■1:V ' V f'j- ^X\: -^ w "<- -^ ■ HV2561.C5T" ""'""'""■"■"'' *,ifK™.,^,^P°''' °' ••'^ Department of Ch 3 1924 013 873 777 Chicago Public Sclioois ; 3 S p> cial Report of the Department of CHILD STUDY AND PE DAGOGI C INVeSTIGATION ON Children Attending the Public Day-Schools for the Deaf in Chicago D. P. MacMILLAN, Ph. D., Director r. C. BRUNER, Ph. D., AssUlant 0fac OMI0A4O NCWBFAPSR UNION, OHIOAttO An Enlarged and Revised EdKIon of a Preliminary Report Submitted to the Board of Education and Printed on Nay 25, 1906 Cornell University Library 3ji The original of tiiis book is in tine Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924013873777 CHICAGO PUBLIC SCHOOLS A SPECIAL REPORT OF THE DEPARTMENT OF Child Study and Pedagogic Investigation ON CHILDREN ATTENDING THE PUBLIC DAY-SCHOOLS FOR THE DEAF IN CHICAGO D. P. MAG MILLAN, Ph. D. Director F. G. BRUNER, Ph. D., Assistant An Enlarged and Revised Edition of a Preliminary Report Submitted to the Board of Educa;tion and Printed on May 25, 1906. ^S3 HI ^1d TABLE OF CONTENTS. Page. Letter of Transmittal 3 Introduction 7 Hearing 9 Classification 11 Speech 16 Visual Acuity 21 PHYSICAL CONDITION: Stature 25 Weight 28 Head Measurements 28 Lung Capacity 32 Strength of Grip 36 Grip Index 38 Motor Ability 41 Motor Index 45 MENTALITY: Imitation and Suggestibility 48 Perception 49 "A" Test ' 49 Shot-sizing Test 57 Lifted Weights 64 Memory 70 Amenability to Training 72 SUMMARY AND SUGGESTIONS 74 LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL. Chicago, October 12, 1908. Mr. E. G. Cooley, Superintendent of Schools — Dear Sir: I submit herewith a report on the deaf children of the pubHc schools of Chicago, and ask that a sufficient num- ber of copies be printed at the earliest convenience. The subject- matter has been on our hands now for a long time, and on ac- count of the large number of requests for the information col- lected by us, as well as the intrinsic interest and value of this data for our teachers in service, I believe the whole subject ought to be put in available form for use. An account of our dealings with the special study of the deaf children is given in a formal way in this letter of transmittal to you. The attention of the Department of Child Study and Peda- gogic Investigation was first officially drawn to deaf children in the public schools of Chicago by action of the Board of Educa- tion at the opening of school in September, 1902. At this time the Superintendent of Schools placed the matter before the Committee on School Management, and on September 3, 1902, the minutes of the Board indicate the following course of action : "The Committee on School Management reports that there are in the School for the Deaf Mutes quite a number of children who might be more properly taken care of in the schools for the feeble-minded. As the task of separating the feeble-minded from the normal children in these schools is a delicate one, the committee recommends that the Superintendent be authorized to employ the teachers in the Child Study Department to aid him in determining whether they should be excluded from the public schools or not." (Proc. 1902, Page 70.) A report on these children was submitted to the committee on October 2, 1902, which showed that a psycho-physical ex- amination had been given to 160 pupils in the rooms for deaf children, among whom were found 16 children who were notice- ably or markedly subnormal in mentality and were accordingly 4 LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL recommended for exclusion from the public schools. Since that time it has been the policy to have each child undergo a psycho- physical examination by the Department of Child Study, as soon as the parents of the child make application for his admittance to these rooms for deaf children. In two or three instances the Supervisor had been prevailed upon to admit to these rooms, when the classes were exceptionally small, certain children who were practically deaf for ordinary school exercises and afflicted with speech defects, for the reason that they would gain excep- tional benefit from the kind of training which the teachers of these children alone could give. On account of the time-worn contention among parents of deaf children as to the relative superiority of the lip-reading or oral and the manual methods of teaching the deaf, and especially as to the alleged non-adaptability of the oral method to any children except those of more than ordinary brightness, and to those with some p»ower of hearing, it so happened that the whole matter was again brought before the Committee on School Man- agement at the beginning of the school year 1904-1905. No definite action was taken by the Board until the following communication was received October 26, 1904, from one of its members which finally was effective in bringing about the more comprehensive and formal report herein embodied. "... Since the last meeting I have visited several deaf and dumb schools, and I found them very much neglected. In one case I found two children who were not deaf or dumb, and had no right to be placed with these children. I also found at least thirty or forty per cent of the children under normal, as well as deaf and dumb, and was informed that teaching these children the oral system was a waste of time on account of their impaired mental condition. I also found teachers with classes so large that it is impossible for one teacher to make progress with all the children — naturally more attention was given to the brighter children and the others were neglected. Therefore I think it the duty of this Board to thoroughly investigate the entire mat- ter and where there are children who cannot learn the oral sys- tem, the combined alphabet or the sign system should be taught in special rooms in schools other than where the oral system is LETTER OF TRANSMITTAL 5 taught. I also think more time should be given to the teaching of reading, writing and arithmetic, as these studies seem to me to be the most essential to a deaf and dumb person. . . I trust your honorable body will give the matter the most thorough con- sideration and investigation, as this department of the public school system is much neglected and should have a thorough reorganization, better rooms, more teachers and more attention in general given to the children afflicted by the loss of hearing and speech." On January i8, 1905, a committee consisting of the Super- intendent of Schools, the Director of the Child Study Depart- ment and three medical inspectors was appointed to investigate the whole matter and to report its findings to the Board of Edu- cation. In accordance with the usual routine of such matters the whole inquiry was referred by the Superintendent of Schools to the Department of Child Study and Pedagogic Investigation, and on April 12, 1905, a brief summary of the results was sub- mitted in a preliminary report. The only action taken on the matter at this time was to refer it back to the investigators with instructions to present the data in detail. The final report herein submitted was placed in the hands of the Superintendent of Schools, subject to call from the Committee on School Manage- ment, which demand finally was made on May 25, 1906, where- upon it was ordered printed at once, and copies distributed among the members of the Board of Education and educators of the city. The original form in which it was printed by the Secretary of the Board was, in certain features, abbreviated and popular- ized and as such it fulfilled a temporary and local service, but owing to the large number of requests for copies and inquiries from school boards of other places, from educational governing bodies and from professional and general libraries in this and other countries, as well as the desirability of placing our re- sults in permanent form for our teachers, it seems highly advis- able to print the whole subject-matter in a form which will more adequately meet the larger demand. Very respectfully yours, D. P. MAC MILLAN, Director. EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES OF DEAF CHILDREN. It is doubtful if more than a very small minority of persons who have not had intimate dealings with deaf children are familiar with the characteristics which differentiate this special group from hearing children. One meets with all sorts of opin- ions and notions concerning their capabilities or incapacities. It is held by some that the senses of deaf children, other than hearing, are exceptionally acute, and again the assertion is made that children who are defective in one sense are deficient in all the senses and for this reason they speak of the mind of the deaf child as of a distinctly lower type. This study is not, indeed, concerned with all the deaf child- ren in Chicago, nor with all deaf children of school age. It deals with deaf children attending the public day schools whom this department considered of high enough grade of mentality to be educable by the special class methods of the public schools for the deaf. These deaf children have been admitted into these special rooms of instruction upon recommendation, after being subjected to a careful psycho-physical examination to determine their mental status and the degree of hearing power which they possessed. The feeble-minded and those of extreme sub-normal mentality are not incorporated in our results. It was, then, our object to carefully examine and estimate the comparative physi- cal and mental status and to make an extended study of the progress and school conditions of the deaf children in the public day schools of Chicago. Some three months were devoted to this study. Children were taken in groups of two or three in a private room in which conditions were similar to those of the classroom, and in all about one or two hours was given to each child. This length of time was found necessary on account of the difficulty of get- ting responses from this class of children, who are naturally so excessively timid and hesitant in manner. This it is natural to 8 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES expect on account of the difference in their social life, which is usually confined to intercourse with parents, teachers or children similarly afflicted. Experience has shown that one must have at least a half-hour's intimate association with any child before he can be reasonably sure that the responses secured are anything at all like normal, and that the estimate made of the child's mental efficiency is not totally erroneous. If this be true of children in general, it is so true as to be axiomatic when deaf children are the subjects of examination. To estimate the men- tal status or educational possibilities of the deaf child requires not only special knowledge of their habits and characteristics, but also considerable intimacy with their environment and social conditions. A cursory examination will very frequently lead one to adjudge a child as of sub-normal mentality, whereas a greater familiarity with the disparities and mental traits of this class of children will enable one to secure surprising results. It was found advantageous, in order to overcome the reticence of these children, to make the first examination brief and preliminary, so that the children might become personally acquainted with the examiners and confident in all their modes of expression. In order to investigate the hearing and speech possibilities and progress of deaf children, it is indispensable that we have some knowledge of the sensory, nervous and bodily conditions which are intimately related to both hearing and speech. Thus, it is necessary to know something of the sight, as good vision is necessary to good lip-reading. It is necessary to know whether the defect in hearing is local or whether it is due to gen- eral lack of sensitiveness. Likewise it is of value to know whether the deaf child is less or more sensitive to impressions of touch, weight, etc. In case these senses are obtuse, one might well conclude that the deficiency in aural acuity is primarily due to general dullness of the whole psychical system. If the other senses are acute, the defect is probably due to the organ of hearing and not to obtuse mentality. To acquire speech it is of the first importance to have good organs of speech. The nose and throat must be open and free. The naso-pharyngeal cavity must be examined for enlarged tonsils and adenoid growths. Something must be known of the hard and soft palate in their DEAF CHILDREN 9 relation to middle-ear deafness and breathing power, as well as the condition of the tongue, both as to structure and mobility. The general nervous condition of children very markedly in- fluences their speech and mental ability. The same nervous con- ditions which induce such defects as stammering, stuttering, hes- itancy, lisping and lalling are as likely to occur in deaf children as in hearing children. Then, too, there are such nervous affec- tions as chorea, nervous inertia and general incoordinations. These especially influence mentality and speech.^ The report of the Child Study Department for the year 1899- 1900 showed that those children who were larger, stronger quicker and better nourished were mentally superior to those who were not so well developed. The same conditions which favor physical superiority are found to have a like or comparable influence on the mental side. It therefore to a degree not only affords a convenient but a serviceable cue to the mental status, to know how deaf children compare in size, strength, quickness, sensory and motor defects with hearing children of the same age. In the case of a child having no language, it is difficult to gain much information directly of his mental ability. It is, how- ever, possible to measure his efficiency in such simple processes as perception, and, in case a child possesses a few symbols, it is possible to learn something of his immediate sense memory. It was our aim to employ only simple tests to study all these various phases of the sensory, physical, nervous and higher mental life of these children, in order to sec-jre cumulative evidence of the exact nature of their hearing defects and of their mental and physical conditions as these are related to the acquiring of speech and school training. HEARING. METHOD OF CONDUCTING THE TESTS. Three instruments were used in the examination of hearing power : First: The Audiometer. The instrument used was an ordinary Seashore audiometer. This is, in brief, a simple elec- trical apparatus for producing a measurable noise which is brought about by the making and breaking of a secondary cur- rent. This noise is transmitted to the ear of the person tested lO EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES by a telephone receiver held tightly against the ear. The child is tested with his back to the examiner and with eyes closed. The instrument was devised for testing differences in aural acuity of persons not ordinarily considered defective in hearing. In many cases its range is too limited for grading hearing power when this power is not of sufificient acuity to detect speaking tones of the human voice. The hearing power of children can be conveniently graded according to the norms for hearing acuity, worked out with normal children and already reported on. (See Report No. 2, Department of Child Study and Pedagogical In- vestigation.) Second: "Snapper." This little instrument when snapped between the forefinger and thumb, makes a mechanical snap of sufficient loudness to be heard by the human ear of average acuity in a quiet medium at a distance of about 500 feet. It is held by the experimenter slightly behind the ear of the child examined, out of range of vision either direct or lateral, and the child is asked to count the clicks, or in case of the younger children, the starting of the body entire, and the super- ficial reflexes, especially the conjunctival, are observed. By this means the hearing power of certain children could be gauged who could not be reached by the more delicate instrument. Third: Differential Tube. This piece of apparatus consists of a tube of soft rubber whose internal diameter is 4 mm. and whose length is 100 cm., fitted with hard rubber ear pieces to be inserted into the ears, one end into the child's ear and the other into the ear of the examiner. This ear piece was held firmly between the first, finger and the thumb, and care was taken that the stimulation was not communicated through the sense of touch. A tuning fork of pitch "a" was struck on a hard sub- stance and its base applied to the tube. First, in order to famil- iarize the child with the sound, the fork was struck briskly on a hard object and its base placed on the child's front teeth. Only after the child becatae somewhat familiar with the sound thus conducted, was recourse had to air conduction. This instrument proved adaptable for testing hearing power of even lower grade than the snapper, owing largely to the fact that the stimulus had a greater duration, thus affording a better opportunity for the element of attention to enter. DEAK CHILDREN II This work with instruments was further supplemented by tests made with the human voice in ordinary speaking tones and in shouting, taking care when within close range that the child was made as nearly as possible dependent upon the sense of hearing. CLASSIFICATION. The hearing ability of this group of children somewhat con- veniently is divided into five classes. This division is purely conventional and practical inasmuch as it is formulated wholly on the basis of school room efficiency and the value which must be attributed to the power of hearing in its relation to the acqui- sition of oral speech. In the first column, Class i, are shown the pupils who are totally deaf — insensitive to the strongest sound vibrations which we had any means of employing. In the second column. Class 2, are presented those who are practically deaf, i. e., only sounds which are continuous and most intense can be heard. It is very probable that their hearing can never be developed sufficiently to hear the human voice even with the assistance of mechanical devices. In column three, Qass 3, is given the number of those child- ren who possess a degree of hearing power — they hear loud sounds, but yet they have not sufficient sensitivity and control of hearing to understand vocal speech. The children of this class offer hope that with the assistance of mechanical devices for intensifying the voice, their hearing power may be developed so that advantage may be taken of this normal means of ac- quiring spoken language. Qass 4. Those pupils who are deaf for ordinary school conditions, are placed in column four of the table. In general, the pupils of this class hear only words spoken loudly and close to the ear and are therefore to be considered as deaf from the point of view of conditions that ordinarily prevail in the school- room. They do, however, possess sufficient hearing to assist them in acquiring speech when appeal is made to their under- standing through this sense avenue. In column five, Class 5, is listed a certain small number of hearing children who were temporarily in the rooms for the 12 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES •deaf and who are greatly in need of the training in articulation which it is not possible for them to get in our school system, €xcept from teachers who have had special training in the rudi- ments of voice culture. TABLE I. POWER OF HBABINO. Schools Class 1. Totally Deaf Class 2. Practically Deaf. Hear only most in- tense continu- ous sounds Class 3. Hear loud noises but can- not hear speech Class 4. Deaf for ordi- nary school purposes Class S. Hearingr chil- dren. In deaf rooms for speech training Holden 2 2 5 2 10 1 4 3 ? 