BOUGHT WITH THE INCOME FROM THE SAGE ""ENDOWMENT FUND THE GIFT OF ilenriS W. Sage 1891 A/..^.f^££..EU.SIC: :^.^^f.7.. Cornell University Library ML 3805.T24 1896 Sound and musician elementary treatise o 3 1924 021 774 918 The original of tiiis book is in tine Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924021774918 SOUND AND MUSIC. CamhriSge : PBINTED BY J. AND C. il'. CLAY, AT THE UNIVBESITY PRESS. SOUND AND MUSIC: AN ELEMENTAEY TREATISE ON THE PHYSICAL CONSTITUTION OF i¥lu£5wal ^outttis antr l^armonp. BY SEDLEY TAYLOR, M.A. FOBMEBLY PELLOW OF TEINITY COLLEGE, CAMBKIDGE. THIED EDITION. Hon&ott : MACMILLAN AND CO. AND NEW YOEK 1896 [All Rights reserved.'] First Edition, 1873; Second liiy, Third, 1896. PREFACE TO THE FIEST EDITION. The following treatise, portions of which have been delivered in lectures at the South Kensington Museum, the Royal Academy of Music, and else- where, aims at placing before persons unacquainted with Mathematics an intelligible and succinct ac- count of that part of the Theory of Sound which constitutes the physical basis of the Art of Music. No preliminary knowledge, save of Arithmetic and of the musical notation in common use, is assumed to be possessed by the reader. The importance of combining theoretical and experimental modes of treatment has been kept steadily in view through- out. The author has incorporated the chief Acoustical discoveries of Professor Helmholtz, but adopted his own course in explaining them and developing their connection with the previously established aS PREFACE. portion of the subject. The present volume, there- fore, even where its obhgations to the great German philosopher are the deepest, is not a mere epitome of his work \ but the result of independent study. Tbinitt College, Cambbidge, June, 1873. Die Lehre von den Tonempfindungen. Dritte Ausgahe. Braunschweig. 1870. Of this profound and exhaustive treatise it is not too much to say that it does for Acoustics what the Principia of Newton did for Astronomy. PREFACE TO THE SECOND EDITION. The present edition is the result of a careful revision applied to its predecessor. An unsatisfactory artificial hypothesis as to the motion of particles in the surface of water-waves, which was avowedly introduced into the first edition for the sake of simplicity, is replaced by a mode of treatment based upon observed facts. Points on which special difficulty was ex- perienced by readers of the first edition are here somewhat more fully elucidated. The author has, of course, consulted the revised edition ' of Helmholtz's Tonempjindungen. Trinity College, Cambkidoe, June, 1883. ' Vierte umgearbeitete Ausgahe. Braunschweig, \2>T1. PREFACE TO THE THIRD EDITION. The present edition is substantially identical with its predecessor. A reference to the chief acoustical discoveries of the late Professor von Helmholtz, which appeared' on the title-page of the previous editions, has been omitted here. It is no longer called for, now that the eminently fruitful contributions to acoustical knowledge made by that great discoverer are de- finitively incorporated in the mass of our scientific heritage. Tbinity College, Cambmdge, December, 1895. CONTENTS. CHAPTER L ON SOUND IN GENEEAL AND THE MODE OF ITS TRANSMISSION. Sensation of Sound, and its cause, § 1 — Connexion of Sound with motion, § 2 — Velocity of Sound, § 3 — Stationary media of Sound, § 4 — Motion of sea-waves, § 5 — Description of a wave, § 6 — Length, amplitude and form of wave, § 7 — Water-waves, § 8 — Waves due to transverse vibrations, § 9 — Periodic vibra- tions, § 10 — Form of wave and mode of vibration, §§11, 12, 13 — Wave on the surface of a field of standing corn, § 14 — Longi- tudinal vibrations, §§ 15, 16 — Condensation and rarefaction, § 17 — Associated wave, § 18 — Law of pendulum- vibration, § 19 — Pressure and density of air; Mariotte's law, § 20 — Trans- mission of sonorous waves along a tube of uniform bore, § 21 — Unconstrained motion of Sound-waves, § 22 — Musical and non- musical sounds, § 23. pp. 1 — 52 CHAPTER II. ON LOUDNESS AND PITCH. Three elements of a musical sound, loudness, pitch and quality, § 24 — Loudness and extent of vibration, § 25 — Pitch and rapidity of vibration; the Syren ; continuity of pitch, §§ 26, 27 — Measure of pitch; vibration-numbers, § 28 — Limits of musical sounds, § 29 — Relative pitch; intervals, § 30 — Tonic intervals of the Major scale; concords and discords, § 31 — Additional notes required for the Minor scale, g 32 — Measure of intervals, § 33 — Vibration-fractions, § 34 — Table of vibration-fractions for the tonic intervals of the Major and Minor scales, § 35 — Calculation of the vibration-numbers of all the notes in a scale from the vibration-number of its tonic, § 36. pp. 53 — 73 X CONTENTS. CHAPTER III. ON RESONANCE. Resonance of pianoforte wires and tuning-forks; cause of the phenomenon, §§37, 38 — Resonance of a column of air ; laws of its production, § 39 — Relation between the length of an air- column and the pitch of its note of maximum resonance, § 40 — Resonance-boxes, § 41 — Helmholtz's resonators, § 42. pp. 74—86 CHAPTER IV. ON QUALITY. Composite nature of musical sounds in general ; series of con- stituent tones, and law which connects them, § 43 — Experimental analysis of musical sounds, §§ 44, 45 — Nomenclature of the sub- ject, § 46 — Helmholtz's theory of musical quality as depending on the number, orders and relative intensities of the partial-tones present in any given clang, § 47. pp. 87 — 99 CHAPTER V. ON THE ESSENTIAL MECHANISM OP THE PRINCIPAL MUSICAL INSTRU- MENTS, CONSIDERED IN REFERENCE TO QUALITY. Sounds of tuning-forks, § 48 — Modes of vibration of an elastic tube, § 49 — Meeting of equal and opposite pulses; formation of nodes, § 50 — Number of nodes formed, § 51 — Nature and rate of segmental vibration, §§ 52, 53 — Motion of a sounding string; quality and pitch of its note, §§ 54, 55— The pianoforte, § 56 — Meeting of equal and opposite systems of longitudinal waves, § 57 — Reflexion of Sound at a closed and at an open orifice, § 58 — Modes of segmental vibration in stopped and open pipes, §§ 59, 60 — Deepest note obtainable from a pipe, § 61 — Relation between length of pipe and pitch of note, § 62 — Theory of re- sonance-boxes, § 63 — Flue-pipes and reed-pipes, § 64 — Construc- tion of flue-pipes and quality of their sounds, § 65 — Mechanism of a reed ; quality of an independent reed, and of a reed associated with a pipe, § 66— Orchestral wind-instruments, § 67 — Mechan- ism of the human voice, § 68 — Synthetic confirmation of Helm- holtz's theory of quality, § 69. pp. \QQ \^\ CONTENTS. CHAPTER VI. ON THE CONNEXION BETWEEN QUALITY AND MODE OF VIBRATION. Composition of vibrations, §§ 70, 71 — Phase of a vibration ; non- dependence of quality on differences of phase among partial-tones, § 72 — Simple and resultant wave-forms; Fourier's theorem, § 73. pp. 142—153 CHAPTER VII. ON THE INTEEFERENCE OF SOUND AND ON 'BEATS.' Composition of vibrations of equal periods, § 74 — Two sounds producing silence, § 75 — Beats of simple tones, § 76 — Graphic representation of beats, § 77 — Experimental study of beats, § 78. pp. 164—166 CHAPTER VIII. ON CONCORD AND DISCORD, Helmholtz's discovery of the nature of dissonance ; conditions under which it may arise between two simple tones, § 79 — Mode of determining the whole dissonance produced by two composite sounds, § 80 — Classification of the tonic intervals of the scale according to their freedom from dissonance, §§ 81-86 — Picture of amounts of dissonance for all intervals not wider than one Octave, § 87 — Consonance dependent on quality, § 88 — Apparent objection to Helmholtz's theory of quality, § 89 — Combination- tones, § 90 — Their use in defining certain consonant