of the apparent curvature of the sky, are not the main determining factors in the illusion. Zoth holds that the apparently smaller size of the moon at the zenith depends on the necessary elevation of the eye. Guttman's experiments, which demonstrate an 228 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES apparent diminution in size of 3^ to 3| per cent, in objects 25 to 36 cm. distant placed so that the line of vision has an elevation of 40 degrees, confirm this view, but only push the difficulty one stage farther back. The association of upward gaze and diminution in size may possibly be due to the con- vergence which occurs when the eyes look upwards (Zoth). Binocular Vision The most obvious advantage of using both eyes is the increased power we notice of observing the solidity of objects, but before we examine the physiological basis of this we must consider some of the data which help us to understand the joint working of the eyes. Since whenever we turn our eyes to a point a separate image of it is formed on each retina, it is a remarkable thing that we see not two points, but one only. This remarkable and, so far as we are concerned, ultimate fact can be expressed by the statement that the retinse con- tain areas or points which " correspond," viz. the simultaneous excitation of which calls up a single sensation. We also know that when the ordinary mutual arrangement of the eyes is disturbed, e.g. by pushing one eyeball with the finger, a single object gives rise to two images. Hence, any two retinal areas do not indifferently " correspond " ; some retinal points are disparate. The first problem is, accordingly, to determine how the "corresponding" points lie with reference to some fixed retinal point taken as origin, or, in other words, to map out the binocular field of single vision. The locus of the external points, images of which fall on " corresponding " points of the two retina, is called the Horopter, or, more strictly, the Point Horopter, and it can be investigated in numerous ways. Theoretically, it might seem an easy task to map out the horopter experimentally ; a point might be fixed and a small object moved round on a peri- meter, the positions being noted in which it was seen as a single point or as two points. Again, a haploscopic arrange- ment can be used, mirrors capable of rotation on different axes being adapted to the purpose. Unfortunately these ex- THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" 229 periments are very difficult to carry out ; they require a highly trained subject as well as an experienced observer, for recog- nition of double images (diplopia) is only easy in extreme cases. It has, therefore, been more usual to determine the horopter theoretically, using a few simple experimental data as a starting point. The data used are the following : — Simple observation appears to show that symmetrical points of the two retinae "correspond," e.g. that when a point on the right retina 1 millimetre to the left of the fovea in the horizontal meridian and a point 1 millimetre to the left of the left fovea in the same meridian are stimulated, that a single image is seen. Accepting this datum, the pro- blem reduces itself to finding the locus of external points, the images of which are formed on symmetrical areas of the retina and becomes purely geometrical. The geometrical problem is indeed somewhat complex, because inter alia the eyes can be moved in so many directions, but it is one which a highly skilled geometer could solve without ever leaving his study. It will be observed that two assumptions are made in stating the problem in this form, viz. (1) the stimula- tion of symmetrical points is always attended by single vision ; (2) the stimulation of asymmetrical points does not call up a single image. Of these assumptions, (1) is probably but not certainly true, (2) is almost certainly false. It thus follows that the geometrical horopter does not correspond accurately to the physiological one, but it is true that the divergence is not, so far as we know, very considerable. I do not think, however, that I should be justified in reproducing the difficult investigation by which the theoretical horopter has been obtained, and it is possible that even a statement of its results will not be quite clear to all readers. Helmholtz found that the point horopter is in general a curve of double curvature, and can be regarded as the section of two surfaces of the second order, which have in addition a