4 11 1 1 2 4 2 1 2 1 1 8 2 8 2 7 4 8 3 4 3 5 17 5 1 2 1 3 6 1 1 1 5 2 2 1 1 2 Beidler Darwin Froebel Hammond Kozminski Normal Seward Yale Totals 55 33 53 25 8 Total number examined 174 Although thus classified, it must not be assumed that these children will always retain the exact relative order here pre- sented. With some the hearing power deteriorates, while the hearing power of others will show marked improvement owing to their general mental training in giving attention and in attaching significance to excitations of this sense organ. Even among those congenitally defective in hearing it must not be assumed that the hearing power will always remain stationary. Some children were in the deaf rooms who were regarded as born absolutely deaf and now at the age of fifteen or sixteen are able to hear conversation. This improvement usually accom- panies the great growth changes at the periods of second denti- tion and of puberty. There are certain children with varying degrees of deafness whose hearing has gradually improved ; there are other cases of deafness in which there is apparently no improvement. The congenitally deaf, however, form, as far as we are able to determine, only about 50 per cent of the total number, but it is difficult to draw a hard-and-fast line of de- markation between congenital cases and deafness as one of the DEAF CHILDREN IJ sequelae of disease. Many parents assign very insignificant causes for this affliction. In many other cases it is impossible to deter- mine just when dfeafness did occur, especially since scarlet fever, spinal meningitis and other febrile diseases sometimes are ex- perienced during the first year of the child's life before speech develops, and deafness is attributed to these, whereas as a matter of fact, it antedated than. The two most frequent maladies which leave deafness as one of the sequelae in their wake, as suggested above, are scarlet fever and spinal menin- gitis. The significance of this is manifold in that very frequently some tendency to mental aberration is also present, together with nervous disturbances and inco-ordinations. Although not feeble- minded always, these children so afflicted would be far below the normal in mentality even were their hearing normal. It is a matter of observation that deafness which occurs as a result of disease is as frequently attributed to some catarrhal condi- tion of the middle ear, atrophy of the ossicles of this region, or degeneration of the typmanic membrane, as to some internal ear trouble or lesion of the brain. Sometimes these diseases cause absolute loss of hearing. In other cases there is but slight diminution in auditory acuity. In every case it is very desir- able to know just where the defect lies, as well as the degree of deafness, in order to classify the child and suit his school train- ing not only to his present status but also to his future needs. Reference to Table I will show the relative proportion of children in each of the five classes. In the first class there are 55 ; in the second, 33 ; in the third, 53 ; in the fourth, 25. Were the number examined very large, extending into the thousands instead of 178, we might reasonably expect to find an equal number of children in each class ; that is, experimental evidence goes to show that there are varying degrees of deafness, rang- ing all the way from slight and temporary impairment of hearing due to a cold, to the stage of absolute and permanent silence. This, of course, necessitates some practical standard by which one must judge whether a child's hearing is sufficiently poor to justify his being assigned to the the rooms for the deaf. Where the recommendation has been made by the Child Study Department, the criterion has been a practical one, based not 14 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES alone upon the child's absolute power to hear, but also upon his inability to carry on the work successfully in the rooms with hearing children. However deaf a child might be, he was never recommended for the deaf classes as long as his progress in the rooms with hearing pupils was satisfactory. Likewise in those cases where the defect was more largely one of mentality in- stead of defective hearing it has been the policy of the Child Study Department to recommend that the child be excluded from the rooms for the deaf. The above criterion seems the only one usable in actual practice. Among normal hearing children, speech is an instinctive ac- quisition which shows itself at a relatively definite time in mental ripening. The activities and speech of all persons in the home are a constant appeal to the young and yearning listener, and so there come to be set up mental associations between his acts of hearing and his vocal responses. The activities of seeing, hearing, touching, etc., which at first operate somewhat inde- pendently of each other, come to be firmly and constantly con- nected, so that one sense organ-act stands for the activity of another sense-process or group of such processes. Seeing, comes to stand for touching. Hearing means doing or going to do. The child hears his own prattle as well as the speech of others about him. Others attribute meaning to his spontaneous bab- blings and his special acts, and, in the initial steps at least, his acts are named for him. These activities in which he indulges and to which others give specific values and names, are at first gross and inchoate, with meanings as vague, but gradually become with his growth of consciousness, more definite and precise. In this way a certain word or name heard is associated with some object reacted upon and the child's natural babblings are made to conform to the word heard. Thus the child, through his own hearing and with very small effort, catches the cues which lead to his articulation of the words he has heard spoken by those about him. But with the deaf child, the process of acquir- ing speech is by no means so simple. No sounds are heard, or if sensed at all, are heard very indistinctly, so that the procedtire becomes one in which intense and concentrated attention is nec- essary to succeed at all. DEAF CHILDREN 1 5 The most direct inherited associations are between speech and hearing, and not between speech and sight, as must be the procedure in the case of those whose power of hearing is ab- sent or very defective. It is always very difficult to divert na- ture's course, but this does not imply that it is never advisable as exigencies arise to modify her laws or methods of procedure. When limitations have been set to one group of associations, it is absolutely indispensable to form a new set of associations if training of any part is to take place, and this re-direction of an instinctive procedure is not accomplished without considerable effort. Intercourse between individuals is possible only by some language medium, and a language medium implies some sort of symbols which refer directly to objects, activities or events, and it further implies that the symbols used have a common meaning to all using them. All articulatory symbols of intercourse are relatively arbi- trary and conventional, and their expression must occur either, as is commonly the case, by the natural way of imitating with the vocal mechanism what is received through the auditory appa- ratus — in which case the symbols are expressed spontaneously — or the meaning of these symbols must be consciously acquired and their expression laboriously worked out, as in the case of the deaf. In the latter there must occur a definite adjustment of mouth-parts and chest activity on the one hand, or the manipulation of the hands on the other, according as the vocal or manual language is used. One must be learned as well as the other. The selection and preference of one method over another, must be decided on other than psychological grounds, inasmuch as psychology properly defined, deals with a descrip- tion and explanation of all mental processes, and so every men- tal reaction, the true and serviceable as well as the false and trivial, must have its history as to its origin, characteristics and function. Both utility in social relations and facility or ease of acquirement must largely determine whether one or the other method of training the deaf shall be used in the school- room. There is the further question as to whether the manual method has certain limitations as compared with the oral in l6 EXPERIMENTAU STUDIES developing the mind. But this is a question that must be de- cided, for the present at least, on theoretical and philosophical grounds. Thus far there have been no scientific investigations on adults, trained by these two methods, which might tend to show that the range of expressive thought is greater or less in one method than the other. The philosophical problem is one that does not concern us here, 'for it leads to nothing more than the expression of an opinion, and offers no hope for the solution of the question. SPEECH. There are found in the rooms for the deaf, children who had no speech when deafness occurred. There are others who had learned to speak a little, and again, there are a few others whose deafness appeared after five years of age and who possessed con- siderable power of speech. It obviously would not be just to group all these together. Therefore the children are distrib- uted into three groups on the basis of their capacity for appre- ciating and using speech, taking into account the time they have been receiving instruction. We have then as a mater of fact the following groups: (i) those whose speech is normal or nearly so; (2) those whose speech is defective either in the quality of tone of the vocal sounds or in the articulation of the consonants, nasals, labials, sibilants, etc.; and (3) those who have no speech or are incapable of uttering intelligently at least fifty words. Of course all children who had lost their hearing be- fore they had learned to talk would be in group 3 on first en- tering school. The problem, then, is that of determining the character of the speech that has been acquired for the different periods that the children have been in school. In keeping with this, the children in the various rooms have been distributed accord- ing to their efficiency in speech after they have been in school three, five, seven and ten years. It must be borne in mind that these groups do not include all the children in the rooms for the deaf, since obviously there is a large number of them who for various reasons have not been in school for as long a period as three years. Furthermore, not only must the length of time the pupil has been in school be considered but also his condition DEAF CHILDREN 1 7 on entrance into school. Under those three groups, therefore, of children whose speech is nonnal, whose speech is defective, and those whose speech is practically wanting, it was thought advisable to indicate the following ( See Table II, p. 19) : I. Children w'ho have sufficient hearing to assist them in acquiring speech. II. Pupils whose hearing was lost after the age of five years — i. e., after quite a degree of facility in speech had been gained before deafness occurred. III. Pupils who lost hearing before five years of age — i. e., where an opportunity had been afforded for the loss of auditory imagery and the fading of muscular habits associated with articulation and phonation. IV. Those born deaf, or those in whom the loss of hearing occurred so early in life that speech had never been acquired. THREE YEARS IN SCHOOL. Of deaf children who have had speech training for no longer than three years, we can make the following summary : 24, speech normal, or nearly so. 23, speech more or less defective — i. e., have considerable speech, but the articulation or tonal quality of the voice is more or less defective. 7, speech wanting — i. e., from the practical standpoint, little or no speech. It must be understood that where speech is herein spoken of as more or less defective, the child can usually be easily un- derstood by almost everybody, and in all cases can be under- stood by those who are accustomed to associate with him, though his voice, to be sure, is unpleasant and unnatural. Oi these twenty-four whose speech was found to be normal, it is interesting to learn that six children possessed some usable remnants of hearing, while 18 were deaf. Of the 18 deaf at least ID had lost their hearing after the age of five years, leav- ing only 6 who were afflicted with deafness before the age, of five and 2 congenitally deaf. That is, we may say, that normal speech had been taught to 8 of the 54 children by the indirect method during the brief period of three years. Turning to the 23, w'liose speech was understandable but l8 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES somewhat defective, the data show, that 3 had some hearing and 3 had lost their hearing after the age of five, 9 deaf chil- dren had lost their hearing earlier than the age of five and 8 were born deaf. Of the 7 who possessed little or no speech ability after three years in school, none had ever had hearing to be of any service in the acquisition of an articulatory language, 4 having been congenitally deaf and 3 having lost their hearing early in life. To summarize then if we exclude from consideration first, those who have sufficient hearing to assist them in acquiring speech and those who lost their hearing after five years of age, which two groups obviously form a separate class, we have remaining those who lost their hearing before the age of five and those who were born deaf. Of these we find : 8, normal speech. 17, speech more or less defective. 7, speech wanting. FIVE OR SIX YEARS IN SCHOOL. Turning now to the children who have been in school five or six years, we find the cases distributed as follows : 13, speech normal. 14, speech more or less defective, o, speech wanting. Considerable interest attaches to a comparison of the make up of the children of these three groups with those of three year school instruction. (See Table II.) Whereas, among the lat- ter but 8 of 24 with normal speech had lost their hearing earlier than five years, or were congenitally deaf, of those with 5 or 6 years of school instruction, 10 of the 13 belonged to this class; all of those with somewhat defective speech had lost their hear- ing early in life ; and no child of 5 and 6 years' school instruction was found without speech sufficient to communicate his simpler needs. Again to summarize the results, excluding as above, that special class comprising those who have some hearing and those 'who lost their hearing later than the age of five, we have those DEAF CHILDREN 19 classified as born deaf and having lost their hearing before the age of five — of these there are: 10, speech normal. 14, speech more or less defective, o, speech wanting. SEVEN OR MORE YEARS IN SCHOOL. Taking up those children who have been in school seven or more years, we find the following distribution. (See Table II.) 13, Speech normal. 9, Speech more or less defective, o, Speech wanting. One boy among those with normal speech had hearing and was temporarily given a place in the rooms for the deaf to rem- edy his very defective articulation. Of the remaining 12, only 2 had lost their hearing later than five years, 2 were deprived of their hearing before the age of 5 and 8 were congenital cases. One only of the 9 with somewhat defective speech had lost his hearing later than 5, 3 before tlie age of 5 and 5 had been deaf from birth. TABLE II. ABILITY TO USE SPOKEN LANGUAGE. Table showing the power of speech possessed by children of different degrees of deafness, who have received speech training for three, five, seven, and ten years respectively. Character of speech Time in school Hearm? sufficient to aid in acquiriner speech. Deaf Hearintr lost after the age of five years Hearingr lost before the ape of five years Congren- itally deaf 6 3 1 10 2 6 7 2 2 3 5 3 Speech normal, or nearly so. 5 or 6 years 10 years or more. . . Speech somewhat defective, either in quality of tone or articulation of sounds. 3 3 1 9 4 3 8 10 5 5 or 6 years 10 years or more. .. 3 Speech wantmg-, or less than S or 6 years SO words 10 years or more... Totals 13 16 34 40 Taking into consideration then the acquirement of speech as it is related to time of instruction, we find of the whole number of 103 children who had had at least three years school training. 20 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES that 96 succeeded in acquiring oral speech, of whom 50 possessed normal speech and 46 were able to interpret oral language, but to speak albeit with difficulty or slight defect. It will be seen, moreover, that of this whole number there are 13 children who possessed sufficient hearing to enable them to be materially as- sisted in acquiring speech under schoolroom conditions. Of the children who must be classed as deaf, 16 lost their hearing after the age of 5 years ; 34 were deprived of their hearing before the age of 5 years, while there are 40 whose parents gave evidence regarding their early lives which indicated that in all probability these children were born deaf. Although 7 children were found whose speech was wanting after a period of training of 3 years, there were none after a school period of 5 or more years who were not able to express themselves more or less intelligibly. Not considering those who lost their hearing later than the age of s, and those who have considerable hearing, 23 had voices which were unpleasant and unnatural after three years of school training, 14 after five years and 9 after seven, or more years. These comparisons are made with reference to the voice of the normal child as a standard and are obviously unfair to a child working under the limitations of the deaf. It would no doubt be better to use a criterion of practical utility rather than one of esthetic value, but it is impossible in the case of the former to arrive at any norm for comparison, and hence we are forced to the use of the apparently less practical standard of reference, which, nevertheless, has, as a matter of fact, considerable signifi- cance. The time that a child has been in school is not the only nor is it the most important, factor in effecting speech efficiency. The natural endowment of the child is an extremely important consideration. Next stands the environment, some of the most important factors of which are the character of the food which the child receives, and the regulation of his rest and exercise. It is further of significance whether the parents speak the lan- guage of the school and are thus able to give the child some drill in the exercise of his speech outside of school hours, or in case the parents do speak the language of the school, whether they have the time and inclination to thus co-operate with the DEAF CHILDREN 2r teachers. An attempt was made to evaluate all these factors in the study of the condition of each child, and formed part of the "basis for the specific recommendations which were given to the child's teacher at the time he was examined with reference to his school possibilities and the best line of treatment adapted to his particular needs. VISUAL ACUITY. TABLE ni. VISUAL ACniTT. Tables showing the distribution of deaf and hearing children, respectively, as regardSi the visual acuity o£ both eyes, whether the vision of the two eyes Is the same or different. DEAF CHILDREX. Vision 1.00 or better. Nor- mal 0.67 of nor- mal. 20-30 0.50 of nor- mal. 20-40 0.40 of nor- mal. 20-50 or less Blind The vision of both eyesbeiipr The vision of one eye belns 1.00. the other The vision of one eye beinff 0.67. the other The vision of one eye being 0.50. the other 225 35 22 21 9 11 Total number examined, 353. HEAItlNQ childre; «. Vision l.COor better. Normal 0.67 of normal. 