intervals for simple tones, § 91 — Divergence from the views of musical theorists, § 92 — Dissonance due to combination-tones produced between the partial-tones of clangs forming a given interval, § 93. pp. 167—189 xii CONTENTS. CHAPTER IX. ON CONSONANT TRIADS. Rules for the employment of vibration-fractions, §§ 94-96 — Inver- sion of intervals, § 97 — Definition of a consonant triad, § 98 — Determination of all the consonant tonic triads within one Octave, § 99 — Arrangement in two groups,§ 100 — Mutual relations between the members of each group, § 101 — Notation of Thorough Bass, § 102 — Fundamental and inverted positions of common chords, § 103— Effects of Major and Minor chords, § 104. pp. 190—201 CHAPTER X. ON PURE INTONATION AND TEMPERAMENT. Successive intervals of the Major scale, § 105 — Requisites for pure intonation in keyed instruments, §§ 106-108 — Tempering and temperament, § 109 — System of equal temperament, § 110 — Its defects, § 111 — Its influence on vocal intonation, § 112 — Cum- brousness and inefficiency of the established pitch-notation for vocal music, §113 — The 'Tonic Sol-fa' pitch-notation, § 114 — Its simple and eflfective character, § 115 — Relation of the physical theory of consonance and dissonance to the sesthetics of Music, § 116 — Importance of extreme discords; conclusion, § 117. pp. 202—223 EEEATUM. Page 16, line 6, for movement read moment. SOUND AND MUSIC. CHAPTER I. ON SOUND IN GENERAL AND THE MODE OF ITS TRANS- MISSION. 1. In listening to a sound, all that we are im- mediately conscious of is a peculiar sensation. This sensation obviously depends on the action of our organs of hearing ; for if we close our ears the sen- sation is greatly weakened, or, if originally but feeble, altogether extinguished. Persons whose auditory apparatus is malformed, or has been destroyed by disease, may be totally unconscious of any sound, even during a thunderstorm or the discharge of artillery. It would be entii-ely in accordance with the mode of action of our other senses if what we feel as Sound is represented, externally to our- selves, by a state of things very different to the sensation we experience. Analogy, then, indicates T. 1 \\ 2 SENSATIONS AND THEIR CAUSES. [I. § 1. that some purely mechanical phenomena external to the ear will prove to be turned into the sensation we call Sound by a process carried on within that organ and the brain with which it is in direct communica- tion. This mechanical agency, whatever may be its nature, is usually set going at a distance from the ear, and, to reach it, must traverse the intervening space. In doing so it can pass through solid and liquid as well as gaseous bodies. For instance, if one end of a felled tree is gently scratched with the point of a penknife, the sound is distinctly audible to a listener whose ear is pressed against the other end of the tree. When a couple of pebbles are knocked together under water, the sound of the blow reaches the ear after first passing through the intervening liquid. That Sound travels through the air is a matter of universal experience, and needs no illustration. In every case accessible to common observation where Sound passes from one point of space to another, it necessarily traverses matter, either in a solid, liquid or gaseous form. We n)ay hence conjecture that the presence of a material medium of some kind is indispensable to the trans- mission of Sound. This important point can be readily brought to the test of experiment, as follows. Let a bell kept ringing by clockwork be placed under the receiver of an air-pump, and the air gradually I. § 2.] CONNMXIOF OF SOUND WITH MOTION. 3 exhausted. Provided that suitable precautions are t^rken to prevent communication of Sound to the external air through the body of the receiver, the bell will appear to ring more and more feebly as the exhaustion proceeds, until at last it altogether ceases to be heard. While the air is being readmitted, the sound of the bell will gradually recover its original loudness. This experiment shows that Sound can- not travel in vacuo, but requires for its transmission a material medium of some kind. The air of the atmosphere is, in the vast majority of cases, the medium which conveys to the ear the mechanical impulse which that wonderful organ translates, as it were, into the language of Sound. 2. Having ascertained that a material medium acts in every case as the carrier of Sound, we have next to examine in what manner it performs this function. The roughest observations suffice to put us on the right track in this enquiry by pointing to a connexion between Sound and Motion. The passage through the air of sounds of very great intensity is accompanied by effects which prove the atmosphere to be in a state of violent commotion. The explosion of a powder-magazine is capable of shattering the windows of houses at several miles' distance. In the case of sounds of only ordinary loudness the accompanying air-motion manifests itself in no such 1—2 4 VELOCITY OF SOUND. [I. § 3. unmistakable way : a little attention will, however, usually detect a certain amount of movement on the part of the sound-producing apparatus, which is probably capable of being communicated to the surrounding air. Thus, a sounding pianoforte-string can be both seen and felt to be in motion : the movements of a finger-glass stroked on the rim by a wet finger can be recognised by observing the thriUs which play on the surface of the water it contains : sand strewed on a horizontal drum-head is thrown off when the drum is beaten. These considerations raise a presumption that Sound is invariably associated with agitation of the conveying medium — ^that it is impossible to produce a sound without at the same time setting the medium in motion. If this should prove to be the case, there would be ground for the further conjecture that motion of a material medium constitutes the mechanical impulse which, falUng on the ear, excites within it the sensation we call Sound. Let us try to form an idea of the hind of motion which the conditions of the case require. 3. We may conveniently begin by determining the rate at which Sound travels. This varies, indeed, with the nature of the conveying medium. It will suffice, however, for our present purpose to ascertain its velocity in air, the medium through which the vast majority of sounds reach our ears. As long as I. § 3.] VELOCITY OF SOUND. 5 we confine our attention to sounds originating at but small distances from us, their passage through the intervening space appears instantaneous. If, however, a gun is fired at a considerable distance, the flash is seen before the report is heard — a proof that an appreciable interval of time is occupied by the transmission of the sound. The occurrence of an echo, in a position where we can measure the dis- tance passed over by the sound in travelling from the position where it is produced to that where it rebounds, gives us the means of measuring the velo- city of Sound ; since we can, by direct observation, ascertain how long a time is spent on the out-and- home journey. The following easy experiment gives a near approximation to the actual velocity of Sound — in fact a much closer one than the rough nature of the observation would have led one to expect. In the North cloister of Trinity College, Cambridge, there is an unusually distinct echo from the wall at its eastern extremity. Standing near the opposite end of the cloister, I clapped my hands rhythmically at a rate such that the strokes and echoes were heard alternately at equal intervals of time. A friend at my side, watch in hand, counted the number of strokes and echoes. The result was that there were 76 in half a minute, i.e. 38 strokes and 38 echoes. A little consideration will show that the sound 6 VELOCITY OF SOUND. [I. § 4. traversed the cloister and returned to the point of its origination regularly once in each interval of time elapsing between the delivery of a stroke and the perception of its echo. Since each such interval was exactly equal to that between the perception of an echo and the delivery of the following stroke, the whole movement of Sound took place in alternate equal intervals, i.e. in half the observed time, or fifteen seconds. Accordingly the sound travelled to and fro in the cloister 38 times in 15 seconds. The length thus traversed, I found by pacing to be about 419 feet. The velocity of Sound per second thus comes out equal to — —z — , or 1061 ' feet and 15 a fraction. Sound, then, travels through the air at the rate of upwards of 1,000 feet in a second, which is more than 600 miles an hour, or about 15 times the speed of an express train. In solid and liquid bodies its velocity is still greater, attaining in the case of steel-wire a speed of from 15,000 to 17,000 feet in a second ^ or, roughly speaking, about 200 times that of an express train. 