common straight line. In a particular case only is the point horopter a plane surface, viz. when the fixation point lies at infinity in the mesial plane, and the horizontal meridia of the retinse are in the visual plane. Under these conditions the horopteric plane is either perpendicular to the line of sight and at 230 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES infinity, or parallel to the visual plane and passes through the line of intersection of the planes of the apparently vertical meridian ; for normal eyes the line of section and the horopteric plane nearly coincide with the plane of the ground on which the observer stands. When we consider not points but lines the problem becomes still more complex, and can- not be even summarised in an elementary treatise. Fig. 25. We must now touch on the rules which appear to govern the projection of objects viewed with both eyes. The most important empirical law is that worked out by Hering, and is very simple to understand. Let A and B represent the right and left eyes, and A^ and Bj their nodal points, C an imaginary "cyclopean" eye, and Cj^ its nodal point. Let D be the point to which the eyes converge, so that its images are formed on corresponding points. Join DAp DBp and DCp and produce to intersect THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE' 231 the retinae. Then the point D appears along the line DC^. In a similar way the localisation of double images is clear.^ We now come to the factors which render the two eyes working together more efficient than the separate organs for discriminating sizes. Of these, convergence and binocular parallax are the most important. With regard to the former, the experiments of Wundt, Bourdon, and others appear to justify the following statement : Convergence affords a better means of measuring distance than do changes in the state of ^B Fig. 26. accommodation, yet the information so obtained does not rank in importance with that due to parallax. The importance of parallax, the apparent difference in an object viewed from two different points in the special case of binocular vision, was first described by Wheatstone in 1838. Let AB (Fig. 26) be the horizontal section of a plane at right angles to the plane of the paper, which we will regard as the visual plane, P and Q the mid-points of the lines of vision of the two eyes, and S a point looked at. E and T are the projections of S on AB. Let a be the distance between the eyes and c the distance from R to T, & the distance 1 When D is fixated, the images of the point E are formed at Ej and Ej. (Fi) and (Ej) are the points which correspond to Fj and Ej upon the Cyclopean Eetina. Join (Fj) and (Ej) to G and produce to intersect EFj and EE^. The image due to the right eye is projected along (Fi)H, i.e. there is Uncrossed Diplopia. In a similar way, we find the image of the nearer point F due to the right eye is projected to the left — Crossed Diplopia. 232 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES of AB from PQ, / the distance from S to PQ, and d the distance from S to AB. Since the triangles PSQ and EST are similar, c a , ad d=f andc = j. a — c = e, can be called the stereoscopic distance (Helmholtz). Further, b a — c be bn j= — or e=j- = -f. a c d, J Both c and e diminish when / increases, and vanish when / is infinite ; in other words, there is no binocular parallax for very distant objects. Both c and e increase with the inter- ocular distances, c increases as the distance of the object from the plane of projection, e as the distance of the projec- tion plane from the eyes. Many experimental determinations of the fineness of depth perception have been carried out. Helmholtz measured how far the central one of three needles must be moved Bourdon's Experiments on Judgment of Distance. [The Plane of the Fixed Needles was 2 meters from the Eyes.] [The Needles were 35 mm. apart.] Results of Twenty Trials. | Position of the Middle Keedle. "Nearer." The same Distance." " Farther." r 3-0 20 2'5 20 In front of the others 2-0 1-5 1-0 19 18 17 1 2 3 •5 11 9 [ -0 2 18 r -5 3 15 2 1-0 13 7 Behind the 1-5 11 9 others ' 2-0 3 17 2'5 20 i 3-0 1 19 forwards or backwards for a difference to be noticed. He found that a needle 34 centimetres from the eyes when THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" 233 moved through half a millimetre, approximately its thick- ness, backwards or forwards from the plane of the others, could be detected with certainty as being no longer in