20-30 O.SOof normal, 20-40 0.40 of normal, 20-50 0.29 of normal, 20-70 orle s Blind or nearly so The vision of both eyes beinjr. . The vision of one eye beinsr 1.00 the other 972 82 119 11 8 31 27 11 18 11 69 21 20 7 24 4 The vision of one eye bein^ The vision of one eye beinp SO 1 he other The vision of one eye beingr Total number examined, 1,436. The oral method of training the deaf implies not only the utterance of speech, but also speech-reading. With normal hear- ing children this is done through the medium of the ear, where- by even the slightest variation of tones and inflections are in- terpreted naturally and without effort, yet each carrying with it a significance that differentiates a shade of meaning. Though the eyes are always used in discerning the attitude of the speaker by the hearing child, the use of these organs plays a subordinate role. Not so with the deaf child. Every idea • communicated to him, except the few he gets through touch, taste 22 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES and smell, must reach him through his eyes. In interpreting speech he must observe the most minute movements of the mouth- parts, the eyes and facial muscles, to interpret their significance, translate them into ideas and observe shades of meaning by the slightest variations in muscular adjustments and movements. Hence the condition of the deaf child's vision becomes a very im- portant consideration. While there are many opportunities for relaxation of the eyes of the normal child, these opportunities are obviously less frequent in the case of those children who have to depend on their vision almost entirely. Defective vision,^ therefore, tends to produce greater limitations among the latter than the former. In Table III are given the distributions of deaf children and of normal hearing children, from comparable social environments and home surroundings, as regards visual acuity. In this the emphasis is laid on the function of vision as a determining factor in acquiring school information. It is well known that a child may have very defective vision in one eye and yet because the other eye functions normally, the child may not be seriously handicapped in receiving impressions from his surroundings. Accordingly the acuity of each eye is registered separately and the number of children is recorded who have good vision with both eyes as well as the number of those who are visually acute with one eye and defective in the other, or indeed, those who have poor vision with both eyes. In the upper half of the table is recorded the distribution of 353 deaf children and in the lower half a similar distribution of 1,436 hearing children. Looking at the upper half of the table for a moment (the part referring to deaf children) it is seen that in the first line, beginning at the left, are tabulated; first the number of children with normal vision in both eyes (225) ; then the number whose vision for both eyes is 0.67 nor- mal (35) ; 0.50 normal (21) ; and 0.40 normal (2), respectively. In the second line is shown the number of deaf children whose vision for one eye only is normal, while that of the other is 0.67 normal (22) ; 0.50 normal (9) ; 0.40 normal (3) ; and where the vision of one eye is wholly wanting (5). Then in the third hne follows the number whose vision in one eye is 0.67 normal DEAF CHILDREN 23 as registered from the vision chart, while that of the other eye is 0.50 normal (ii) ; 0.40 normal (4) ; and vision entirely want- ing (o). This will suffice to make the whole table easy of in- terpretation. From the distributions in the table it is thus seen that of the total number of deaf children from whom we have visual acuity records (353) there are 225 or 63.7 per cent whose vision is normal as judged by the standards of the optician or occulist. Two hundred and sixty-four of the 353 or nearly 75 per cent have vision of at least one eye normal according to the same standards, and nearly 90 (88.9) per cent have at least one eye whose visual acuity is 0.67 normal. It is of interest and significance to compare the visual acuity of deaf children with that of the 1,436 hearing children in the lower half of the table. About 3 per cent fewer of the deaf have the vision of both eyes normal. Four per cent fewer have vision of at least one e3'c normal, but almost 2 per cent more of the deaf have a visual acuity of both eyes 0.67 normal or bet- ter, as compared with their hearing companions. Certainly of considerable pedagogical significance is the fact, moreover, that the per cent of hearing children whose vision is very poor — 0.50 normal or lower for one or both eyes — is nearly one-half greater than in the case of deaf children. Accordingly, it is clear that deaf children are not handicapped by visual defects, to any greater extent than are those children not afflicted with loss of hearing. By way of accounting for the relatively poor show- ing of deaf children among those whose vision is normal or nearly so, it is probable that resort must be made to some psycho- logical explanation induced by the deaf child's inability to react to the test as efficiently as do hearing children. And owing to the paucity of numbers too much weight must not be attached to the apparently better showing of deaf children when com- paring those whose vision is very poor. As much as can be asserted with confidence is that it is extremely probable in the light of the experimental data exhibited in Table III, that deaf and hearing children, on the whole do not dififer as regards vis- ion. A summary of the comparisons just made will perhaps show what has been said to better advantage. 24 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES Wiole number of children : Deaf, 353 ; hearing children, 1,436. Vision, 1.00 (20-20) or better. Both ( Deaf eyes ( Hearing* child. . Vision, 1.00 (20-20) or better. One! Deaf eye at least 1 Hearlner child.. Vision, 0.67 (20-30) or tietter. Both ( Deaf, eyes 1 Hearing child.. Vision, 0.67 (20-30) or better. One J Deaf eye at least 1 Hearing diild.. Vision, O.SO (20^40) or less. Both J Deaf eyes 1 Hearing child. . Number. Per cent 225 972 63.7 66.9 264 1153 74.8 79.0 282 1123 79.9 78.2 314 1287 88.9 89.6 33 188 . 9.3 13. 1 In the foregoing statements of results and comparisons nothing has been said as to what constitutes defective vision. Of course it is well understood that defective vision from the educator's standpoint is quite a different consideration from visual defects as an oculist understands the matter. For in- stance, a child may have normal vision (20/20) in one eye, and the other may be only two-thirds, two-fourths, two-fifths, two- sevenths or even less in visual acuity. It will readily be granted even by oculists that if a child possesses sufficiently acute vision to be able to carry on his school work well he must be placed in a different category from those whose vision seriously impede their school labors. Very unfortunately the minimum degree of visual acuity that must be possessed to insure against eye strains, inefficient book- and black-board work, and certain physical ills which attend the functioning of weak visual organs, has not yet been determined. Indeed it is improbable that any hard and fast line can ever be set to mark off the functionally efficient from the inefficient eyes, and particularly not in terms of visual acuity alone. One frequently finds instances where the dioptric defect is seemingly grave, and yet the individual appears to see well and experiences no ill effects from the use of his eyes, and on the contrary a very slight irregularity in the organ of vision itself is frequently attended by very serious pains and strains. In the latter the trouble is induced by bodily or nervous depletion, primarily, and only secondarily is it to be attributed to irregularity in the structure of the dioptric media. PHYSICAL CONDITION. Besides the foregoing data on the comparative sensitivity of the sight and hearing of deaf children, and their acquired power of speech after definite periods of school instruction, it is of im- portance to gain some definite idea of their general physical con- dition. Here again it is to be observed that so-called practical obser- vations and verbal descriptions of differences between this group of children and normal children have always led to exaggerated accounts and unwise statements regarding the deaf, sometimes to their advantage, but more often to their disparagement. There- fore the more positive and precise are the statements and the greater variety of aspects of the physical make-up investigated, the greater is the hope of our understanding the exact condi- tions that must be encountered in attempting to educate these children into citizenship. It is on this account highly desirable to present certain quantitative statements of the physical status and deficiencies of deaf children as are evidenced in the two more or less, closely related aspects of physical growth, viz., size ov bulk and movement or control. In pursuance of this end, in the following tables are given the comparative stature, height-sitting, weight, lung capacity, maximum length and breadth of head, strength of hand-grip and voluntary motor ability, distributed according to age and sex throughout. (The greater number of cases (293) recorded in some of these tables is brought about by the fact that they in- clude measurements other than these made during the period of this special investigation.) STATURE. The stature records represent the net heights expressed in millimeters. (See Table IV.) It will be seen that deaf boys are not so tall on the average as normal hearing boys at any age with the exception of those at the age of 10 years, at which time a slight difference is in favor of the deaf boys. This exception may be accounted for by 26 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES a class of boys who had been receiving instruction and unusual physical training since their kindergarten days and who pos- sessed especially favorable home surroundings. The deaf girls before the age of lo years are noticeably shorter than hearing girls and the differences are greater than the inferiority of deaf boys for their corresponding age — ^norms ; but after this age the deaf girls are almost uniformly taller than their hearing companions. To be sure the numbers are small and the cases (75) of those years of apparent superiority may be exceptional in this respect, but the fact is indicative at least that for the deaf girls the disabilities which they experience in hearing or the causes of their affliction have not, during this span of years, impaired their growth as measured by their stature. The height-sitting measurements show (Table V) that deaf boys are uniformly inferior throughout the years represented. The girls on the other hand are inferior up to the age of 11 years; at the ages 11, 12 and 13 they are taller sitting; at 14, 15 and 16 years they are again inferior, and thereafter the four cases show excess over the hearing girls. Taking the deaf children as a whole and comparing them with hearing children with regard to stature it is found that only seventy-seven out of one hundred and seventy-seven, or 43.5 per cent, of deaf boys are equal to or above the average stature of hearing boys for equal age as against eighty-eight that would be found with the same normal hearing boys, and in height-sit- ting only sixty out of one hundred and sixty-seven boys, or 35.9 per cent, are equal to or above the average of normal hear- ing boys, instead of a possible eighty-three or eighty-four out of one hundred and sixty-seven in a normal group. Again we find only fifty-two out of one hundred and twelve deaf girls, or 46.4 per cent who are equal to or above the average statures for the normal hearing girls of their respective ages, and in height-sit- ting only forty-three out one hundred and ten, or 39.1 per cent, are equal to or above the average of hearing girls for the same ages represented. Both deaf boys and girls, consequently, it is learned, are not quite so tall as are hearing children of corre- sponding ages, and the deaf of both sexes have shorter bodies than do their hearing companions. DEAF CHILDREN H O > "-* -^ th o i-~ c- e ■ U"; VTj ir, ir. \fi SSSKS^Sss p w ^ bt u 2-5 2 o y c ■^rr-t^niHoorHirjo^ooro n-j-^h-r-roror-(r-r-<©o cr irjTj- r-rir- oo ■- incooono^cc- ^ i-H M M M ro ^ -)- If , O -c -2 f- o(MOMonu^Oi-(i-ir-no OQ 1- XT) i-H »r* o h* cr c- oc m ri c~^ r5 "* >f J u"* u-j o iTi o -D .o I- fo o nriTj-r-iTtorjo-rHmc^no ) "* iTj Uj O »0 nO ■ifSiHni-Hr-IWT-ltHr O'^iOOO^O^O OO'-O^O'OO •or-MC>o»-OM'* •Si? h is ItcS .in I a W"" ■* coM^n•onoo^■* ooi^-* ^o 1,5 b t3 !^"Si3 ■g at § S 82 3sa ■oooM^mi-tnoTfi- ^O ^ ^O O ^ '•O ^£ ^ o o o ^ ^ ^ ^ ID OI—OOCO^^firOTl-ii^'^t— 00^ 30 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES In addition to average lengths and breadths are given the averages of the products of length and breadth of the head of each child of both the deaf and the hearing children of the sev- eral ages (Table VII). By consulting the columns of the table which represent products it will be seen that in the case of the ■boys, the deaf are uniformly inferior to the hearing children, and this comparative status likewise obtains throughout with the girls with the exception of the age of 12, where the nine cases measured show a superiority on the side of the deaf. In these measurements again let us consider the deaf as a group and compare them with normal hearing children of com- parable ages. In length of head only sixty-seven cases out of a total of two hundred and seven, 32.3 per cent, are equal to or in excess of the average hearing child for each age group. In breadth, however, there are one hundred and twenty-four of the whole number of two hundred and seven boys whose heads are as broad or broader than the average — 59.9 per cent. Practically the same ratio obtains in the same measurements with the girls. Only thirty-seven out of a total number of one hundred and twenty-seven girls, or 29.1 per cent, are equal or greater in length of head than the averages of our normal group and in breadth there are eighty-seven out of the whole number of girls, one hundred and twenty-seven, or 68.5 per cent, whose heads are as wide or wider in greatest breadth than the av*age of the normals for their ages of hearing children.f Deaf children have, as a whole, therefore, smaller heads than do hearing children, taking as a criterion the length, breadth product,* and they are more brachiocephlic, broad-headed. fXliese norms of head measurements have been taken from the Statistics on Growth by Franz Boas, published in Report of Com- missioner of Education, Chapter II, 1904. It Is found in consulting the records of this Department that with reference to this particular physical measurement our cases are so largely atypical, that it is thought unsafe to adopt them in a comparative study of this character, and on this account recourse is had to the careful record of indi^'idual measurements in the report of Professor Boas. The standard devia- tions recorded in his tables are translated into the average deviation by using the formula A. D.^.79796. *It would have .been better, no doubt, to have rendered a comparison of head size based on the three head dimensions, employing as the height measurement the maximum height of the head above the floor of the auditory meati, but it was Impossible for us to get satisfactory records of the last named measurement. DEAF CHILDREN > — 00 IT) 00 f*) O O M n -Mr, f 1 i~I O 00 o So ("I .-^ o f r- vr, .-< o> 00 .H r^ n (V( -t ri -f m o r- iZ ir* n n ri ri r< i~< ri r-i ^ ?*i ro f^ sO I- o -o rr o -M^ '-I c- Hnoxo-Dca-nr-i-i IT, -t-^r-i/innr-rou o o ■* oc o ir J -r vf i c- IT, -c -r •-' 30 -n- ir> i o 1^ I- sC ifj rj >f J m ro -s f^j X cr ^ i-i rirt-t- II, ^j fo i/i r* I/, f » u. fo u7 o -* &. ^ rt N r4 r-j ri rj N 1-1 rt M n rH o o I"! rr ri o r- r-i 'n o -x o o i- -l-.-tCO'+OrHa'f-'-frHX.-lr-IC o '-T -r r- r 1- irj u-j ir; ^ lOcr-r-i^Tooxui^nrir-iri^ -Or-000'0»Hr'irT«*-li-)'Ot-XC C r-i-r t-l tH ^ i-( iH iH tH 1-1 rH t-< r-< n ri 32 EXPEEIMENTAL STUDIES LUNG CAPACITY. The measurement of the lung capacity was taken by means of the ordinary wet spirometer (Narragansett) and simply in- dicates the greatest amount of air that can be expelled from the lungs after a forced inspiration. This is after everything is considered, perhaps also the best test of the amount of air that is available for use. It may not give a direct index of the actual or possible size of the lungs, but it affords in a way a convenient, safe and valuable criterion of the degree of activity to which the vocal apparatus has been subjected, or is capable of exercis- ing at the time the test is made, as well as the extent of the child's general physical activity. It is on this account quite natural that we should find that the deaf children are markedly inferior in lung capacity to normal hearing children, and this palpable inferiority, even in the case of those deaf children who were given special physical and speech training, gives us an in- dication of the immense amount of physical exercise other than speech that normal hearing children naturally indulge in and from which the deaf are barred by reason of their sensory handi- caps. The intimate relation which exists between middle ear infirmities, high and narrow bone-palate formation with all the accompaniments and causes of obstructed breathing and dimin- ished lung capacity has doubtless been observed by every careful investigator of normal children with merely defective hearing. It has likewise been pretty well recognized that the amount of usable air in the lungs of normal adults varies, within limits, with the degree of general physical activity. But besides suffering the consequences of inertness during early years, the deaf children do not give their lungs anything like the same de- gree of exercise that normal hearing children indulge in, since indeed they vocalize so little, and hence the cumulative effects of this double handicap becomes palpable and hard to overcome even with persistent exercise. In no other particular are the deaf children so uniformly or markedly inferior to the hearing normal children as in lung capacity. This is seen clearly by reference to Table VIII. The deaf boys are inferior at every age. This inferiority, to be sure, is not so pronounced at ages ii and 12 years, or at DEAF CHILDREN 33 s ^o^tn^ojfjt^oooo-Jo^ > ">■ 11 s« (Z Sa cr % 5 s « 30!3S:ggasgi:3SS; sr a p* » cr 3 :3 > i£ j_^ *.c^tnc/»o^tno^WJ».