4. Though the Sound-impulse advances with ' This is about 50 feet below its exact value under the circumstances of my observation. See Tyndall's Sound, Third edition, p. 23. = Tyndall's Sound, p. 38. I. §5.] MOTION OF SEA-WAVES. 7 a steady and high velocity, the medium by which it is transmitted clearly does not share such a motion. Solid conductors of Sound remain, on the whole, at rest during its passage, and a slight yielding of their separate parts is all that their constitution generally admits of. In fluids, or in the air, a continuous forward motion is equally out of the question. The movement of the particles composing the Sound-conveying medium will be found to be of a kind examples of which are con- stantly presenting themselves, but without attract- ing an amount of attention at all commensurate with their interest and importance. 5. An observer who looks down upon the sea from a moderate elevation, on a day when the wind, after blowing strongly, has suddenly dropped, sees long lines of waves advancing towards the shore at a uniform pace and at equal distances from each other. The effect to the eye is that of a vast army marching up in column, or of a ploughed field moving along horizontally in a direction perpendi- cular to the lines of its ridges and hollows. The actual motion of the water is, however, very differ- ent from its apparent motion, as may be ascertained by noticing the behaviour of a cork or other body floating on the surface of the sea, and therefore sharing its movement. The floating body does not 8 MOTION OF PARTICLES IN A SEA-WAVE. [I. § 5. advance with the waves, but rides over their crests and sinks into their troughs as though it were a buoy at anchor. Hence, while the waves travel steadily forward horizontally, each of the fluid particles which compose them describes over and over again a fixed orbit of its own. Thus, when we say that the waves advance hori- zontally, we mean, not that the masses of water of which they at any given instant consist advance, but that these masses, by virtue of the separate motions of their individual particles, successively arrange themselves in the same relative positions, so that the cui-ved shapes of the surface, which we call waves, are horizontally transmitted without their materials sharing in the progress. The accompanying figure will show how this happens. Fig. 1. A.-""^^ A'- '^^^\ -. ^ B B' Let the full curved line AB represent a section of a part of the sea-surface at any given instant made by a vertical plane through the direction of wave-motion, and suppose that during, say, the next ensuing second of time, the separate fluid particles rearrange themselves by virtue of theii' I. §5.] TRANSMISSION OF WAVES. 9 respective orbital motions in such a manner that, at the end of that second, they constitute a curved line identical in shape and size with AB, and only differing from it in horizontal position. Let the dotted line A'B' represent the curve thus formed. As the two outlines AB and A'B' are exactly alike, the joint effect produced by the separate particle- movements on the eye of a spectator is just what it would have been had we pushed the curve AB along horizontally untU it came to occupy the position A'B'. In order further to illustrate this point, let us suppose that a hundred men are standing in a line and that the first ten are ordered to kneel down : a Spectator who is too far off to distinguish individuals will merely see a broken line like that in the figure below. Now, suppose that after one second the eleventh man is ordered to kneel and the first to stand j after two seconds the twelfth man to kneel and the second to stand; and so on. There will then con- tinue to be a row of ten kneeling men, but during each second it will be shifted one place along the line. The distant observer will therefore see a depression steadily advancing along the line. The 10 TRANSFERENCE OF RELATIVE POSITION. [I. § 6. state of things presented to his eye after twenty, sixty and ninety seconds, respectively, is shown in Fig. 2. Fig. 2. There is here no horizontal motion on the part of the men composing the line, but their vertical motions give rise, in the way explained, to the hori- zontal transference of the depression along the line. The reader should observe that for no two con- secutive seconds does the kneeling row consist of exactly the same men, while in such positions as those shown in the figures, which are separated by ten or more seconds of time, the men who form it are totally difierent. 6. Let us now return to the sea-waves and examine more closely the elements of which they consist. Fig. 3 represents a vertical section of one com- plete wave. I. §6.] THREE ELEMENTS OF A WAVE. 11 The dotted line is that in which the horizontal plane forming the surface of the sea when at rest PVj.3 cuts the plane of the figure; it is called the level- line. The distance between the two extreme points of the wave, measured along this line, is called the length of the wave. C is the highest point of the crest DCB ; ^ the lowest point of the trough AED. CF and EG are vertical straight lines through C and E ; HCK and LEM are horizontal straight lines through the same two points. The vertical distance between the lines HK and LM is called the amplitude of the wave. Thus AB is the length of the wave, and, if we produce EG and CF to cut the lines HK and LM m N and P respectively, we have, for its amplitude, either of the equal lines EN, PC. Each of these is clearly equal to FC and GE together, that is to say, the amplitude of the wave is equal to the height of the crest above the level- line together with the depth of the trough below it. In addition to the length and amplitude of the wave, we have one more element, its form. The wave in the figure has its crest shorter than its 12 THREE ELEMENTS OF A WAVE. [I. § 7. trough and higher than its trough is deep. More- over the part DC of the crest is steeper than the part CB, while in the trough the parts AE and ED are nearly equally steep. Sea-waves have infinitely various shapes dependent on the character of the wind which originated them and on the depth and configuration of the sea bottom over which they pass. Hence, before we can lay down a wave in a figure, we must know the nature of the wave's curve, i.e. its form. Since the crests of the waves are raised above the ordinary level of the sea, the troughs must necessarily be depressed below it, just as, in a ploughed field, the earth heaped up to form the ridges must leave empty the furrows from which it was taken. Each crest being thus associated with a trough, it is convenient to regard one crest and one trough as forming together one complete wave. Thus each wave consists of a part raised above, and a part depressed below, the horizontal plane which would be the surface of the sea were it not being traversed by waves. 