that plane. Bourdon's experiments reveal even greater accuracy. Calculation on the basis of Bourdon's work shows that a diiference in position of the retinal images corre- sponding to five angular seconds is appreciable. In ordinary observation of a solid body we are accustomed to change the fixation point frequently, and it has been thought that these eye movements are of great importance in spatial perception. It is, however, fairly certain that although such movements are not without value, their importance in comparison with binocular parallax is secondary. Stereoscopy I shall conclude this chapter with a few words descriptive of a matter which, although not a part of the physiology of visible space, is germane to it, viz. the theory of the stereoscope. If it be true that binocular parallax is the main founda- tion of what we call solidity or depth, it should follow that when two pictures of an object taken from the point of view of each eye are so presented to the binocular combination that the images are made to fuse, an impression of solidity is produced. This deduction, first made by Wheatstone, has been the foundation of optical experiments which have produced instruments of great interest and practical use. Photographic pictures for stereoscopy are taken with objectives somewhat farther apart than the average distance between the eyes, and accordingly represent such effects as would be produced naturally by rather nearer objects than those which they represent, i.e. there is an exaggeration of the relief. Owing to the fact that the observed points lie in a plane, the state of accommodation associated with the convergence of the optic axes is not quite appropriate ; there is not a com- plete reproduction of the natural state of affairs. To unite the two pictures Wheatstone employed an arrangement of mirrors, but Brewster's device of two prisms (Fig. 27) has 234 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES proved more satisfactory. The older instruments were mostly box-shaped, light being admitted through a slit in the front wall, but these have been largely replaced D P^ J>, ^-' A and B The Eyes. C and Ci Prisms. D and D, Stereoscopic Pictures. E Partition. F Apparent situation of Blended Picture. Fig. 27. by open instruments, such as the admirable hand stereoscope of the Zeiss works. Very useful applications of the stereoscope have resulted from modifications of the distance between the eyes; the principle of one of these is shown in the diagram (Fig. 28). Supposing, now, that one places in the image planes of a binocular telescope two glass plates with a series of corre- sponding signs, in accordance with the ocular distance (modified by and a constant of the instrument) ; some object is fixated, and one notices at which point in the series of marks it is seen stereoscopically. Each of the pairs of marks corresponds to a definite distance (depending on the cor- rected ocular distance), and in this way the distance of the object can be directly read off. This is the principle of the Stereotelemeter made by Zeiss ; the accuracy of their smaller THE PHYSIOLOGY OF "SPACE" 235 instrument is ± 5 cm. for a distance of 20 metres, +31-3 m. for 500 metres. Their larger instrument reads to 440 m. in 10 kilometres. If two absolutely identical pictures or impressions on a plane surface are examined with a stereoscope, the fused impression, as we should expect, is once more that of a flat surface. If, however, the two patterns are not absolutely B and J5, The Eyes. B and B, Small Mirrors inclined at an angle of 45° to the mesial ©plane of the observer. C and C| Large Mirrors parallel to E A and £?,. -^ By this arrangement an effect is produced much as if the eyes were situated at A and B. Fig. 28. 6 J3 identical, then, in accordance with the principles of stereo- scopic vision an imperfect appearance of relief, an obliquely vaulted image is obtained. Medals stamped with the same die but from different metals, although indistinguishable to touch or by the naked eye, have been found to produce this effect (Dove) ; different editions of printed books and forged bank-notes can also be detected in this way. Eor such purposes, a modified stereoscope, Pulfrich's Stereocom- parator, has been introduced with satisfactory results ; indeed, the adaptations and modifications of Wheatstone's instru- ment are numerous, and will in time be more so. The brief sketch presented in this chapter will, perhaps, help the reader to form an idea of the