-i^(T' £3" O.)->H'H>O>JM-400 4^l-*O O "J S 3 3 f 28S£S22§5^::l5!Diid 3 CD oo-Jwtn-iowoowc>^i-» y cr W s > P *. 4>. *. t/i a\ *. Jk ^ C/» *. *. W 4».Uil-iH'4^WGJ^O-J00-4 rt> -^ •1 11' SSIII55ISSSS 3 3 w B 4kOCitnH'0*.OvOO^U\M n. S r+ s H >• § «.. ui u ■»■ cn K> -P^ oi u lo ■«>. OJ & oo-c»-jma'00*--J-4O'>-' d o W » •a *l*ttS5SSS§S B B tja,^oo*oi-'Miowi-'0 p. 3 cr B W !0 > p -I ><■ o Vr M O o 0.0 o a H9 s?a» CO 3SS£St«^S5g5is a 9 D MO-ut ^WWi-'O-OOO-JO' 5 iJCCiOCCX'ODOOCOOOai^-J 4^ C^ 4^ '^ at 4^ 4^ 4k '^ ^ ^71 Vt -JO o*cno -Ji ijWC>WH'O0J*'H'H*'O604' tn io M O 00 C- O' UX O Ch H* ^ ooo-^ooootnoo^-^ooMM 0J*'KiQWQ'OGJ-vIMU»-4-4 Q0UX-JJWyi*.>O0^Cni-*O'-4O^ ooo^(/\ai&>c»JCo^ootJa'Oo 3 : u Oo-o f § = £■ IIP 34 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES the particular epoch in their lives at which we notice that some of the other measusements showed an equality or even superior- ity to the norms for hearing children. The same inferiority is apparent in the case of the deaf girls, and here again the great- est inferiority is shown at those years, lo, ii and 12 years, at which we previously saw a lack of uniformity. Here again it should be noted that because of the relatively small number of children examined at each age it is probably a safer criterion to take the total number of deaf children whose lung capacity is equal to or above the average for hearing child- ren of the same age. In counting over the cases with this object in mind we find among the one hundred and forty-seven deaf boys, of all ages examined, that only eighteen, or 12.2 per cent, are equal to or above the average for hearing boys, and that in the case of the deaf girls thirty-two out of a total of ninety-five of all ages, or 33.9 per cent, are equal to or above the average for hearing girls, instead of 50 per cent, which figure would indicate them as normal. It is interesting to note the position of deaf children with reference to lung capacity in the per centile group of hearing children.* These are shown in Table IX, in which is indicated the per centile group of hearing children into which the average of the records of the deaf of each age fall. In a parallel column is given also the average for each age, of the ratios which the in- dividual lung capacity records of deaf children make with the average lung capacity of hearing children of the same age. Thus it is seen that the average lung capacity of deaf boys, and girls as well, of the mean age of six and one-half years, falls in the *For an explanation of these percentile grouplnps, see Report of tlie Child Study Department, Public Schools, Chicago, 1901, p. 13. In brief, the method of procedure was as follows : the four or five liun- dred records secured for the children at each age were distributed according to ranli. The minimum measurement gives the zero per- centile for the whole gi-oup. To determine the ten percentile, ten per cent of the nnmiber of cards is removed, beginning with the minimal record, and the highest measiu-ement on the card so removed is re- corded as the maximal limit of the ten percentile group. A similar placing and selecting of the next highest ten per cent of records with their minimal and maximal limits gives us the twenty percentile group and so on, for the others. DEAF CHILDREN .fcl o as < §§gslSgSsg3is5«»* SlsS^S^^gsiS^ oo«oo»ooor-ey'iHir>0'U)'Orj ^3 t aS "* *Q * "^ "^ f* yj fi f" 25 1 n ^8 is o S 36 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES TABLE IX. LUNG CAPACITY. Boys Girls Aee. Percentile Group Average of the Percantiie Group Average of the ratios of of Normals m ratios of Yrs. Mos. which deaf deaf children to which deaf deaf children to children fall normal hearing: children fall normal hearing 5 .5 0-10 0-10 .74 6. ...6 0-10 .81 .62 7. ...6 10-20 .82 0-10 .77 8. ...6 10^20 .83 20-30 .83 9. ...6 30-00 .90 10-20 .82 10. ...6 20-30 .88 40-50 .96 11. ...6 20-30 .91 60-70 1.03 12. ...6 10-20 .85 10-20 .85 13. ...6 10-20 .81 20-30 .88 14. ...6 10-20 .77 10-20 .84 15. ...5 10-20 .75 40 .96 16. ...6 10-20 .79 40-50 ■ 98 17. ...6 0-10 .63 40-50 .98 Below limits of normals .48 19 6 30-40 .95 ten per centile group of hearing boys and girls of that age, and on the average, deaf boys have a lung capacity .8i as large as hearing boys, and deaf girls .74 as large as hearing girls, at this age. The table presents the data so clearly that further comment is unnecessary. The striking inferiority of deaf children of both sexes at all ages, almost uniformly, stands out in this table even more convincingly than in Table VIII. STRENGTH OF GRIP. To determine to what degree the strength of the grip of deaf children is inferior or superior to normal children of the same age and school conditions, an adjustable grip dynamometer was used, which registers the strength of grip in kilos (i kilo= 2.2 lbs.). The gross results of these tests are given in Table X. The ages, number of pupils of each age tested, grip of right and left hand are given in parallel columns for boys and girls respectively, and the norms of the grip of normal children (taken from Child Study Report No. 2, 1899-1900),. placed in columns adjoining for ready comparison. By consulting Table X we find that the deaf boys are slight- ly superior in right hand at ages 7, 8, 9 and 11. After the age of 12 years they are uniformly inferior, and this inferiority in- creases with age. The strength of left hand is superior with DEAF CHILDREN 37 TABLE X. STKENGTB Off GEIP. DEAP BOYS. Right Hand Left Hand Age lYrs. Mos. No. of cases Deaf children, average grip, legs. A. D. Hearing children, average grip, kgs. Deaf children, average grip, kgs. A. D. Hearing children, Average, grip, kgs. 5. ...6 2 7 28 24 20 23 17 18 18 14 14 11 6 4 6.5 97 12.7 15.8 16.4 19.5 21.7 21.7 23.4 28.9 28.1 38. 6 37.7 355 .5 1.9 2.7 2.5 2-4 2-8 3.5 2.8 3.0 81 4.9 7.2 2.3 70 5.5 8.7 11. 7 14.6 15.6 17.8 19.5 20.7 211 28.5 26.3 37.2 37-4 37.5 .5 1.7 2.0 2.8 3.0 21 2-4 3.0 2.2 65 6.3 6.7 1.6 10.0 7. 12 6. ...6 7. ...6 8. ...6 9. ...6 10.. ..6 11. ...6 12. .6 13. ...6 14. ...6 15. ...6 16. ...5 17. ...6 18. ...6 19 .... 6 9.97 11-51 13.31 15.37 17. 67 20. 03 22.45 26. 43 30.40 36.38 42.35 47.14 51.43 9.29 10.92 12-43 14.51 16.66 18.80 20.62 24.40 28.40 33.73 39.40 42.87 47-16 20. ...6 STRENGTH OF GRIP. DEAP GIRLS. Right Hand Left Hand Age Yrs. Mos. No. of cases Deaf children, average grip, kgs. A. D. Hearing children, average grip, kgs. Deaf children, average grip, kgs. A. D. Hearing children, average grip, kgs. 6 6 7 6 8 6 9 6 10 6 11 6 12 6 13 6 14 6 15 6 16 6 17 6 18 6 10 7 14 11 13 11 9 12 10 14 5 7 1 4 8.8 12.9 11.1 12.6 15-4 16-8 19.3 24.2 23.7 27.5 35-2 32-0 33.0 39.0 2-2 1.9 23 1.6 1.7 3.3 2-3 4.2 3.5 3-8 3-4 3-1 "iio 9.12 10.65 11.67 13-88 15-43 17-65 20-19 23-49 26-10 27.91 29-50 29-63 29.87 8-10 9.80 10.60 12-20 14.70 15.60 16-70 22-90 22-40 25.40 30.10 29.00 28.00 32.25 2.10 2.50 2.00 1-10 2.30 3.10 270 4-58 3.12 4.07 2-80 2-85 "s.is" 8.49 9.99 10.96 12.99 14.46 16.58 18.97 21.80 24.03 25.80 27.31 27.55 27,77 20 6 1 the deaf boys at ages 7, 10, 11, 12 and 14 years and inferior at ages 6, 8, 9, 12, 13, 15, 16, 17 and 18. The deaf girls are weaker in right hand grip than the norms for hearing girls at ages 6, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, 14, 15 and slightly stronger at ages 7, 13, 16, 17 and 18. Practically the same com- parative status obtains in grip of left hand of these deaf girls. Again because the number of cases of the deaf is so small» a comparative estimate of the children as a whole may better be 38 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES made from the number of children of the group who are equal to or superior to the average of our normal children in this test of strength. In actual numbers it is found that ninety boys out of two hundred and four, or 44.1 per cent, are equal to or superior to the averages for hearing children. With the left hand, as a group, the deaf boys stand higher than with the right hand, in- -Bsmuch as one hundred out of two hundred and four, or 49.7 per cent, are equal or superior to the average of children of the ages included in the norms. In the case of the deaf girls there are fifty-five both left and right hand out of the total number of one hundred and twenty- four, or 44.4 per cent, who stand equal to or superior to the norms of hearing children for the ages represented. It appears, therefore, that the deaf boys as a group more nearly approach the norms of their hearing companions than the girls with the left hand, while the advantage is slightly in the favor of deaf girls as a group in respect to the right hand. GRIP INDEX. The observation has quite frequently been noted * that left- handedness is found more frequently among deaf children than among the hearing. Various reasons have been assigned for this peculiarity, although all agree, however, in supposing that there is some intimate connection between speech and this dif- ferentiation in hand power or selectiveness in manipulation. In Report No. 2 (1900) of this Department, the data with reference to changes in growth of hand strength of normal children were presented. Moreover, it will be remembered that in Prof. J. Mark Baldwin's studies of his two children ** there was an account of a noticeable preference for one hand over another in grasping for attractive objects which made its initial appearance about the time of speech beginnings. This apparent correlation of the two modes of expression at the time of their origin seems to be again in evidence at the time of the great **Mental Development in the Child and the Race, 1896, chap. I. *Hartmann, Ueber Taubstumme, Deutsch med. Wochenschr, III, 1877. Deaf -Mutism — (trans. Cassell), 1S80. J. Kerr Love; Deaf- Mutism, Ohap. I, Glasgow, 1896. St. Hilalre; La Surdi-ilutite, p. 251, Paris, 1900. DEAF CHILDREN 39 transformations of life. An instance of this occurs during the pubertal epoch. In the reports of .this department attention was called to the fact that the greatest difference between the strength of the hands occurred at early adolescence contempo- raneously with the greatest changes of voice. Another group of facts somewhat akin in import relates to speech defects. It was found in our work that by far the greatest number of minor speech defects was discovered among children who were pro- nouncedly left-handed or who acquired ambidexterity by reason of the teacher's persistence and the parents' training. This whole line of reasoning may be merely a matter of coincidence. It seems much more reasonable to suggest that all cases of minor speech defects correlate directly with inferior intelligence, as TABLE XI. MANUAL INDEX.* GRIP. Boys Girls Deaf Hearing: Deaf Hearing Age Yrs. Mos. No. of Index No. of Index No. of Index No. of Index cases per cent cases per cent cases per cent cases per cent 6....5 7. ...6 7 28 88.8 90.0 8 93.1 7 85-0 9 90.0 8. ...6 24 87.0 29 92.0 14 93.0 28 92-9 9. ...6 19 86.0 44 93.1 12 91. 39 91.5 10. ...6 23 89.0 42 93.04 13 920 41 91. 7 11 .... 6 16 94.0 44 95-4 11 94.0 45 921 12. ...6 18 92-0 40 91.6 9 86-0 47 92-4 13. ...6 18 91.0 48 91.1 12 95.0 45 93.6 14....6 14 94. 36 91.8 8 92.6 64 93. 1 15. ...6 14 93.0 31 92.5 14 91.0 47 91.7 15. ...6 11 93.0 20 90-9 6 93.16 39 90.6 17. ...6 5 94-2 90 91.8 7 90.85 51 89.9 18. ...6 4 93.2 37 89.4 4 83.0 75 89.9 •Note. — The measure of the Index is In each case, the -per cent which the wealier hand Is of the stronger. The degree of right- or left-handedness Is not therefore indicated. indeed our work with subnormal pupils would seem to indicate, and that ambidexterity is one of the marks of inferior mental- ity instead of the converse, as further work with reform school boys gave evidence of. (See General Reports II and III). This feature of the whole matter is held in reserve for another series of studies. It must not be forgotten, however, that this matter of the 40 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES number of deaf children who are left-handed is a relatively sec- ondary problem in comparison with the larger aspects of growth in hand strength which center about the differences in hand power between the right and left hands of deaf children through- out the years of growth. This growth in specialization is indicated in Table XI, in terms of manual index. The measure of the manual index is in each instance the per cent which the weaker hand is of the stronger, and for convenient comparison the index of normal hearing children is placed in adjoining columns, both for boys and girls. There seems to be greater difference — that is, the manual in- dex is less — between the strength of grip of the hands of deaf boys as compared with hearing boys at the ages 7, 8, 9, 10 and II years. Beginning with the age of 12 years and from 12 to 18 years, both inclusive, there is less difference in the strength of the hands of deaf boys as compared with their hearing mates of corresponding ages. In comparing the younger deaf boys with those older in years it is found that, in general, the differ- ence between the strength of the hands tends to diminish, that is, whereas, in the early years the manual index is less than ninety, it goes up to ninety-three or ninety-four in the older years. As the deaf boys increase in age there is less and less difference between the strength of their hands, less specialization or selectiveness in manual power. This is the direct opposite of what we find among hearing boys, among whom we find that the strength of hands is more equal in the early years, and that the difference between the hands increases perceptibly as they grow older.* This may be only another indication that deaf boys are later in maturing than hearing boys, or that the lack of activity of all sorts and especially of constructive manipulation of ma- terials is so great an environmental handicap as to practically make the deaf boys a species by themselves. There is by no means so great uniformity found in the differences of hand power of deaf girls as compared with their hearing companions of equal ages. There is less difference between the strength of ♦Compare Binet Quotation from Dr. St. HUaire, "that grip of left hand differs less from right hand than normals." Also, Kilian. DEAF CHILDREN 4I the hand of deaf girls at ages 8, lo, ii, 13, 16 and 17 years; and greater difference between the hands of deaf girls than in the case of hearing girls at ages 7, 9, 12, 14, 15 and 18 years respectively. The general tendency, however, so far as this can be read from the data at hand, seems to show that up to the age of 12 years there is less difference between the strength of hands of deaf girls than hearing girls of equal ages — the direct oppo- site of the relation that was found to obtain in the case of deaf boys. In comparing the younger deaf girls with those of older years we find that the differences between the hands increase as they grow older, the opposite of what we found prevailing among deaf boys, and more in line with the trend of develop- ment of normal hearing girls. Again this may be largely due to the fact that the difference in activity which is found between deaf and hearing girls is not so great as the difference between deaf and hearing boys. To summarize, the nature of the data is such as to make it unwarrantable to assert that any differences exist in relative strength of the two hands of deaf girls as compared with their hearing sisters, but with reference to deaf boys the data seem to indicate that for younger years, during pre-adolescence, more differentiation in the strength of the two hands, respectively, ob- tains than among the hearing, and for the adolescent years, less differentiation prevails than among children with normal hear- ing — that is, the deaf are more ambidexterous. MOTOR ABILITY. In examining the voluntary output of energy one should take into account not only the gross and spasmodic total result of effort usually called strength and represented in one instance as hand-grip (Table X), but also the factor of the quickness or speed of voluntary movement. This factor of voluntary motor ability is recorded herein as simple tapping-time, and in our tests registers the number of taps that can be made with an electric tapper during thirty seconds. The two most important elements in a test, namely, precision of movement and constancy in effort and execution unfortunately we shall have to ignore entirely, and hence there is presented only the total number of taps made during thirty seconds. 42 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES TABLE XII. MOTOn ABILITY DEAF BOYS. Riffht Hand Left Hand A ere Yrs.Mos. No. of cases Deaf children, averasre no. of taps, 30 sees. A. D. Hearing children, average no. of taps, 30 sees. Deaf children, average no. of taps, 30 sees. A. D. Hearing children, average no. of taps, 30 sees. 5. ...5 6. ...6 7. ...6 8. ...6 9. ...5 10. ...6 11. ...6 12. ...5 13. ...6 14. ...6 IS. ...6 16. ...5 17. ...6 18. ...6 19 . 6 2 7 20 22 22 22 17 18 17 14 17 12 7 4 76.1 . 117.8 120.1 129.2 137.9 138.0 146.8 163.3 165.3 174.0 176.4 1820 162.4 164.5 15.0 19.5 13.5 18.6 10.8 19.5 16.1 15.5 16.9 21.5 26.4 21.5 9.9 12.5. 72.0 100.5 97.7 105.8 115.0 114-6 124.1 141.2 145.0 145.4 152-8 159.5 146.3 150-6 12.0 2.6 13.8 22.6 12. 3 19.0 16.1 14.4 20. IS. 7 16.6 20. 1 18.6 25.7 147 151 161 169 170 184 184 191 196 196 197 117 127 132 141 145 156 155 159 170 174 183 MOTOB ABILITY DEAF GIBLS. Right hand Left hand Age Yrs. Mos. No. of cases Deaf childen, average no. of taps, 30 sees. A. D. Hearing children, average no. of taps 30 sees. Deaf children, average no. of taps, 30 sees. A. D. Hearing children, average no. of taps, 30 sees. 5....5 6....6 7....6 8.. ..6 9. ...5 10. ...5 11. ...5 12. ...6 I3.---6 14---. 6 15. ...6 15. ---6 17. ...6 18. ...6 19----6 20-. .-5 1 8 13 14 10 13 10 9 12 7 12 6 7 1 3 115.0 112.9 109.9 122.5 134.9 141-5 148-2 153.7 155.9 157-7 174.0 177.1 175.7 178.0 188.0 105.0 95.1 103.7 93.8 115.5 128.2 132.2 131-4 137-1 133.3 158.7 161.1 163-4 159.0 171.0 11.1 14.5 12-2 14-5 17-2 22-4 20-6 18.1 21.1 24-1 12-8 14.9 "is.o" 25.0 21.3 17.0 13.8 12. 