7. The length, amplitude and form of a wave completely determine the wave, just as the length, breadth and height of an oblong block of wood, i.e. its three dimensions, fix the size of the block. These three elements of a wave are mutually independent, I- § r.] WATER.WAVES. 13 that is to say, we may alter any one of them with- out altering the other two. This will be seen by a glance at the accompanying figures. (l) shows variation in length alone; (2) in am- plitude alone ; (3) inform^ alone. Fig. 4 (1) (2) ^ The reader may rightly object that between the three curves in (2) there are what would, according to ordinary phraseology, be described as dififerences oiform. That term, however, as here used, means generic shape in the same way that the terms 'triangular,' 'rectangular,' 'elliptic,' &c. have meanings inde- pendent of differences of mere dimension. 14 WATHB-WAVUS. [I- § 8. 8. We will next examine more closely the separate movements of particles at the surface of water, and their joint result the transmission of waves. The brothers Heinrich and Wilhelm Weber published, in 1825, a series of very careful obser- vations on water-waves passing along troughs with glass sides not unlike very long and very narrow aquariums. By ingeniously planned arrangements, which it would take too long to describe here, they ascertained that each particle on the surface continued, during the passage of a succession of equal waves, to describe a fixed oval path in a vertical plane, the longer diameter of the oval being horizontal. From the fact that the difference between the horizontal and vertical diameters of the oval diminished as the depth of the water in- creased, the brothers Weber inferred that at the surface of very deep water these diameters would be equal, and the orbits of the particles there- fore exact circles. Fig. 5 shows how a series of particles, by de- scribing fixed circular paths in a vertical plane, cause progressive waves to be transmitted hori- zontally. At the top of the figure are 17 spots representing as many particles floating at equal distances along a straight line in the undisturbed surface of very deep water indicated by dots in the I. § 8.] WATHE-WAVES. Fig. 5. 15 0) lO-fl ><* (D ©d) ©(i)(D (i)CDd> Ocr>0 axDO 3 ij a a ^ S S^ "^ '»» 16 WATEE-WAVMS. fl. § 8. figure. The equal vertical circles are the paths which the particles are respectively about to de- scribe in the direction in which the hands of a watch move, as indicated by a curved arrow in the figure, and with equal uniform velocities. (0) represents the state of things at a movement when the group of particles plot out two complete equal waves included by the brackets A and B, each consisting of a crest to the right and trough to the left as marked by the sub-brackets a, a' and b, V respectively. The horizontal dotted line represents, as before, the section of the undisturbed surface or level-line. The reader wOl at once observe, on looking along the line of circles firom left to right, that each particle occupies in its own orbit a position one-eighth of a revolution behind that of the particle to the left of it. (1) shows the arrangement of the particles when an interval of time equal to one-eighth of the period of a complete cu-cle-revolution has elapsed from the moment at which they were grouped as in (0). Similarly (2) represents the state of things after two-eighths of that period, (3) after three-eighths of it, and so on, until in (8), one complete period of revolution having in all elapsed, the particles are necessarily again in the same positions as those shown in group (O). I. §9.] WAVE-MOTION. 17 If now the reader allows his eye to be guided by the oblique dotted lines of the figure, he will readily perceive that the wave A has moved horizontally to the right at each of the above stages and, after the lapse of one complete period of particle-revolution occupies, in (8), the position which the wave B held in (0), i.e. has traversed its own wave-length from left to right. We may express this result generally by saying that while an individual particle performs one complete orbital revolution, the wave advances one wave-length. This is the fundamental pro- position of wave-motion, and should be carefully mastered and remembered. In the figure employed to demonstrate it a wave has been only roughly plotted out by a small number of spots, and its movements estimated at but a few arbitrarily chosen instants. By increasing the numbers of these two elements, however, we might make an indefinitely near ap- proach to continuity both of form and of motion. 9. The characteristic phenomenon of wave- motion, viz. an apparent forward movement unshared by the materials which give rise to it, though most frequently seen on the surface of water, is by no means confined to fiuid bodies. When a carpet is being shaken, bulging forms exactly like water-waves are seen running along it. A flexible string, one end of which is tied to a fixed point and the other T. 2 18 TRANSVERSE VIBRATIONS. [I. §,9. held in the hand, exhibits the same phenomenon when the loose end is sharply jerked aside. It has accordingly been found convenient to extend the term wave in order to designate as ' wave-motion ' any movement coming under the definition just given. We proceed to an instance in which th& individual particles, instead of describing circular orbits move to-and-fro, i.e. vibrate, in straight lines perpendicular to the direction of wave-propagation. Before applying to this case the method followed in Fig. 5 it will be desirable to define what is meant by a ' complete vibration ' as that term is used by Enghsh writers. Let a point move from A' to A along the straight Tig.G A JL' line A'A and then back again to A'. A French writer would describe this movement as made up of two complete vibrations executed in opposite directions. An English writer would designate it as one complete vibration, consisting of two half- vibrations performed in opposite directions. This latter usage will be adhered to in the sequel. The I. § 9.] TRANSVERSE VIBRATIONS. 19 completion of a vibration is thus to be determined by the moving point having traversed its entire path, in both directions, and this is equally true if instead of starting from either extremity it occu- pies initially an intermediate position such as O and moves from to A, from A to A' and from A' to ; or reversely from O to A', from A' to A and from A to O. The ' period ' of a vibration accordingly means the time which it takes the moving point to perform one such complete cycle of its motion. Let the 17 spots lying equidistantly along the dotted straight line at the head of Fig. 7 represent as many particles of an elastic string stretched between fixed points' of attachment not shown in the figure. These particles are about to vibrate in straight lines perpendicular to the direction of the string. (0) shows the state of things when the particles plot out two complete waves A and B. The distinction between 'crest' and 'trough' has now disappeared. All that can be said is that each wave is formed of two protuberances lying on oppo- site sides of the undisturbed position of the string, which replaces the 'level-line' of the water-waves and is, like it, represented by a dotted line in the figure. By looking along from left to right the reader 2—2 20 TRANSVERSE VIBRATIONS. Fig. 7 rr* [I. § 9. • : • • ! • • ,*' ,• >•■ .*'■ .