diverse fields of intellectual research which are touched on by the physio- logical study of spatial magnitude ; he will find that almost any of the branches of inquiry which each part of the subject 236 PHYSIOLOGY OF THE SPECIAL SENSES suggests can be made the source of great pleasure and in- struction. RECOMMENDED FOR FURTHER STUDY (i) The masterly article on Space in the second volume of Professor William Jomes' Principles of Psycholocry should first te mastered. Next come (2) Helmholtz's treatise, and (3) Hering's article in Hermann's Handb. d. Phys., Bk. iii. part 1. (4) An admirable summary of the physiological data relating to visual space is given in Zotlt'a article in Nagel's Handb., vol. iii. INDEX Acuity — Auditory, 72 Visual, 222-3 Adaptation — Theory of Visual, 118 Thermal, 15 Visual, 101-112 After-images, 155 Burch on, 163 Goethe on, 156 Hering on, 198 Positive, 156 White Values of, 162 Analysis, Cochleal, 81 Answers, Method of Right Wrong, 4 Beat Tones, 76 Beats, 75 Blue Sightedness, 137 Canals, Semicircular, 58 Cochlea, 80 Analytical Theory of, 81 Coefficient Rule, 159 Colour — ■ Absorption by Macula Lens, 136 Complementary, 135 Memory, 125 Mixing, 128 Real and Accidental, 125 Specific Brightness of, 195 Triangle, 131 Vision, 124-214 Colour-blindness — Artificial, 184 and and 194, Colour-blindness (continued) — Blue- Yellow, 152 Goethe on, 139 Green-Red, 141 Tests for, 146 Theories of, 182, 189 Total, 107, 108, 111 Cones, Vision of, 120 Contrast — Binocular, 209 Colour, 207 Smell, 44 Spatial Efi'ect of, 205 Taste, 37 Temporal Succession of, 206 Theory of, 209 Visual, 203-214 White-Black, 207 Corti's Organ, 81 Cutaneous Sensations, 8-24 Cyclopean Eye, 230 Deaf Mutism, 59 Depth, Perception of, 224 Deuteranopia, 142 Dichromatism, 139 Difference, Smallest Percep- tible, 4 Diplopia, 229 Direction, Sense of, 49 Donders' Law, 221 Dove, Vision of, 117 Ear, Anatomy of, 77. See also Hearing Epicritic Sensations, 28 Error, Mean, Method of, 4 Eustachian Tube, 80 237 238 INDEX Fechner's Law, 5 Fehlpunkt, 150, 153 "Flicker "Method, 103 Hearing, 64-85 Anaxagoras on, 65 Aristotle on, 66 Democritus on, 65 Empedocles on, 65 Plato on, 65 Horopter, 228 Illusions — Of Position and Movement, 52, 55 Visual, 229 Image — After-, 155 Satellite, 113 Secondary, 113 Tertiary, 113 Induction — Simultaneous, 203 Successive, 155 Labyrinth — • Ewald's Experiments, 58 Goltz, Theory of, 60 Mach-Breuer Theory, 60 Leech, Vision of, 92 Limen. See Threshold Values Listing's Law, 221 Mach-Breuer Theory, 60 Meniere's Disease, 59 Meyer's Experiment, 204 Movement, Sense of, 54 Muller's Law, 2 Muscular Sense, 55 Neutral Points, 145 Night Blindness, 119 Olfactometer, 39 Otolithic Organs, 51 Owl, Vision of, 117 Pain — Points, 19 Referred, 21 Visceral, 21 Parallax, Binocular, 231 Perception — Of Pitch, Limiting Values, 71 Spatial, Berkeley on, 216, 217 Position, Sense of, 52 Protanopia, 142 Protapathic Sensation, 28 Purkinje's Phenomenon, 101 Purple, Visual, 98 Eetina— Chemical Reaction of, 97 Electric Response of, 96 Phototropic Response of, 97 " Retuning," 158 Rod Vision, 118, 120 Sensations — Auditory and Labyrinthine, 48-84 Cutaneous, 8-18, 24r-35, 70- 86 Gustatory, 34-39 Olfactory, 39-47 Visceral and Deep, 21, 29 Visual, 101-163, 203-209 Shape, Judgment of, 226 Smell- Antagonistic Stimuli, 43 Biological Importance, 46 Examination of, 39 Memory and, 46 Paths of Afferent Impulses, 45 Snellen's Test, 222 Space — Lucretius on, 215 Tactual, 218 Visual, 219-236 Spectrum — Twilight Value of, 110 Use in Colour Testing, 128, 134, 141 INDEX 239 Stereocomparator, 235 Stereoscope, 233 Stereotelemeter, 234 Taste — Examination of, 36 Gasserian Ganglion and, 38 Greek Theories of, 35 Paths of Afferent Impulses, 38 Temperature Sensations — Localisation, 14 Theories of, 14, 15, 26-33 Threshold Values, 3 Tones — Beat, 76 Compound, 74 Touch, Sensations of — Aristotle on, 8 Examination of, 9, 10 Localisation of, 11 Organs of, 10 Triangle, Colour, 131 Trichromatism, 133 Unlust, 19 Vertigo, 55 Vision {see also Contrast, Colour- blindness, Space) — Adaptation in, 118 Anaxagoras on, 169 Aristotle on, 171 Binocular, 228 Comparative Physiology of; 86- 95 Cone, 118, 120 Dichromatic, 139 Empedocles on, 167 Foveal and Peripheral. See Adaptation Bering's Theory of, 189-203 Monocular, 221 Plato on, 170 Recurrent, 112 Rod, 118, 120 Trichromatic, 124 Twilight, 110 Young-Helmholtz Theory of, 177-189 Weber's Law, 3 Yellow Sightedness, 137 THE END Printed by Ballantyne, Hanson &' Co. Edinbursb &* London