1 13.0 20.2 25.9 14.0 21.0 10.5 11.9 "■9.'3" 145 149 157 169 159 178 181 181 188 184 193 117 118 U9 139 140 153 157 159 167 162 169 In Table XII are given in parallel columns the following: (a) The number of children tested at each age. (b) The average number of taps made in thirty seconds by the children. (c) The average deviation of the records from the average. For ready comparison the averages for normal children for cor- responding ages are placed beside those of the deaf. The rec- ords are given for both boys and girls for the right and left hands. DEAF CHILDREN 43 By going over thife table somewhat in detail one sees that the deaf boys are with the right hand notably and uniformly slower than normal hearing boys, and that although not so quick at any age with the left hand, they are not in this case so in- ferior, and at the age of twelve years almost equal to the hear- ing boys. Moreover, this relative slowness seems to increase as they grow older. In the case of the girls we find that they too are much slower at all ages with the right hand, and with the left, though slower than hearing girls, they are at no age as much slower relatively as with the right hand. Indeed, at the ages fifteen to seventeen, they are almost equal in quickness to hearing girls with the left hand, and at the age of seventeen years they are actually quicker on the average. Moreover, the inferiority of the girls, unlike that of the boys, appears to decrease with age, and this may be only another indication of the increased degree of general physical activity in which they come to indulge, as well as of other environmental factors. Again on account of the paucity of numbers at each age it is advantageous to take the deaf boys and girls as a group and compare them in quickness directly with the hearing boys and girls for the corresponding ages represented in each deaf group. It is thus found that of all deaf boys examined, only forty-one out of one hundred and seventy-two, or 23.8 per cent, are as fast or faster than the averages for hearing boys of their ages with right or left hand, and that only twenty-nine of a total of one hundred and one, 23.8 per cent, deaf girls are, with the right hand, equal to or above the average for normal hearing girls of corresponding ages, while only thirty-four out of ninety- nine cases, 37.3 per cent, recorded for quickness of left hand, equal or surpass the averages of hearing girls for the same ages. The relative standing of the deaf, all in all, when compared with hearing children in the cases of both boys and girls, is therefore found to be, even less favorable, when rapidity of movement is involved, than was found in the measurements of stature and weight, and only slightly better than the relative standing of the deaf in lung capacity. The facts are diagramatically exemplified in the accompany- 44 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES ing figure, which indicates the positions occupied on a per centile scale drawn from a large number of hearing children of each age, the average standing of deaf boys and girls of correspond- ing ages. From this it is seen for example, that the average standing of seven year old deaf girls is lowfer than the average of the lowest lo per cent of the records selected from the performances of hearing girls. In one instance, only, 12 year old deaf girls, does their average tapping rate exceed the aver- n TAPPIBS ABILITISS- Rieht Hana . A / // /7 /7 /9 ^Z THE HOBIZOUTAL NDMEEALS BEPKESENT THE AGES OF THE BESPECTIVE GKOtTPS THE PEBCENTS ON THE VEBTICAL LINE SHOW THE BATINQS OF THE n EAF BOTB AND GIBLS FOB THE DIFFEBENT TEABS. age of hearing girls, and in the averages for three age groups only among boys do their performances equal the average of hearing children. Further, in the tests of motor ability as with the hand- strength there is an advantage and interest in giving the manual index. This index of manual quickness (See Table XIII) measures in each instance the fraction which the slower hand DEAF CHILDREN 45 is of the faster. In comparing then the manual index, as deter- mined by relative quickness of movement, of deaf boys with hearing boys, it is seen that there is practically no difference observable until after the age of eleven and one-half years. After this age, however, the manual index is higher among the deaf boys, showing that the difference in motor control of the right and left hand is not so great as among hearing boys of the same ages, or that specialization in this method of ex- pression has not attained as high a degree among the deaf as the hearing, that is, the deaf tend more to ambidexterity than do ordinary hearing children. TABLE XIII. MANUAL INDEX.* MOTOE ABILITT. Boys Girls Deaf Hearingr Deaf Hearingr Age Yrs.Mos. No. of cases Index No. of cases Index No. of cases Index No. of cases Index 6. 6 (y 79 3 g 85.5 83-9 7. ...6 25 79. 9 8 79 12 11 80.1 8. ...6 22 81. 18 33 81 4 14 79.0 8 80.32 9. ...6 21 83-7 60 82 6 10 83.6 44 79.4 10. ...6 22 81-9 47 « 3 12 860 48 82-4 11.. ..5 17 82.5 49 83 10 88-2 46 83.1 12. ...6 18 87.1 44 82 5 9 86.1 49 80.7 13.. ..5 17 85.5 50 84 6 11 85.5 44 86.7 14.... 6 14 83.2 40 82 08 7 85-1 68 86.1 15-. ..6 16 861 35 86 6 11 87.2 48 86.7 16. ...6 11 91.7 21 87 1 6 91.1 40 88.0 17. ...6 7 88-9 13 88 7 7 92-1 51 88.1 18. ...6 3 86-2 3 93-3 1 89. 24 90. 9 *- ^ be o^ -S CS OQ O^ O X OS cs be 03 be « O oj be K cs t-i 03 • pH O 03 SI O ^ CS P-^ ^ ^ X cs OS N 03 >^ it PS 3 P« cs I §- 08 o pO 03 X C8 be pfi Is 03 be o C8 be ■§ oe •i-t QC N cr 08 ($ ^ -^ CS 03 GC CS -^ • p-l ^ pTS cs o CS § I I— H 'W N OS a a po o cs X m 03 cs a 03 X ;^ X & be a^ OS cs &« 52 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES ters of the alphabet, so as to form one hundred groups of letters of five each. The first part of the "A" test was designed with two ob- jects in view. By taking the time required by the chifd to place a stroke with a pencil through each of the one hundred "a's" when he has been requested to do it as rapidly as possible, a measure is afforded of a certain type of motor ability. But in addition to this there is also afforded, which is more particularly to the purpose of the test in hand, a measure of the physiological, or motor time consumed in marking one hundred "a's." After completing part one, the child was instructed to place a stroke through the a's in the page print (see p. 51), as quickly as possible, being sure to mark out every "a"; the instructions be- ing uniformity as given above for deaf and hearing children alike. A measure of the time required to perform this test was also taken and a record made of the number of "a's" not marked. It is readily apparent that at least two factors enter into the selection and marking of "a's" as called for in this test; the one a mental factor concerned in the several acts of discrimina- tion in finding the "a's," and the second, physiological, having to do with the motor task of making the marks. To disinte- grate these factors and arrive at a measure of the purely men- tal time actually involved in perception, is the point of chief moment, to make the test of value in stating quantitative differ- ences between individuals and groups. And this is acomplished approximately . by subtracting the motor time gotten in the first part of the test from the total time required to perform the sec- ond part. This latter we shall call the "Perception Time" (P. T.) in the discussions and tables. There is one factor in connection with the "A-Test" which makes the results awkward to handle. If every child marked out all the "a's," differences in perception might be stated directly in terms of the perception time. The fact is, however, that many children fail to find all the a's, there being sometimes as many as forty that the child does not see and mark out. Here, con- sequently, are two variables, which thus far there has been found no satisfactory way of equating in terms of each other. Thus DEAF CHILDREN rt jj g < n < 00 ■* 00 N in •* 00 p* t- o 1 XI i a Sis a < o>lnc^-opHo6oo^ou^o ll a ^ o'rt •-• >o m t- ^ (*i fH o ■"SRRSSRSS" lno.H.-^^o-u■iOOT^'J••>*■lrt^- o sO 00 o 1^ r- Tf n n r» u^ ir, -* < oo-OrH{*)r-'*Tf-Moorouir- isssasssssss"-- s ■Li oxnuir»M-»-oa>r-NOooi/>M Ui 1 -* * o r« 00 o r< ■* o* 00 in 00 r^ S3 o-ooo'*NDooi-«t-ooo«r- S is O'fH-OTrr-^^a'.HMOc I- gg&gssKSSffis^ ^ o-sOuiNpinc^ r-o*OMWiinn sOO-OOOOj3-00£iOOO r-orjo- o.-iriro-tu'ivot- or o- 54 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES it is impossible to say, exactly, whether a child who has marked out all the a's and whose perception time is 60 seconds, has bet- ter or poorer ability, as regards this test, than the child whose perception time is, say one-half as great — 30 seconds — but who has failed to mark out ten or fifteen of the a's. A probable de- termination of the efficiency of children in the test, however, was secured by calculating from the time required to mark out the a's in each case, the estimated time that would be required to mark out 100 a's. That is, in the event that it took 170 sec- onds to pick out 85 per cent of the a's, it was estimated that 200 seconds would be required to pick out all the a's. No cor- rection, however, was made for a's overlooked in those cases where at least 96 per cent had been marked. For the most part the children were tested one at a time, and in no case were there more than three children given the test together. All the data from the "A-Test" available for statistical pre- sentation will be found in the accompanying tables. (See Tables XIV and XV.) The tables are sufficiently self-explanatory to require little direction in reading off the data and their sig- nificance. To facilitate comparisons with hearing children the data from this group are placed in parallel columns with those from the deaf. It is seen that there are indicated for each age: (i) the number of children examined, (2) the average motor time, and (3) the average perception-time (total time less motor time), together with the average deviation of the individual rec- ords from the mean or average of each group; also the number of a's on the average not discerned. Table XIV exhibits the data so as to show the relative num- ber and per cent of deaf children of each age whose records fall below the average or mean performance of hearing children of corresponding ages. It is, indeed, the more telling of the two tables, too, inasmuch as the influence of the small number of rec- ords which make up the average of each group, and the skewing tendencies of extreme records, loose their undue weight. Motor Time — We are not concerned particularly, with the question of motor proficiency in the present instance, but it is worth while in passing to note the comparative standing of deaf and hear- DEAF CHILDREN 55 ing children, respectively, in motor time as measured by the first part of this test — a form of motor-ability test. The rela- tive motor inferiority of deaf children stands out most clearly during the earlier years, but it is by no means lacking for any of the ages considered in these comparative measurements, since at no age is the average motor time for deaf children as low as for their normal hearing companions. Table XV shows the relative motor inefficiency of deaf children, if anything, even more conclusively. We find that at the age of seven the rec- ords of all deaf children are poorer than the average for hearing- children. At eight years of age, ii of i6, or 69 per cent of the deaf make poorer records; at nine years, 9 of 15, or 60 per cent; at ten years 20 of 26, or yj per cent, and when children of eleven years are compared, again all rank lower than the average for hearing children of the same age. Of the total number of deaf children who performed this part of the test, 148 in all, 122 or 82.4 per cent, stood lower in motor efficiency than the averages of the records for children with normal hear- ing of comparable ages. The numbers are too few to warrant closer mathematical treatment, and indeed such treatment is unnecessary in the face of the data just offered. The reasons for this manifest motor inferiority have already been com- mented upon, and nothing requires to be added in this place. Perception Time — Again looking at Table XIV, in the columns headed "Per- ception Time," differences between the two classes of children, more pronounced than those shown in the 'Motor Time," are observed. The time required by deaf children to pick out the one hundred a's, on the average, ranges from almost twice that required by hearing children at the age of seven, to more than a third longer for the later years, fourteen and fifteen. For none of the age groups is the average perception time of chil- dren who have no hearing anything like as small as for chil- dren who hear normally, though the difference between the two groups progressively diminishes with increasing years. Judging from this tendency of the difference in perception time of the two classes to become less with age, it is not un- 56 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES likely that were deaf adults and those with hearing examined and compared little difference would be discovered. Considerable variability is exhibited by the average devia- tions of both groups, and a much greater amount of variability among the performances of the deaf than of hearing children. That the differences brought out by the averages of the two groups, however, is real, is conclusively shown by the charac- ter of the data shown in Table XV, where the number and per cent of deaf children whose standings are inferior to the aver- ages of hearing children of comparable ages are given. Thus it is seen that at the age of seven years all deaf children pos- sess a lower standing than the average for children who hear ; at TABDE XV. TEST PERCEPTION. SUMMABY OF TABLE XIT. Relative number and per cent of Deaf cMldren whose records stand lower than the averages for their respective ages of Hearing children. Motor time Perception time Age Number Number Per cent Number Number Per cent examined lower examined Lower 7 9 9 100 7 7 100 8 15 11 59 14 12 85 9 15 9 60 15 12 80 10 25 20 77 25 23 88.4 11 15 15 100 IS 11 73.2 12 9 9 100 9 9 100 13 17 12 70 16 12 75 14 9 7 78 9 9 100 15 20 18 90 20 15 75 16 12 12 100 12 8 66.7 All agres 148 122 82-4 143 118 82.5 eight, 12 of 14, or 86 per cent; at nine, 12 of 15, or 80 per cent; at ten, 23 of 26, or 88.4 per cent of the deaf require a longer time to pick out from among four hundred other letters the one hundred "a's" than the averages of their hearing com- panions for comparable age-groups. Concerning deaf children of all ages grouped together, we discover that 82.5 per cent, 118 of the whole 143 examined, have a longer perception-time than the averages of children of the same ages not thus af- fTicted. When the number of "a's" not marked is taken into account DEAF CHILDREN 57 the results are in all essential respects comparable to those above. (See Table XIV). Deaf children not only require a longer time on the average to discriminate between the "a's" and the remaining letters of the alphabet when they are intermin- gled, as in the "A" Test, but they are also able to discriminate less accurately than children whose ears function normally, as is indicated by the greater number of errors, — "a's" overlooked, — and this deficiency likewise extends to all ages. Indeed, if we follow closely the results of the test, we are forced to say that deaf boys and girls from twelve to fourteen show rela- tively less discriminability than do those of the younger years. The data regarding the A-Test are strikingly conclusive. Deaf children,' on the average, are from two to three years less mature than hearing children of the same ages in the kind of ability that is called for in this test. And certainly this can not be attributed to the fact that a certain kind of skill is called for in the test, that the lack of school training from which deaf children suffer would fully account for. On the contrary, it is difficult to see how ordinary school exercises would conduce to rapid and accurate symbol discrimination, except in the gen- eral sense of leading the child to be more keen and alert in meeting any general and unfamiliar situation. From other tests and measurements, as well as from intimate association and observation, we are forced to conclude that a relative imma- turity of from two to three years, as compared with hearing children, extends to all phases of the mental life of the deaf, and no doubt, too, its cause is to be found in the natural alien- ation which the minds of these children suffer by reason of the sensory barriers that have been set against their intermingling in the mental and social life of the communities in which they were born and are maturing. Perception of Size. Shot Test — The second test, which deals primarily with perception, had for its object, as stated above, the determination of a child's sensitivity as measured by the ability shown in discriminating the sizes of small objects by the active exercise of the sense of 58 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES touch. This end was brought about by using a series of light canvas bags containing ordinary commercial shot of the fol- lowing sizes: Nos. BB, B, i, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, dust shot. The test had, like the foregoing test, grown out of the daily duties and needs of the Department of Child Study, and had been effectively used almost daily for four or five years, in the regular series of tests and measurements given to normal and abnormal children in the laboratory. In conducting the test a child was first given a bag con- taining the median-size of the series, or shot number 6, at the same time another bag containing a size shot so large that ordinarily little difficulty would be encountered in perceiving the actual size-differences, after the child had rolled the shot- bag between the forefingers and thumb. It was found that in the case of very young deaf children these ideas of comparison, "greater than," "less than," are rather late in developing, and teachers of the manual alphabet assert that such relational ideas are probably of all ideas the most difficult to appreciate and express in communicable sym- bols. With young children, especially those who had practically no language, considerable patience and perseverance was found necessary in order to have them get this conception of greater and less associated with or abstracted from the objects or quali- ties compared. Hence the necessity of using large differences at the beginning of the test. These considerations are equally pertinent to those perceptional tests dealing with the attempts to determine and express differences of weights, which are con- sidered later on. As soon as a degree of familiarity was gained by the child with the test and method of procedure, bags containing shot- sizes of smaller differences from the median-size, number 6, were presented and compared by him each in turn with the standard, until each size, both larger and smaller than the me- dian, was tested and a judgment expressed on the sensed differ- ence. In each instance great care was exercised by the exam- iner that there be no marks of identification on the bags examined, nor acquaintance with any particular way in presenting the bags. DEAF CHILDREN 59 nor any other means or measure of the child's devising or dis- covery which could interfere with the end desired, viz. : to deter- mine the least difference, greater or less, than the median size, number 6 shot, that might be accurately sensed under controlled conditions. Whether indeed the sizes of shot used in the series are graded uniformly or not, is a question which need not concern us, since it would be impossible to state, even though the actual