• • i. yy: y ^■\>^ *' ,•' i •' k >< i • \m J^ 0\ i • i^" •' ; I .if'' y; ,••'! > •'; y ;,•' m « H y ly y • i • V^ ;_*■' ! y ; y _y • i j y iy .^i' • i • ly' y' •! • i • Y • I • i • I " • ' i y • • • • "a" Is^ "«■ '»" ^S, >2, -2^ S ^ I. § 9.] TRANSVERSE VIBRATIONS. 21 will notice that each particle occupies in its own path a position one-eighth of a period of vibration behind that of the particle next to the left of it. (Compare p. 16.) (1), (2), (3). ..show the positions of the par- ticles after one-eighth, two-eighths, three-eighths... of a complete period of particle- vibration. By follow- ing any one of the vertical lines of spots it will be seen that in the instance selected for this figure, each particle moves more rapidly in the neighbour- hood of its undisturbed position than it does hear the extremities of its swing. In (8) the particles have returned to their original positions and the wave A is where B was in (0). The particles have completed one vibrational cycle and the wave has advanced by its own length. This result may be thus generalised : While an individual particle performs one complete vibration the wave advances one wave-length. The proposition proved above (p. 17) for water-waves is, therefore, also true of waves due to transverse vibrations, i.e. such as are executed perpendicularly to the direction of wave-propagation. As waves thus produced are of leading importance in the theory of Sound, it is necessary to study them in some detail. Let a particle originally at rest at in the initial line (Fig. 7 his) be cooperating in the transmission of a wave. This wave is drawn in the figure in two 22 SUCCESSIVE VIBBATIONS. [I. § 10. positions such that the two points of its curve the most distant from the initial Hne, A and A', are situated in two straight lines OA and OA' drawn through in opposite directions, each perpendicular to the initial line. It is evident that, at the moments when the wave is in these positions, the particle originally at will be at A and A' respectively, and that these two points mark the limits of its vibration. Hence the line AA' is the extent of the particle's vibration. But by drawing parallels to the initial line through A and A' it wOl be seen, by reference to the definition in § 6, that AA' is also the ampli- tude of the wave. ' Extent of particle- vibration ' and 'amplitude of corresponding wave' are, therefore, only different ways of expressing the same thing. 10. When a series of continuous equal waves are being transmitted, each particle, after completing one vibration, will instantly commence another pre- cisely equal vibration, and go on doing so as long as the transmission of waves is maintained. This I. § 10.] SUCGESSIVE VIBRATIONS. 23 kind of motion, in which the same movement is continuously repeated in successive equal intervals of time, is called 'periodic,' and the time which any one of the movements occupies is called its ' period.' Thus, to continuous equal waves corre- spond continuous periodic particle-vibrations. We will next compare the periods of the vibrations which produce waves of different lengths advancing at the same speed. In Fig. 8, waves of three different lengths are represented. One wave of (l) is as long as two of CO c^) (?) (2), and as three of (3). Therefore, in virtue of the fact proved in § 9, a particle makes one complete 24 SUCCESSIVE VIBRATIONS. [I. § 10. vibration in (1) while the long wave passes from A to B, two in (2) while the shorter waves there pre- sented pass over the same distance, and three in the case of the shortest waves of (3). But the velocities of these waves being by our supposition equal, the times of describing the distance AB wUl be the same in (l), (2) and (3). Hence a particle in (2) vibrates twice as rapidly, and in (3) three times as rapidly, as in (l); or conversely, vibration in (l) is half as rapid as in (2), and one-third as rapid as in (3). The rates of vibration in (l), (2) and (3) (by which we mean the numbers of vibrations performed in any given interval of time) are, therefore, propor- tional to the numbers 1, 2 and 3, which are them- selves inversely proportional to the wave-lengths in the three cases respectively. We may express our result thus ; The rate of particle-vibration is inversely proportional to the corresponding wave-length. Simi- lar reasoning will apply equally weU to any other case ; the proposition, therefore, though deduced from the relations of particular waves, holds for waves in general. The converse proposition admits of easy indepen- dent proof as follows. It has been shown (p. 21) that in one period of particle-vibration a wave traverses its own length. This length must there- fore, if the velocity of the wave remain constant, I. §11.] MODES OF VIBRATION. 25 be proportional to the period, i.e. inversely propor- tional to the rate of vibration. 11. We have now connected the extent of the particle-vibration with the amplitude, and its rate with the length, of the corresponding wave. It remains to examine what feature of the vibratory- movement corresponds to the third element, the form of the wave. Fig. 9. Suppose that two boys start together to run a race from O to -4, from A to B, and from B back to 0, and that they reach the goal at the same moment. They may obviously do this in many different wayi?. For instance, they may keep abreast all through, or one may fall behind over the first half of the course and recover the lost ground in the second. Again, one may be in front over OAO, and the other over OBO, or each boy may pass, and be passed by, his competitor repeatedly during the race. We may regard the movement of each boy as constituting one complete vibration, and thus convince ourselves that a vibratory motion of given extent and period may be performed in an indefinitely numerous variety of modes. Let us now compare the positions of a particle at the expiration of successive equal intervals 26 VIBRATION-MODE AND WAVE-FORM. [I. § 11. of time, when cooperating in the transmission of waves of different forms. In each of the three cases in Fig. 10 the front of a wave is shown in the positions it respectively occupies at the end of ten equal intervals of time during which its intersection with the level-line moves from O through the equidistant points 1, 2, 3, 4, &c. of the initial line. A particle whose place of rest is will neces- sarily assume corresponding positions in the ver- tical line OA : thus the points where this line cuts the successive wave-fronts show the positions Fy.lO 01S3 i S 6 1 a 10 Oia34SeTBS»lO I. §12.] CONSTRUCTION OF WAVE-FORM. 27 of the vibrating particle at equal intervals of time. On comparing the three cases it wiU be seen that the mode of the particle's vibration is distinct in each. In (1), it moves fastest at 0, and then slackens its pace up to A. In (2), it starts more slowly than in (l), attains its greatest speed near the middle of OA, and again slackens on approaching A. In (3), the pace steadily increases from O to ^. The different wave-fronts shown in the figure have been purposely constructed with the same amplitude and length, in order that only such variations as were due to differ- ences of form might come into consideration. The reader should construct similar figures with other forms, and so convince himself more thoroughly that to every distinct form of wave there corresponds a special mode of particle-vibration. 12. Conversely each distinct mode of particle- vibration gives rise to a special form of wave. We will show this by actually constructing the form of wave produced by a given mode of particle-vibration when the mode in which a particle moves is given. Suppose that each particle makes one complete vibration per second about its position of original rest in the initial line and that the law of vibration is roughly indicated in Fig. 11. AB is the path described by a particle ; its 28 CONSTRUCTION OF WAVE-FORM. [I. § 12. position when in the initial line ; 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6... 12, 13... 16 its positions after 16 equal intervals of time B e- 7- 2 -I 9- 10 — Jl- 12— •0,16 -15 14 -13 A each one-sixteenth of a second : 1 6 coincides with O, as the particle has returned to its starting-point. Next, select a series of particles originally at rest in equidistant positions along the initial line, and so situated that each commences a vibration identical with that above laid down in Fig. 11 one-sixteenth of a second after the particle to the lefb of it has started on an equal vibration. Fig. 12 shows the rest-positions of the series of particles (Xj, ttj, O,^, Ctg...(tjj, in the initial line, and their contemporaneous po- sitions during a subsequent vibration. I. §12.] CONSTRUCTION OF WAVE-FORM. 29 The moment selected for the figure is that in which the first of the series, a^, is on the point of Fig. 12 .^> ^ &o i% j«a j^s i«5« H K h \ '"a % «u '"iz *«b "H* '"m \ k U moving vertically upwards out of the initial line, i.e. when the front of a wave of the form in question has just reached the point a„. Since the second particle started one-sixteenth of a second after the first, its position in the figure will be below the initial line at a/ making the line o^ al equal to the line 015 in Fig. 11. The next particle, which is two-sixteenths of a second behind «„ in its path, wUl be at al making a^al equal to 014 in the same figure. In this way the positions of all the points a/a/aj', &c., in Fig. 12 are determined from Fig. 11. They give us, at once, a general idea of the form of the resulting wave. By laying down more points along the line AB in Fig. 11, we can get as many more points on the wave as we please, and should 30 EXTENSION OF THE TERM ' WA VE.' [I. § 13 aud 14. thus ultimately arrive at a continuous curved line. This is the wave-form resulting from the given vibration-mode with which we started, and, since only one wave-form can be obtained from it, we infer that each different mode of particle-vibration gives rise to a different form of wave. 13. It has now been demonstrated that when a wave is produced by particles vibrating in a plane passing through its hne of advance, and in paths perpendicular to that line, the amplitude of the wave is equal to the extent of particle-vibration and the length and form of the wave are determined by the rate and moc?e of vibration respectively. These rela- tions were also shown to hold conversely. We will next consider a type of oscillatory movement which is important from its similarity to that to which the transmission of Sound is due. 14. Anyone who has looked down from a slight elevation upon a field of standing corn on a gusty day, must have frequently observed a kind of thrill running along its surface. As each ear of corn is capable of only a slight swaying movement, we have here necessarily an instance of loave-motion, the ear- vibrations corresponding to the particle-vibrations in the cases already examined. There is, however, this important peculiarity in the instance now before us, that the ears' movements are mainly horizontal, I. § 15.] LONGITUDINAL VIBRATIONS. 31 i.e. executed in the line of the wave's advance. The wave, therefore, no longer presents itself to the eye as exclusively a condition of alternate protuberance of outline in opposite directions, but mainly as a state of more tightly, or less tightly, packed ears. The JFig 13. annexed figure give& a rough idea how this takes place. The wind is supposed to be moving from left to right and to have just reached the ear A. Its neighbours to the right are still undisturbed. The stalk of C has just swung back into its erect posi- tion. The ears about B are closer to, and those about G further apart from, each other than is the case with those on which the wind has not yet acted. After this illustration, it will be easy to conceive a kind of wave-motion in which there is no longer any movement transverse to the direction in which the wave is advancing. 15, Let a series of particles, originally at rest in equidistant positions along a straight line, as in that at the head of Fig. 14, be executing equal 32 LONGITUDINAL VIBRATIONS. [I. § 15. periodic vibrations in that line, in such a manner that each is the same fixed amount further back in Fig. 14 B (0) « (a) (3) (4) (--) ( and hold it close to one of the apertures. As soon as the fork reaches the position (1) Fig. 22', its tone will unmistakably swell out. In order to estimate the increase of intensity produced, it is a good plan to move the fork rapidly to and fro, a few times, between the positions (1) and (2). In the first case we have the full effect of reson- ance, in the second only the unassisted tone of the fork, ' This figure was drawn for a cylinder only six inches in length, but suffices for the purpose of illustration for which it is here used. III. §39.] RESONANT AIR-COLUMNS. 81 and the contrast is very marked. We may shorten or lengthen our cylinder within certain limits and still obtain the phenomenon of resonance, but the greatest reinforcement of tone attainable with the fork selected will be produced by an air-column about twelve inches long. If we close one end of the paper cylinder, by placing it, for instance, on a table, and repeat our ex- periment at the ojaen end, only a very weak resonance is produced ; but we obtain a powerful resonance by operating with a fork of pitch | > _^ E= making half as many vibrations per second as that before employed. In this case, then, a column of air contained in a cylinder of which one end was closed resounded powerfully to a note an Octave below that which elicited its most vigorous resonance when the air-column was contained in a cylinder open at both ends. By operating in this fashion, with forks of dif- ferent pitch, on air-columns of diflferent lengths, we arrive at the following laws, which are universally true : 1. For every single musical note there exists a corresponding air-column of definite length which, when enclosed in a pipe, open at both ends, re- sounds the most powerfully to that note. T. 6 82 RESONANT AIR-COLUMNS. [III. § 40. 2. The maximum resonance of ail' in a pipe, with one end closed is produced by a note one Octave below that to which a pipe of the same length, open at both ends, resounds the most power- fully. 40. In order to ascertain the precise relation between the pitch of a note and the length of the corresponding air-column, we will examine the way in which resonance is produced in a column of air contained in a pipe closed at one end. Let A, Fig. 23, be the open, and B the closed, ends of the pipe, and let us for a moment replace the contained air by an elastic spiral spring fastened at B, and of length equal to AB. Fig. S3. uvmwwmMMVsi^imJW B A Suppose the end of the spring suddenly pushed a little way from A towards B. The coils of the spring nearest A will be squeezed together, and this condensed state of the spring will be transmitted to B, where further movement to the left is stopped by the fixed end of the spring. Hence the coils crowded together near B will begin expanding to- wards the right, i.e. the condensed state of the spring will be turned back, or ' reflected,' at B and will then travel to ^. In virtue of the elasticity III. § 40.] RESONANT AIR-GOLUMNS. 