differences between the variously numbered shot were equal, that the qualitative results obtained in the tests have absolute significance, e. g., that one child has twice or three times the sensitivity to tactual perception of size that another child pos- sesses. Below are given such measurements of the shot in diame- ters, in both the English and metric systems, as are actually necessary for a speedy application of the test: It is admitted that this test is in itself a very complex proc- ess, involving not only the ordinary skin sensations of touch, but also calling into exercise the muscular and articular sensa- tions. But this variety of sensory data, as well as further dif- ferentiations due to individual variations, such as differences in the thickness of the cuticle, and again lack of uniformity among the subjects who were examined in relying upon the compo- nent factors in the actual test of sizing — such, as for example, may be brought about by one person's dependence in the test upon pressing the shots between the fingers and thumb, while another relies upon lightly rolling the contents of the bags — will enter only remotely into the test from the point of view herein taken, which is to determine whether deaf children, on Size 2 3.70 mm. 0.146 inches Size 3 3.15 mm. 0.128 inches Size 4 2.99 mm. 0.1 18 inches Size 5 2.89 mm. 0.1 14 inches Size 6 2.79 mm. 0.1 10 inches Size 7 2.66 mm. 0.104 inches Size 8 2.36 mm. 0.093 inches Size 9 1.83 mm. 0.076 inches Size lo 1.75 mm. 0.069 inches Size II 1.52 mm. 0.060 inches 6o tXPERIMENTAL STUDIES the average rank higher or lower than hearing children in this very primitive and still in our day substantial mode of gaining information. The results of this test are presented in Table XVI. In this the deaf children are compared directly with the hearing chil- dren of their respective ages. The table is divided into two main divisions, or parts. The main division on the left, for both deaf and hearing, indicates the portion of the experiment in which the standard is compared with shot of a smaller size, and the other main division has reference to that part in which the shot compared is larger than the standard. The sizes of the shot sensed are given at the top of each column from I to XII, and the results of comparative sensitivity under these headings show the per cent of children who are able to sense one, two and three size-dififerences. As already indicated, differences in sensitivity of subjects do not always run parallel to objective differences, but it is, however, quite natural to make the inference that actual dif- ferences in the sizes of shot discriminated measure to a degree subjective differences in the sizing sensitivity of individuals, so that a child who can discriminate size 6 from size 7, or size 6 from size 5, has a keener sense of touch than a child who can only sense differences between two, three or more sizes. Owing to the small number of cases, the girls and the boys, respectively, are divided into three groups; first, those chil- dren from 6 to 8 years; second, those from 9 to 12 years; third, those between the ages of 13 and 18 years. The ages of the groups are given in the first column of Table XVI, and in the second column is presented the number of cases examined in each group. For ready reference the recorded per cent of correct answers in each instance is marked by Roman numerals. Thus, in the third column, marked I, is recorded the per cent number of children who correctly sensed a difference between the sizes 6 and 7. In comparing, therefore, the deaf children with the hearing of like ages, in this particular test, we exam- ine columns I and IV, VII and X, for records of their re- spective abilities in dealing. with one size; columns II and V, VIII and XI, for differences of two sizes; and columns III DEAF CHILDREN n u p s Eei o H m ^, M > 09 kj tH § M N ■< 03 y b O !? O *fl*-S «ie m-^i CO** to « *^C0^ to 0) M^ tc V 4sa S3K CO eo S ?ss SSr t-op6 <5i ?SS ^2 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES and VI, IX and XII, for the results secured when the test in- volved actual differences of three sizes. It is readily seen that at no age-group do so many deaf children accurately sense small differences as their hearing mates. Thus, in the 6 to 8 age-group of boys, 23.5 per cent only of the deaf children gave correct answers in attempting to sense a difference between sizes 6 and 7, whereas 60 per cent of the hearing children of corresponding ages were able to answer correctly (See columns I and IV), and in comparing sizes 6 with size 5, the deaf and hearing children stand, re- spectively, 21 and 80 per cents (columns VII and X). Owing to the nature of the data in this Table, it is unnec- cessary to discuss any more than the recorded abilities of the children in dealing with differences of one size. (Columns I, IV, VII, X). It is interesting, nevertheless, to note that where- as 15.4 per cent of all deaf boys, in the age-group 9 to 12 years, are recorded as being able to perceive no smaller differ- ences than three sizes (column III), there were no hearing boys of the same age who were unable to detect size differences of at least two. It is, moreover, apparent that while sensitivity in this test increases with age, there is a much greater increase observed among the deaf boys and girls than among the hearing chil- dren of comparable ages, for we see that the per cent stands, respectively 23.5-42.3-56.2 and 21. -37. 7-56.2 for the deaf boys, as against 60.-60.-78. and 80.-73.-67. for hearing boys (columns I, VII, IV, X), and 40. -61. "1-60.7 on one-half of the Table, and 40-55.6-71.4 on the other half for deaf girls, as against 67.58.3- 70.6 and 44.-54.-83. for hearing girls (columns I, VII, IV, X). Besides a discussion of the differences due to increased ages of the deaf and hearing children, it is interesting to not* whether our tables show any sex differences in this particular test of sensitivity. It is apparent, to speak in general terms, that deaf girls are more sensitive in this test than deaf boys, while, on the other hand, among the hearing children, the ad- vantage in sensitivity Hes with the boys. (See especially age- group 9 to 12 years, for deaf and hearing boys and girls). The reasons for this are, however, difficult to point out. It may, DEAF CHILDREN 63 indeed, be due to some fundamental difference between deaf and hearing children; again differences of training and daily ex- periences may affect their skill in reacting in this test, or we may have to fall back on the observation that the number of deaf children in each age-group is so small as to preclude positive statements on this score. For the very reasons, however, that the number of cases is so comparatively small, and the data of the sizing-test are pre- sented in terms of ranks of individuals, it is of some interest to turn to the additional data subjoined, which show the num- ber and per cent of children who are able to size differences as TABLE XVIL SIZING TEST (ACTIVE TOUCH). Showing the number and the per cent of children who are able to sense differences as indicated, the children being grouped without reference to age. Deaf boys, all Deaf etItIs, all aees Boys and eirls- Heariner boys, all aeres Hearlner fflrls, all ayes Boys and firirls. No. of cases 75 56 131 51 105 I. Size 6 from size 7 33, or 4456 32, or 57?i 65, or 50% 35, or 65?6 32, or 63JS 67, or 63.85S II. Size 6 from size 8 38, or Sl% 21. or yj% 59, or 45* 19, or 35Ji 18, or 359i 37. or 35.236 III. Size 6 from size 9 4, or S% 3, or b% 7, or S% 1, or 2% 1, or 1% IV. Size 6 from size 5 32, or 43% 34. or 61?i 66, or 50Ji 40, or 7436 32, or 6336 72, or 68-536 V. Size 6 from size 4 35, or 4736 19, or 3436 54, or 4136 12. or ■22% 18, or 3556 30, or 2836 VI. Size 6 from size 3 8, or 1036 3, or 536 11, or 936 2, or 436 1, or 236 3, or 336 indicated in Table XVII, where the children are grouped with- out reference to age. In this Table XVII it is more than ever apparent that deaf children, as a group, are not so sen- sitive as hearing children. Giving exact statement, it is seen that only 50 per cent of the 'deaf children sensed the smaller size differences (See columns I and IV of Table XVII), where- as 68.5 per cent, or at least 63.8 per cent of the hearing boys and girls, accurately detected a difference of one size. Of course, the qualification should be made that the number of children in the different age-groups are not equal. However, because there are more children in the older age-groups than in the younger, and again since sensitivity is observed to increase with age, whatever advantage there is, lies with the deaf chil- dren. Again, by reason of the further groupings of children 64 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES which this table makes, the sex differences of the deaf and hearing children, already alluded to, become more significant. Taking the deaf first in order, it will be seen that 44 per cent of the boys gave correct answers to the test on one side, below size 6, and again 43 per cent of the whole group were correct for a difference of one size above size 6, whereas the deaf girls gave 57 and 61 per cent, respectively, correct. The deaf girls, as a group, although inferior to their hearing sisters in sensitivity, are not so much below as the deaf boys in this par- ticular. The hearing boys stand 65 and 74 per cent for one point's discrimination, while 63 per cent of the hearing girls, as a group, show ability to perceive differences of one size above or below the standard. Moreover, from internal evidence in the table, the deaf and hearing boys seem more variable in abilities than the hearing and deaf girls. Least Noticeable Differences in Lifted Weights. Turning now to the third perception test, we come to con- sider the child's power to discriminate small differences in lifted weights, where the lifting or hefting is done by a free, full-arm movement, such as occurs when a child passes alternately from one weight to the other in lifting them from a table, upon which they have been placed. For the test series small wooden bottles of uniform size and dimensions were employed. So far as external appearances go, they could not be easily distinguished the one from the other. The bottles were loaded to the required weight with a filling of shot. They were then packed with paraffin and cotton, so as to prevent any movement of the contents within, after which they were carefully sealed. The series ran at intervals of two grams from a weight of 8 grams to one of 32 grams. In order it stood, 8, 10, 12, 14, .16, 18, 20, 22, 24, 26, 28, 30, 32 grams. With the 16 gram weight, which was used as a point of departure, or standard, weights heavier and lighter were com- pared; the 16 gram weight and the one heavier or lighter than it, having been placed upon a table before the child, and he asked to heft them, one after the other, and to indicate which of the two was the heavier. Care was taken, however, to keep the DEAF CHILDREN 65 child dependent upon actually sensed factors in hefting, and to guard against his identification of the standard weight by extra- neous signs. A comparable method of procedure was followed, sometimes with weights heavier, sometimes with those lighter than the standard, in irregular order, until the experimenter, after several determinations around the standard weight, had satisfied himself of the least noticeable difference in weight, both lighter and heavier than the standard, that the child could sense. In recording the data it seemed best in indicate simply which weights the child could discriminate, accurately and invariably, from the standard, or i6 gram weight. Consequently the tables, showing the comparative sensitivity of deaf children for weight, like the preceding tables for siz- ing, give the results in terms of the number and per cent of boys and girls who were able to distinguish between (i) a weight of 16 grams and one of i8 grams, (2) one of 16 grams and one of 20 grams, (3) one of 16 and one of 22 grams on the one side of the standard, and between (4) weights of 16 and 14 grams, (5) 16 and 12 grams, and (6) 16 and 10 grams, re- spectively, on the other. Such data as we were able to collect are recorded in Table XVIII, arranged so as to show sex differences, and the com- parative standing of deaf and hearing children, respectively, of comparable ages. As in the preceding test, because of the pau- city of numbers, it was deemed advisable to somewhat arbi- trarily group the records according to age. Thus, in the first group, we placed the records of the boys and girls ranging in ages between 6 and 8, inclusively; in the second, the preadoles- ents (9 to 12 years), while the last group is composed, for the most part, of children beyond the age of puberty (13 to 18 years, inclusively). Inasmuch as the primary interest in comparing hearing and deaf children, with respect to this test, centers about the least perceptible difference in weight, which, on the whole, the groups, respectively, are able to sense, it is with the parts of the data which indicate the number and per cent of children making the smaller discriminations, that we are chiefly 66 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES concerned. Indeed, since the figures are, for the most part, mutually exclusive, in casting up age, sex and group compar- isons, we need consider only the data found in the columns in- dicated by the Roman numerals I, IV, VII and X. Examining critically now the data in the columns indicated, it is to be noted that for the younger years (6 to 8) deaf boys are rather strikingly inferior to their hearing mates in this sense, while the converse holds in the case of the girls. In the i6 and i8 gram set, 66.7 per cent of deaf boys were able to make the discrimination invariably, as against 80 per cent of the hearing boys. The figures for deaf and hearing girls are 66.7 per cent and 44.4 per cent, respectively. In the 16 and 14 gram set again 66.7 per cent of the deaf boys always rendered correct judgments, while 100 per cent of the hearing boys were able to do. so. For deaf girls the figure is 71.4 per cent, against 66.7 per cent for hearing girls. The number of hearing chil- dren, especially at these ages, is unfortunately small, so that too much weight cannot be attached to the results. In the next age-group (9 to 12 years) the numbers are lar- ger, and the conclusions consequently a little more reliable. Deaf boys now rank in muscular sensitivity above hearing boys, the per cents being, for the 16-18 gram pair 88.4 and 74, re- spectively, for the deaf and hearing boys, and for the 16-14 gram pair, 77.3 and 72, respectively, for the groups considered. Between deaf and hearing girls of this age-group, the differ- ences are discovered to be inconsequential. The figures for the 16-18 gram pair are 53.3 and 54.0 per cent, respectively, and for the 16-14 gram pair of weights, 64.2 and 58.3, per cent. The groups of adolescents (13 to 18 years) are not so large; but differences between the groups in the mental attitude in which the test would be approached, ought to be less signifi- cant than for the younger ages. (If this factor actually enters into the test.) So far as the results go, no significant differ- ences in muscular sensitivity between deaf and hearing boys or deaf and hearing girls stand out. Deaf boys rank lower in the 16-18 gram set than hearing boys, as is shown by the rela- five per cents who invariably rendered correct judgments, 60 per cent of the deaf and 66.7 per cent of the hearing boys, but this DEAF CHILDREN 67 > a n o •3 .s u XII. 16 firms. from 10 s^ms., per cent ■ in rot--* 000 1 p 2 - "^c 3^ VIII. 16 enms. from 12 firms., per cent orto >O00rt ^ -H ft t-rjo 1 =3 ■3 .S H 2 - « fl i 2 rj 5 -^ 2 - « S OOro ■*©■* 5^a 2 - « « *0^0 OS as ""S^S =^SS •3 1 ; -+ MOO sis 2 - " ^ SS8S t-fon s ^3 ^?3S ''"IhS 1 oc oc 68 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES inferiority is compensated for in the 16-14 gram pair of weights, where the per cent of deaf who sensed a difference was 64 as against 61 for the boys with hearing. In the case of the girls, the deaf have a slight advantage in the 16-18 gram pair, 78.3 per cent making correct judgments, as opposed to 70.6 per cent of the hearing girls, but with the 16-14 gram set 69.6 per cent of the deaf girls invariably gave correct replies and 70.6 per cent of the hearing girls. Looking for differences between the boys and girls as re- gards sensitivity for lifted weights, among deaf children it is discovered that for the younger years, the sexes do not differ very significantly, the same per cent of boys and girls sensing a weight difference between 16 and 18 grams, and only a slight advantage being shown in favor of the girls in the 16-14 gram set. Among hearing children of the same ages, on the con- trary, the boys show up markedly better than the girls, 80 per cent sensing the difference in the 18-16 gram set, and icx3 per cent in the 16-14 gram set, as against 44.4 per cent and 66.7 per cent, respectively, for the two sets, in the case of the girls. Comparing the sexes in the second age-group (9 to 12 years old), we find the boys uniformly better both as regards deaf and hearing children. The figures run as follows : Deaf. Hearing. 16-18 set. 16-14 set. 16-18 set. 16-14 set. Per cent. Per cent. Per cent. Per cent. Boys , ... 88.4 77-3 74.0 72.0 Girls 53-3 64.2 54-0 58.3 Conditions are reversed when we turn to the older group of boys and girls, those between the ages of 13 and 18 years, as may be seen from the following summary: Deaf. Hearing. 16-18 set. 16-14 set. 16-18 set. 16-14 set. Per cent. Per cent. Boys 60 64 Girls 78.3 69.6 Thus, although some sex differences were discovered, when age groups were considered separately, taken all together the group tendencies counteract one another, so as to obliterate any Per cent. Per cent. 66.7 61.0 70.6 70.6 DEAF CHILDREN 69 conclusive evidences of such tendencies in one direction or an- other. Looking for differences in sensitivity that might come with increased age, the data also offer nothing very promising. The 16-18 set does show such a tendency in the case of the hearing girls, but the exact converse is shown, with both this weight set and the 16-14 gram set in the case of the hearing boys; The data for the deaf are altogether too irregularly distributed to warrant any general conclusion. A larger representation in each age-group, however, might bring out some decided evidence of improvement in the weight sensitivity with age, but it is equally likely that the responsible factor is the elementary nature of the test. PHRiCBPTION OP WEIGHT (ACTIVE). BKIEF SnMMABY OF TABLE XTIII. Deaf boys, fill ages- . ■ Deaf srirls. all apes . ■ ■ Soys and ?irls (deaf) ■ No. of cases 62 47 109 16 8TS. from 18 gTS., per cent 71 68.1 69.7 16 £rrs. from 14 arrs., per cent 69.3 68.1 68.8 Eearin? boys, all ases . . Hearing ?irls, all aeres - Boys and girls (hearingr) 55 50 105 72.9 58.0 65.7 70.9 64.0 67.6 In the foregoing discussion, because the numbers at each age are so small, the children were placed in three groups, and, to a degree, the disadvantages due to small numbers were over- Come, though, to be sure, not wholly obviated. There are sev- eral advantages, therefore, in comparing the deaf boys and girls as a whole, irrespective of age, with hearing boys and girls under like conditions, as well as to compare our deaf children as a whole, with their hearing companions. The results of such groupings are presented in Table XVIII (Summary), and we accordingly see that, in sensing a difference of two grams be- tween two lifted weights, 71 per cent and again 69 per cent of the deaf boys gave correct responses, as against 73 per cent and 71 per cent of the number of hearing boys examined. Further, 68 per cent and again 68 per cent of all deaf girls correctly sensed a difference of two grams, whereas only 58 per cent and 64 per cent of the hearing girls were equally sensitive. Finally, 70 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES in the group representing boys and girls, we see in comparing the deaf children with hearing children in this test that 70 per cent in one instance and 69 per cent in the other case, of all the deaf girls examined, accurately sensed the actual differences of two grams in the weights presented, whereas 66 per cent and again 68 per cent of the whole number of hearing girls ex- amined, were able to note the same differences. Deaf girls, therefore, as a whole, irrespective of age, rank better than their hearing sisters, but, on the contrary, deaf boys rank more poor- ly, and, taking boys and girls together, irrespective of age, while the advantage lies slightly with the deaf, their superiority over their hearing companions is not sufficiently marked, con- sidering the paucity of numbers, to warrant us in asserting that the difference is real. Memory. Among the different materials employed for testing the im- mediate sense-memory, the ordinary Arabic numerals must be considered among the best, both from the point of view of con- venience and reliability of results. Numerals are symbols used in our civilization, and inasmuch as they are in themselves rel- atively non-significant, they can be employed readily in making a comparative study of one kind of memory. A numeral is not liable to have more associations with one individual than with another. To test the visual sense-memory, cards on which were placed in rows the ordinary Arabic numerals were held before the child, after which the cards were removed and the child was required to reproduce the numerals which were seen, in the exact order in which they appeared on the cards, either orally or in writing. In order to avoid groupings the child read the numerals from right to left, and the greatest number which he could accurately reproduce was considered a measure of his vis- ual sense-memory. In the second part of the test the numerals were repeated to the child by the experimenter, care being taken that there should be no rhythm which could induce groupings; and the child reproduced the numerals as before. The test thus consisted of the two parts (i) pure visual sense-memory, (2) indirect visual-memory, read from the lips of the experimenter. This latter also afforded a test of the DEAF CHILDREN 71 child's ability to read unrelated word-symbols from the lips of the speaker. TABLE XIX. MEMORY SPAN. Afire Number Visual Lip readiaff Yrs. Mos. Deaf Normal Deaf 7 6 8 6 9 5 10 6 11 6 12 6 13 6 14 6 IS b 16 6 17 6 18 6 19 6 10 13 13 17 12 7 12 12 17 6 4 2 3.3 3-3 3.7 4.1 4.4 4.7 48 5.2 4.9 5.7 5.5 5.0 5 5 5 6 6 6 8 3.2 3.0 3.1 3.7 4.0 4.4 4.2 4.7 4.3 4.3 4.7 4.0 5-0 In Table XIX are given the memory spans of deaf and nor- mal hearing children in parallel columns. It is seen that where- as the hearing child of from seven to eight years has, on the average, a memory span of five numerals, the deaf child has a memory span of but three numerals, and while the hearing children from 12 to 19 years of age have, on the average, a memory span of seven numerals, the deaf children can remem- ber but four or five. It is further seen that there is but a slight superiority when the numerals are directly seen in comparison with the indirect reading, or reading from the lips. This offers a suggestion that lip-reading has kept pace with the child's general development. Taking all these tests and observations together for the purpose of estimating the mental status of the deaf children, we see that it is possible to get a general view of their compara- tive abilities with respect to particular mental powers. But when these mental functions are all taken together they do not ex- haust the content of such a highly complex thing as mentality. It is necessary always to bear in mind what we really mean in everyday life by mental capability or mentality. Is it the abil- ity to speak, to read, to write, to follow directions, carry on or- dinary functions of life without assistance, or does it mean the capacity an individual has to improve by training and prac- tice and the relative facility with which training can be accom- 72 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES plished? With the deaf child we necessarily must place more emphasis on the latter, i. e., the teachableness of the individual. Sometimes he cannot speak, read, write or follow directions as given by us, because he lacks the symbols — language, the me- dium by which we, as hearing individuals, indicate directions. And until he learns the symbols and the meanings attached to them it is impossible to test him by such standards. Such a pro- cedure would be as unfair as to give the unclassified man direc- tions in Greek, and then classify him as mentally deficient be- cause he was unable to follow them. Therefore, the chief criterion used in testing the mentality of the children in the deaf rooms was their amenability to train- ing. The question put to ourselves was this: Has the child sufficient mentality so that, with the training he shall receive in school, he will be partially or wholly self-supporting when he has reached maturity, or will he likely become a ward of the State? Such a criterion naturally gives us three somewhat distinct classes, which may be called: 1. Mentally Normal: Those who will be wholly self-sup- porting at maturity. 2. Slightly Sub-Normal: Those who will not be, in all proba- bility, wholly self-supporting when they attain to man- hood and womanhood, but may require some external direction in any occupation which they may take up, or in the general conduct of life. 3. Those who will probably require direction and care, and hence must be regarded as wards of the State. We find, then, that as thus classified, the deaf children are distributed in the following order, and these ratings are made partly on the basis of the personal judgment of the examiner — being the result of an hour's careful inspection of the child's work — but more particularly based on the child's performance in the mental tests given : Number of Normal Mentality 173 Number Slightly Retarded or Sub-Normal 11 Number Mentally Defective, or of those who do not give promise of being at least partially self-supporting at maturity o Total number examined 184 DEAF CHILDREN "J^ It is thus seen that of the total number of 184 children in attendance at the deaf schools, only 11, or 6 per cent, are so backward mentally that they do not give hope of being able to support themselves independently after they have reached the ages of manhood and womanhood, and further, there is not a single individual whom it seems the State will have to take over as a ward.* It may be noted that this number, 6 per cent, of the group of deaf children who are accounted as Sub-Nor- mal, is considerably higher than we find among all school chil- dren, among whom 5 or 6 per thousand are regarded as dis- tinctly Sub-Normal in the very technical use of the word. This rather large number is doubtless intimately related to, if not, indeed, caused by, the diseases and disorders that bring about impairment of hearing in such a large number of the deaf chil- dren. ♦This does not mean tbat tbere are no mentally-defective and feeble-minded children among the deaf In Chicago, but that in the pre- liminary examination of deaf children, toy this Department, for admis- sion to the rooms for the instnietion of the deaf, the parents of those who were too low a grade of mentality to profit by such instruction as the public schools afford, were advised to send their (Jhildren to institu- tions where special province is the care of such cases. In no case were feeble-minded childi'en permitted to enroll in the classes for the deaf. SUMMARY AND SUGGESTIONS. In making a brief resume of the study heretofore presented in detail, the attempt will be to give the results in barest out- line only and then add the suggestions which come to mind as a consequence of these results. It was pointed out that the intimate relation between the mental and physical life of children made it necessary, if the study should be full and complete, that these aspects of the child's life should receive careful consideration. Further, by way of introduction, it was indicated that owing to the extreme timidity and narrow social life of deaf children, time, patience and skill were required in order to elicit any responses which would be indicative and characteristic of this type of children. The hearing ability of the deaf was found to vary by degrees from absolute silence and insensibility to any sounds whatever, to a condition of semi-deficiency where the auditory acuity was too defective for the child to carry on his work in the grades with hearing children, i. e., it was found that deaf children do not form a distinct group. There are all grades of hearing power and the classification night better be based on the use which the power of hearing serves in social communication. It was found that only thirty-two per cent of the whole num- ber of deaf children were totally deaf ; nineteen per cent were found to be practically deaf, that is, they gave no hope of ever being able to hear the human voice, even with the aid of mechan- ical instruments. There were thirty per cent who were unable to hear speech, but could hear loud noises. This and the next group, which contains fourteen per cent, who could hear loud speech, form a class which may for certain purposes be grouped together. In comparing the intensity of the noise which some of these children are able to hear, with that which some adults, whose hearing became impaired after maturity, are capable of appreciating and responding to intelligently, it is found that there are deaf children who hear no speech at all whose absolute hearing power is superior to that of some of those who hear and DEAF CHILDREN 75 depend on speech. This diiference in speech-hearing abihty seems to be due entirely to the absence of the habit of attention to the significance of sound-sensations on the part of deaf chiU dren. These considerations oiler the suggestion that if some means were employed of intensifying speech-sounds and in this way create the habit of attending to sound sensations of speech, in many cases the hearing of the deaf children might be so im- proved that to a considerable extent they could be made depend- ent on hearing in carrying on social intercourse. Of course, such a habit could not be developed short of many months of training and for this reason we found it impracticable to make the test of the value of any apparatus on the market for this purpose. Differ- ent devices of hearing apparatus are very highly recommended, but we are not aware that any of these have been extensively tried anywhere in Chicago. Not having given any apparatus for developing the power of hearing a careful and crucial test, the Child Study Department does not therefore feel justified in recommending any particular form or device, but we do think that some apparatus should be given a trial, especially since twenty per cent of all children in attendance at different rooms for deaf children give the hope of having their hearing sufficient- ly improved to hear conversation. Passing to the part of the investigation relating to the speech and speech-reading of the deaf, the main consideration in rela- tion to the time that had been spent in acquiring it, was the character of the speech. Necessarily the character of the deaf child's speech depends on the amount of training he has received. This aggregate of training, in a great majority of cases, can be measured by the time the child has been in school. Waiving all consideration of those children who possess sufficient hearing to enable them to speak in the natural way and those who lost their hearing after they had learned to speak, thus limiting ourselves to those who are born deaf and to those who lost their hearing early in life, we find that after a period of three years in school, twenty-five per cent have speech which is normal, or nearly so; there are fifty-three per cent whose speech is defective and twen- ty-two per cent in whom speech is practically wanting, i. e., less than fifty words. 76 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES To repeat here, for the sake of clearness, a statement made in the body of the report, when we speak of a deaf child's speech as normal, we mean that his articulation and tonal quality are good, but not that his range of vocabulary is equal to that of a hearing child of the same age. A latitude in this particular must be allowed of from three to five years. After a school period of five years we find that forty-two per cent of those born deaf or those who lost their hearing early in life, have speech which is normal or nearly so, fifty-eight per cent have speech which is more or less defective and not a single indi- vidual was found who could not intelligently talk at least fifty words. At the end of seven years of school training forty-seven per cent were found to have normal speech and fifty-three per cent with speech somewhat defective. Only four individuals had been in school as long as ten years. Of these one had some hearing; the other three, congenital cases, were found to have speech which was normal. From the foregoing it appears that at least where conditions ■obtain as they do in our day schools for deaf children, more than three years of training is necessary in order that the average deaf child shall acquire speech, but that almost every child (so far as our results indicate) is able to learn the use of speech and speech-reading after a school period of five years. It is unfortunate that the school has no control of the home and social conditions of children and especially the deaf. If it were possible to control, in a measure, the food, rest, exercise and language environment of the deaf child, and if it were pos- sible to enlist the co-operation of parents in employing school tnethods of communicating with their children, whatever these methods may be, the acquirement of any language would be greatly facilitated. In fact, it is found to be so with the method ■now in use, in those homes where parents are eager to learn and adopt the school room practices. Children from better homes, it was observed, were from two to four years in advance of those who came from less favored homes. However, it is doubtful whether many consider the time element as after all the factor of paramount importance. Probably the most vital consideration is whether the great majority of deaf children or all deaf chil- DEAF CHILDREN "JJ dren can acquire speech even after ten years of training, and if (as shown by the results of our investigations) all children, after they have been in school from five to ten years, do gain a suffi- cient mastery of speech to carry on intercourse with hearing, people, then the question arises whether the oral method be of more service to them than the manual in the succeeding forty or fifty years, while they are earning a livelihood and are getting the most out of life that it has to offer. But as before men- tioned, the problem of the relative value of oral or manual lan- guage as a means and medium of intercourse in the social and industrial world is one which it was impossible for us to evaluate and on this account we prefer to leave it untouched. There are, however, certain suggestions that refer more spe- cifically to those who are engaged in teaching the deaf, which we feel ought to be made.. Where oral speech is to be taught, it is undoubtedly the part of wisdom to take cues from nature's ways of dealing with normal hearing children. Every effort should be made to ascertain as early as possible in the life of these children the nature and degree of deafness, together with the causes which brought about the defect, so that perchance something may be done to remedy or improve the conditions. In the case of the congenital deaf and those who lost their hear- ing before the middle of the second year, the ideal procedure would be to have instruction begin as nearly as possible after the time that speech tendencies normally develop.* It is one of the laws of instinctive behavior that if tendencies to activity are not utilized at the time they appear in nature's program, they fade and the activity must be developed anew at the expense of considerable effort, and there is no reason why this does not apply to speech as well as to other human activities. Whether kindergartens should form a part of the public school system of education for the deaf is a matter pertaining wholly to public policy and administration, but certain it is that those few chil- dren now in the deaf schools who have had the advantage of early instruction are far in advance of the less favored chil- dren in their power to adjust themselves to the educational force* at work in our social life. •Of. Kerr Archives of Crtol. 33 : 129. 