8.3 of the spring its free end will now be caused to protrude beyond the mouth of the pipe as far as it was at first pushed into it. The coils near A being thus drawn somewhat more apart than was the case when they were in their undisturbed condition, will exert a pull on those near them, which will in their turn be drawn further apart, and thus a rarefied state of the spring will be produced at A and trans- mitted to B. On reaching the fixed end there, the increased tension will act on the coils near B, i.e. the rarefied state of the spring will be reflected at B, just as its condensation was in the case previously considered. When this rarefaction returns to A, the free end of the spring will momentarily resume the position within the mouth of the pipe to which it was originally pushed. The elasticity of the spring thus causes it to lengthen and shorten as a whole in consequence of the single push originally given it, and this motion would for a time continue, its successive periods being four times the space of time occupied by a pulse of condensation or rarefac- tion in traversing the length of the pipe. The free end of the spring may, however, be pushed and pulled alternately so as to reinforce each pulse as it arrives at the mouth of the pipe, and in this manner the maximum of motion will be communicated to the spring. In this case, one outward and one 6—2 84 RESONANCE-BOXES. [III. § 41. inward impulse must be communicated to the free end of the spring during the time which elapses while a pulse traverses four times the length of the pipe. Reverting to the actual conditions of our problem, we have the resonance of the air-column in place of the alternate lengthening and shortening of the spring. The to-and-fro impulses at A are impressed by a vibrating fork. The Sound-pulse traverses four times the length of the pipe while the fork is performing one complete vibration. We know, however [§ 15 p. 32], that during this latter period the Sound-pulse produced by the fork's action traverses precisely one wave-length corresponding to the pitch of the note produced by the fork. Hence, for maximum resonance in the case of a pipe closed at one end, the wave-length corresponding to the note sounded must be four times as great as the length of the air-column, or the length of the column one quarter of the wave-length. 41. These principles give us the explanation of a valuable appliance for intensifying the sound of a tuning-fork. Such a fork, when held in the hand after being struck, communicates but little of its vibrations to the surrounding air ; when, however, its handle is screwed into one side of a wooden box of suitable dimensions, in the way shown in Fig. 24, the tone becomes much louder. The vibrations of III. § 42.] RESONATORS. 85 the fork pass from its handle to the wood of the box, £'^.24 and thence to the air- column within, which is of appropriate length for maximum resonance to the fork's note. This convenient adjunct to a tuning- fork goes by the name of a ' resonance-box.' 42. When a number of musical sounds are simultaneously sustained it is generally difficult, and often impossible, for the unaided ear to decide whether an individual note is, or is not, present in the mass of sound heard. If, however, we had an instrument which intensified the note of which we were in Fig. 25. search, without similarly reinforcing others which there was any risk of our mistaking for it, our power 86 RESONATORS. [HI. § 42. of recognising the note in question would be propor- tionately increased. Such an instrument has been invented by Helmholtz. It consists of a hollow ball of brass with two apertures at opposite ends of a diameter, as shown in Fig, 25. The larger aperture allows the vibrations of the external air to be communicated to that within the ball ; the smaller aperture terminates in a nipple of convenient form for insertion in the ear of the observer. The air contained in the ball resounds very powerfully to a single note of definite pitch, whence the instrument has been named by its in- ventor a resonator. The best way of using it is, first to stop one ear closely, and then to insert the nipple of the instrument into the other. As often as the resonator's own note is sounded in the external air, the instrument will sing it into the ear of the ob- server with extraordinary emphasis, and thus at once enable him to single out that note from among a crowd of others differing from it in pitch. A series of such resonators, tuned to particular previously selected notes, constitutes an invaluable apparatus for analysing a composite sound into the simple tones of which it is made up. CHAPTER IV. ON QUALITY. 43. The laws of resonance enable us to establish a remarkable, and by most persons utterly un- suspected fact, viz. that the notes of nearly every regular musical instrument with which we are familiar, are not, as they are ordinarily taken to be, single tones of one determinate pitch, but composite sounds containing an assemblage of such tones. These are always members of a regular series, form- ing with each other fixed intervals which may be thus stated : if we number the separate single tones of which any given sound is made up, 1, 2, 3, &c., beginning with the lowest, we have (1) The deepest, or ' fundamental,' tone, whose pitch is ordinarily regarded as that of the whole sound. (2) A tone one Octave above (l). 88 CONSTITUENTS OF COMPOSITE SOUNDS; [IV. § 43. (3) A tone a Fifth above (2), i.e. a Twelfth above (1). (4) A tone a Fourth above (3), i.e. two Octaves above (l). (5) A tone a Major Third above (4), i.e. two Octaves and a Major Third above (l). (6) A tone a Minor Third above (5), i.e. two Octaves and a Fifth above (l). These are the most important members of the series. Their vibration-numbers are connected by a simple law, which is easUy deduced from the above relations. If the fundamental tone makes 100 vibrations per second, (2) will make twice as many, i.e. 200 ; (3), being a Fifth above (2), will have for 3 its vibration-number - x 200, or 300. For (4), which is a Fourth above (3), we get similarly 4 5 - X 300, or 400 ; for (5), - x 400, or 500 ; for (6), - X 500, or 600. Thus the numbers, come out 100, 200, 300 and so on ; or generally, whatever be the vibration-number of (1), those of (2), (3), (4), &c., are respectively twice, three times, four times, &c. as large. If C in the bass clef be selected as the fundamental tone, the series, complete up to the tenth tone, is shown in musical notation as follows : — IV. § 44.] SERIES OF CONSTITUENT TONES. 89 „ f^' -?- -e- "? eE i 12 S46fi789 10 The asterisk denotes that the pitch of the 7th tone is not precisely that of the note by which it is here represented, but sHghtly flatter. The reader must not suppose that, because the tones into which a note of a musical instrument may usually be decomposed are members of a fixed aeries, all those which we have written down are neces- sarily present in every such note. No more is meant to be asserted than that those which are present, be they few or many, must occupy positions de- termined by the law connecting each tone with its fundamental. The sound may contain, say, (1), (3) and (5) only, or (1), (4) and (8) only, and so on, the rest being entirely absent, but in no case can a tone intermediate in pitch between any two consecutive members of the series make its appearance. 