78 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES The important part which hearing plays in the acquiring of speech is another element that cannot be too much emphasized. By all means advantage should be taken of the hearing, power which the child has, however little this may be. Especially im- portant is this in the case of those children whose hearing be- came impaired through disease after speech had been acquired. Unless the hearing factor is utilized, there is a tendency for con- fusion to result from the inability of the child to distinguish the hearing images from the imperfectly formed visual images. (Of course reference is here made only to language images.) In the matter of sight, which is so overwhelmingly impor- tant in the life of the deaf child instructed in the oral method, who must depend on this sense almost entirely, the investigation showed very happily that the visual acuity of the deaf is on the whole as good as that of the hearing children. Nor is the rela- tive number of those suffering from decidedly defective vision greater in the former group than it is in the latter. The sug- gestion is pertinent here that the eyes of the deaf children should receive frequent testing so as to obviate any permanent impair- ment of vision which may result in consequence of eye strains. In comparing the stature of the deaf with that of hearing children, it was discovered that both deaf boys and deaf girls were, on the whole, shorter in stature for corresponding years, and both sexes among the deaf have shorter bodies in proportion to the total height, than hearing boys and girls. With respect to weight, deaf boys were slightly lighter, on the whole, than their hearing fellows, but on the other hand, deaf girls were slightly heavier, age for age, than hearing girls. Deaf children in gen- eral, of both sexes, were found to have smaller heads than nor- mal hearing children, when as a criterion of size, the length- breadth product is employed. On the average, too, they were more brachiocephalic (broader headed.) The most striking comparative inferiority of the deaf was fwinted out in connection with the data on lung capacity. Deaf boys as a group, it was seen, stood about on a par with the twenty-five percentile group of hearing boys ; and the deaf girls had about the same rank as the thirty-five percentiles of their hearing sisters. In general average, deaf boys and girls rank DEAF CHILDREN 79 seventy-five and eighty-five per cent, respectively, of the norms for hearing children of their respective sexes. It is thus seen that the efifect of sedentary life is especially marked in diminish- ing the vital capacity of the deaf. Of course, speech normally has a tendency to develop lung power, but the deaf child suffers from the deprivation of this as well as from the deficiency of physical exercises, so that his blood is insufficiently aerated and oxydized, giving rise to anaemic conditions with all their attend- ant evils. Taking as a measure of strength, the ability to compress an adjustable dynamometer, with each hand separately, it was found that deaf boys, although slightly stronger than hearing boys during their younger years, with the on-coming of adolescence, showed a marked inferiority, and this was equally palpable in the case of the less used and stronger hands. Between deaf and hearing girls no significant difference stood out, and this, as previously intimated, is but natural to expect. Hearing girls are much less given to activity, as a whole, than hearing boys, but that between hearing and deaf girls is probably not such as to carry significant weight. Further, in the way of indicating strength differences between the two groups of children, may also be employed the grip indices, i. e., the fraction which the weaker hand is of the stronger in each individual case, on the average. Here again, there was found to be no significant dif- ference between the grip indices of deaf arid hearing girls; on the contrary, deaf boys were found to be less ambidexterous, during the pre-adolesent years, but more ambidexterous after the age of maturing, than hearing boys. Taking as another measure of bodily efficiency, the quickness with which a child is able to perform voluntary co-ordinated movements as determined by the number of taps which can be made in thirty seconds, it was found that at no age was the average for deaf boys equal to the average of hearing boys, with the right hand, and with the left hand the difference in motor ability in favor of the hearing boys is still more apparent. Deaf girls in the case of both hands, were found strikingly in- ferior to hearing girls, and at no age is the average performance of the deaf significantly higher than that of their hearing sis- 8o EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES ters. It was further noted that both deaf boys and deaf girls of 12 years and older more nearly approached the standings of hearing children in tests of motor ability than the deaf of young- er years. Furthermore, while the grip indices showed deaf boys approaching maturity only, to be more ambidexterous than hear- ing boys, the motor ability indices showed that the deaf of both sexes, at all ages, are more ambidexterous on the average than their hearing companions. It may be added, too, that this latter index is, all in all, a better criterion of manual selectiveness than that of grip. The relative standing then, of deaf children as a class, in comparison with hearing children, was found to be even less favorable, where rapidity of movement is involved, than was apparent in the measurements of stature and weight, and only slightly better than their relative standing in lung capacity. By way of accounting for the more favorable showing of the deaf children of more mature years, it was pointed out that there seemed to be some intimate relation between an increased variety of mental content and growth in motility, that is, accord- ing to this view we should expect, with increased mental train- ing, a greater degree of motor activity. By this indirect means, therefore, the outflow of significant motor energy is enhanced. But the problem of increasing bodily efficiencies should be at- tacked directly. Certain provisions should be made to develop the bodies of the deaf children. Their tendencies to be inert and inactive should be counteracted or overcome. Considerable en- couragement or stimulation is necessary to bring about continual increase in bodily activity in order that their movements may ap- proach that free and unstrained type of the normal hearing chil- dren in their plays and home life. These large, free and easy movements have, besides the advantage of developing and main- taining the physical tone, the added value of furnishing stimula- tion to mental growth. On this account, deaf children should be encouraged to participate in the spontaneous out-of-door games of hearing children as far as they are able, i. e., the plays of the door-yard and of the street. Likewise, the periods of desk work in the schools, especially for children under 12 years of age should be as short as possible and the emphasis should be placed DEAF CHILDREN 8l upon the gross physical activities which are called into play in their marches, games and constructive arts. In turning to the more distinctly mental features of the in- vestigation, the results show the deaf to be equal to normal hear- ing children in the more elementary functions, such as imitation and suggestibility, which are, to be sure, extremely elementary and fundamental in character, but in the higher functions they were found to be generally inferior. In the first test, for which measurements were received — the "A-Test" — deaf boys as well as deaf girls of all ages, are found to rank from three to four years below hearing children, both as regards the time of discrimina- tion and accuracy, in selecting one symbol from others of its kind in a page of printed matter. A smaller per cent of deaf children of both sexes than hearing children were found to be able to discriminate the smallest differences in size, and in heft- ing or sensing small differences in weight, probably no differences obtain between the two classes of children. Deaf boys were found to be slightly inferior to hearing boys in general, whereas deaf girls were discovered to be generally superior to the hear- ing. Discriminations made in the latter test (weight) are based on shocks of differences into which a comparison with a con- scious standard weight-image is not thought to enter, in spite of many contentions to the contrary, that is, the sensory data are relatively less complicated than are those which enter into the sizing test, and hence the weight test, like those of imitation and sensibility, would be less liable to indicate individual differences than the test calling for discrimination of size. With regard to immediate sense-memory, which was the only aspect of that important function which it was found prac- ticable to employ on these children, it was pointed out that at all ages, the memory span of the deaf averages about two nu- merals less that that for hearing children, about the same rela- tive inferiority that was observed in the case of the "A-Test." This inferiority of the deaf on the mental side perhaps means no more than that the child is from three to four years less mature than the hearing child of his age, and that his date of maturing will be correspondingly delayed. It does not neces- sarily mean that adult deaf individuals will be much inferior in 82 EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES mental comprehension and initiative to hearing adults. Indeed, this mental retardation may be due to conditions in training, and were the deaf child's instruction begun in infancy instead of at six years, the difference might be reduced or even elimi- nated. The problem of determining the number of deaf children who are sub-normal, using a relatively pure mental standard, pre- sents difficulties which we find it impossible to meet. One can never determine just how much training, a deaf child has had. It is found much more feasible and practicable to employ as a criterion the individual's susceptibility to training. On this ba- sis the investigation showed that there are but 6 per cent of those in the rooms for deaf children who do not give promise of being able to at least support themselves on reaching ma- turity. And even these 6 per cent of the total number, will be at least partially self-supporting; that is, able to earn a live- lihood under direction. Further, not one was found whom it would seem that the State must eventually take over as a ward. The question of grading the deaf children is one that merits consideration, and it seems highly desirable to have the chil- dren well classified. Although individual instruction undoubt- edly has many advantages over class work, nevertheless the stimulus that comes from companionship with their mates in a class exercise has a decided educational value. This associa- tion without doubt goes far to incite, continue and cultivate tendencies to express and communicate ideas. It would also tend to prevent the too close dependence of children upon the teacher's initiative, and to avoid the narrowing effects ensuing from their fitting responses only to that which is expected or demanded of them. In day schools where the children are dis- persed over the city, proper classification is of course a puzzling problem. Commendable efforts have already been made in the public day schools for the teaching of the deaf in Chicago, in estab- lishing within certain districts of the city centers where a con- siderable number of deaf children are collected, making it possi- ble to grade and classify children according to age. natural apti- tude and school experience. But there is the additional desid- DEAF CHILDREN 83 eratum of a proper Course of Study for the deaf. As in the case of instruction of hearing children, there should be pro- vided for deaf children, adapted to different years of their school training, a certain definite body of information with which teachers should be expected to make the children con- versant. This is especially important in view of the fact that it becomes necessary sometimes to transfer a deaf child from a school in one part of the city to another. It thus ought to be possible to have a certain repertoire of words and activities which all deaf children should acquire during their first year of school life, and in the same way for the following three or four years. The saving of the teacher's time and energy in duplicating work already done, in discovering the amount and character of the previous training of the child, would be con- siderable, and a decided step in advance would have been taken in the direction of a proper grading of the deaf. In offering the above experimental study of the compara- tive physical and mental life of the deaf there are, of course, many psychological and educational problems which have not been touched upon. The foregoing is, however, presented with the hope that it will throw considerable new light uf>on a prob- lem which has hitherto been approached only from the stand- point of theory and personal experience. APPENDIX. HISTORICAL NOTES. So far as the Department of Child Study is aware, no scientific study has ever been undertaken with the same object in view, similar methods of procedure and with comparable groups of children. Reference should, however, be made to Dr. Love's studies * of fifty-four deaf children of the Glasgow In- stitution in 1896, and their reported standing as compared with boys of corresponding ages in the public schools of that city. The statistics on the physical characteristics of the children (grouped at ages 15, 13, 11 and 9 years respectively), relating to height, weight, circumstances of head and chest, seem to show that they compare favorably with the normal hearing children of corresponding ages, but the numbers are so small as to render it very unsafe to generalize the findings on this special group or to compare the results obtained with those of other workers. The same is true in referring to the mental qualities of the deaf children, with the added stricture that no ultra-school tests were given. Classifications of mental status were based on observa- tions and estimates of the teachers and no check is presented to us of the personal equation of the observers whereby compari- sons can be made between the mentality of these children and any other selected group of deaf children. Again one must note Binet's work ** on the head measure- ments of deaf children at the National Institute of Asnieres, France, in 1901. To these cephalic measurements should be added, as indeed they were incorporated by the author, the sta- tistics previously published by the attending physician to the in- stitution. Dr. St. Hilaire *** on height, weight, lung capacity, thoracic perimeter, strength of hand grip, circumference of head, in every item of which the deaf children are recorded as in- *J. Kerr Liove, Deaf Mutism, 1896. **Alfred Binet, Les Proportion du Crane chez Sourds-Meuts L'Annee Psychologlque, VIII, 3901 ; 385. *«*Etlenne SaJnt-Hllaire, La Surdl-Mutitfi, fitude Medicale, 1900. 86 APPENDIX ferior to the normal hearing children of the same ages. The spe- cific object of this study was to note cephalic changes in a com- parison of the three groups according to age of the children ex- amined. The shape of the heads of deaf children between five and seven years was compared with the group nine to eleven years, at which time the transformations evidenced by the vari- ous diameters were; pronounced, and these two groups were, in turn, compared with the older groups, ranging in age from fifteen to seventeen years. The comparative status of the deaf and hearing children in these French institutions will be referred to on a later page of this study. Any attempt to draw direct com- parisons between groups of children must always be open to certain reservations, but in this case it may be noted at once that the deaf children attending the public day schools of Chicago are a specially selected group, due to the preliminary entrance examinations to these rooms which are given by this depart- ment. Dr. Ferrai, in 1902, made some comparative psychological experiments * on twenty- four deaf children and twenty-two normal orphaned children from socially comparable institu- tions in Rome. The recorded results of the mental tests which dealt, it should be noted, only with simple and complex memory, ought to be of the greatest importance, but unfortunately they are not of such a character as, to show anything more than dif- ferences due to training and environment. The general conclu- sions exhibit pretty much what might be expected from the ma- terial presented — that the deaf-mutes have a memory much in- ferior to hearing children, and give evidence of much greater improvement and progress during a definite period, because in- deed, there is greater possibility of rapid improvement in their case before the psychological limits are reached with the subject matter at hand. The study made by Schaefer and Mahner (**) of four deaf, four blind and four normal hearing children is likewise of interest, mainly for the attempt to establish direct compari- sons between these special and variant groups of children. The *I>r. Carlo Ferrai, Recerchl comparative Psicologia ^)erimentale sui sordo multl. Intern. Archiv fur Schulhygiene. Band I. heft 4. **Ztsch. f. Psych., 38; 1905, pp. 1-23, APPENDIX 87 results are, however, vitiated by the fact that none of the deafs were congenital, but became afflicted between the ages of 2 to 9 years. Furthermore, the attempt to make comparisons be- tween the deaf children at each age and the normal hearing chil- dren two years younger may have seemed suggestive for the special group of institutions investigated, but certainly cannot be supposed to yield results susceptible of very general applica- tion. The somewhat intensive study of Dr. Decroly and Mme. Degand on visual memory of seven deaf children is in a measure deserving of special mention for its insistence upon specifically psychological methods of attacking the problem of the development of the deaf. The deaf children who were com- pared with normal hearing children are reported as not of equal ages in each instance, and consequently no true index of the rate of natural mental maturing is given us. Such an important investigation as Dr. F. Danzieger's of the 286 deaf mutes of the institution at Ratibor, Germany, deals as the title shows, specifically with malformations of palate and its relation to nose, eye and ear defects, but in general it can- vasses the characteristics of the family medium into which deaf children are born — the prevalence of deafness in the blood rela- tions and the diffused neuropathic tendencies of the near-of-kin. It may be described as a study which is largely taken up with a discussion and comparison of groups of deaf children with one another and not of deaf children with hearing children. These groups are divided on the basis of the casual factors of deafness and also more or less upon the time of appearance of the afflic- tion. •L'Annee Ps.vehol. (3906), Vol. 13; p. 128. **Dr. P. Danzieger, "Die ilissbilclungen des Gaumensi nnd ihr Zusammeuhang mit Nase, Auge, n. Ohr," Wiesbaden, 1900, APPENDIX A COMPAEISON OF DEAP AND HEAEINO BOYS OP CHICAGO AND PAEIS. 10 years 12 years 14 years Features examined Deaf Hear- ing Per cent which deaf is of hearing Deaf Hear- ing Per cent which deaf is of hearing Deaf Hear- ing Per cent which deaf is of hearing Height Chicago . . . Paris mm. 1335.4 1243.0 mm. 1330.3 1340.0 100.4 92.7 mm. 1370.2 1310.0 mm. 1418.9 1370.0 96.5 94.9 mm. 1528.1 1430.0 mm. 1547.4 1510.0 98. 9 94.7 Weight Chicago . . . Paris kgs. 28.18 27.47 kgs. 29.3 31.00 95.8 88.6 kgs. 30.4 32.24 kgs. 35.6 33.0 85.3 97.9 kgs. 40.09 39.17 kgs. 44.7 41.0 89.5 95.5 Luner capacity Chicago . . . Paris cu. cm. 1466 1000 cu. cm. 1659 1670 88.3 59.8 cu. cm. 1704 1190 cu. cm. 1956 1800 87.1 66.1 cu. cm. 2171 1741 cu. cm. 2527 2030 85.9 85.7 Circum. of CUcago . . . Paris mm. 527 508 521.2 519.0 539.3 529.0 Head 521 97.3 533 97.3 533 99. 2 Hand I erip 1 Chicago R. H... L. H... Paris R. H... L. H... kgs. 19.5 17.8 12.87 13.37 kgs. 17.7 16.6 17.5 17.0 108.0 107.0 73.5 78.6 kgs. 21.7 20.7 17.5 16.6 kgs. 22.4 20.6 21.12 19.5 92.4 100.0 82.8 85.1 kgs. 28.9 28.5 23.66 23.0 kgs. 30.4 28.04 25.45 21. 18 95.0 101-8 92.9 95.4 The direct comparison of the Deaf and Hearing Boys; of Chicago and Paris, as well as the relatiye standing of the Deaf as compared with the Hearing can be easily read off In the Tahle.