44. Experimental evidence shall now be pro- duced in support of the extremely important pro- position just enunciated. We will begin with the sounds of the pianoforte. Let the note BEzznz be first silently pressed down, 90 ANALYSIS OF RESONANCE. [IV. § 44. and then B^- p— be vigorously struck, and, after three or four seconds, allowed to rise again. The lower note is at once extinguished, but we now hear its Octave sounding with considerable force -e- from the wires of 3-^ . If we permit the damper to fall back on these, by releasing the key hitherto held down, the whole sound is immediately cut off. Next, retaining the same fundamental note, ^ n , let ^— 5^ r be quietly freed from its damper, and the experiment repeated as before. We shall then hear this note sounding on after the extinction of ?zEa=: • Similar results may be obtained with the three next tones, ^-°^^^'^ = . but they drop off very rapidly in intensity. The tones above A are so weak as to be practically inaudible. The series of tones produced in this succession of cases can only be due to resonance. But, as has been already shown, the vibrations of any instrument are excited by resonance only ivlien vibrations of the same period are already present in the surrounding air. Accordingly, the only sound directly originated in each variation of our experiment, viz. that of the note '^^ o — , must have contained all the tones sue- IV. §45.] ANALYSIS OF RESONANCE. 91 cessively heard. The reader should apply the method of proof here adopted to notes in various regions of the key-board. He will find considerable differences, even between consecutive notes, in the number and relative intensities of the separate tones into which he is thus able to resolve them. The higher the pitch of the fundamental tone, the fewer will the recognisable associated tones become, until, in the region above ■7- — ■ — , the notes are themselves approximately single tones. The causes of these differences will be explained in detail in a subsequent chapter ; it is sufficient here to indicate their existence. The result arrived at, thus far, is that the sounds of the piano- forte are in general composite, the number of consti- tuent tones into which they are resolvable being largest in the lower half of the instrument, and dimi- nishing in its upper half, until at last they become practically tones of only one degree of pitch. 45, The above resolution has been effected by means of the principle of resonance. It can, however, be performed by the ear directly, though only to a small extent and with less ease. In applying the direct analysis to the sounds of a musical instrument, it is best first to produce gently the note correspond- ing in pitch to the tone which it is wished to isolate, and then to develope the compound sound contain- 92 DIRECT ANALYSIS. [IV. § 45. ing it as a constituent. In most cases, provided the observer has kept his attention unswervingly- fixed on the pitch of the note for which he is Ustening, he wUl hear it come out clearly from among the group of tones included in the composite sound. If the pianoforte note B^ ^_ be thus examined, the Octave, ^ , and Twelfth, ggi can generally be recognised with considerable ease ; the second Octave, ^ ) " , with a little trouble ; the next two tones of the series on page 89 with increasing difficulty, and those which succeed them not at all. The reader approaching the phenomenon for the first time must not be disappointed if, on tiying this experiment, he fail to hear the tones he is told to expect. He should vary its conditions by changing the note struck, in such a way that his attention will not be liable to be diverted by the presence of strong tones more acute than that of which he is in search. Thus a note near i may be advantageously chosen to observe the first Octave, ^ " — ; one near ^=i^=^ to observe the m Twelfth, ^ ; one near to observe the IV. § 46.] DIFFICULTY OF DIRECT ANALYSIS. 93 second Octave, H^ . He may however altogether fail in performing the analysis with the unassisted ear. This by no means indicates any aural defect, as he may at first be inclined to imagine. It rather shows that the life-long habit of regarding the notes of in- dividual sound-producing instruments as single tones cannot be unlearned all at once. The case is analo- gous to that of single vision with two eyes, where two distinct and different images are so blended together as to appear to all ordinary observation as one. The acoustical observer who is thus situated must at first rely on the analysis by resonance, and on the testi- mony of those who are able to perform the direct analysis. As he pursues the subject further experi- mentally, his analytical faculty will develope itself. 46. The composite character of musical sounds, which we have recognised in the case of the piano- forte, and shall have ample opportunity of verifying more generally in the sequel, requires the introduc- tion here of certain verbal definitions and limitations. The phraseology hitherto employed, both in the science of Acoustics and in the theory of Music, goes on the supposition that the sounds of individual instruments are single tones, and therefore, of course, contains no term specially denoting compound sounds and their constituents. ' Sound,' ' note,' and ' tone ' 94 THEORY OF QUALITY. [IV. § 47. are used as nearly synonymous. It will be conve- nient to restrict the meaning of the latter so that it shall denote a sound which does not admit of resolu- tion into simpler elements. A single sound of deter- minate pitch will accordingly, in what follows, be called a tone, or simple tone. For a compound sound the word clang will be a serviceable term. The series of elementary sounds into which a clang can be re- solved we shall call its partial-tones, sometimes dis- tinguishing among these, the lowest, or fundamental tone, from the others, or overtones, of the clang. This nomenclature is the direct adaptation of the German terms employed by Helmholtz. Its introduction is due to Professor Tyndall. 47. This long discussion has paved the way for the complete explanation of musical quality which is contained in the following proposition. The quality of a clang depends on the number, orders and rela- tive intensities of the partial-tones into ivhich it can he resolved. We have here three different causes to which variations in the quality of composite sounds are assigned. 1. A clang may contain only two or three, or it may contain half-a-dozen, or even as many as fifteen or twenty, well developed partial-tones. 2. The number of partial-tones pi'esent remain- ing the same, the quality of the resulting sound will IV. §47.] THEORY OF QUALITY. 95 vary according as they occupy different positions in the partial-tone series, i.e. on their orders. Thus, a clang containing three tones may consist of (1),(2), (3), or of (1), (3), (5), or of (l), (7), (10), and so on, the quality varying in each instance. 3. The number and orders of the partial-tones present remaining the same, the quality will vary according to the relative degrees of loudness with which those tones speak. Thus, in the simplest case of a clang consisting of only (l) and (2), either tone may alter in intensity while the other remains constant, and so cause variation in the quality of the sound resulting from their combination. It is clear that these three classes of variations are entirely independent of each other, that is to say, any two clangs may differ in the number, orders and relative intensities, of their constituent partial-tones. The variety of quality thus provided for is almost in- definitely great. In order to form some idea of its extent, let us see how^ many clangs of different qua- lity, but of the same pitch, can be formed with the first six partial-tones, by variations of number and order only. We will indicate each group by the corresponding figures inclosed in a bracket ; thus e.g. (1, 3, 5) represents a clang consisting of the first, third and fifth partial-tones. All the possible groups, each of course contain- 96 THEORY OF QUALITY. [IV. § 47. ing the same fundamental tone, are given in the following enumeration : — Two at a time : (1, 2), (1, 3), (1, 4), (1, 5), (1, 6). Total 5. Three at a time : (1, 2, 3), (1, 2, 4), (1, 2, 5), (1, 2, 6), {1, 3, 4), {1, 3, 5), (1, 3, 6), (1, 4, 5), (1, 4, 6), (1, 5, 6). Total 10. Four at a time : (1, 2, 3, 4), (1, 2, 3, 5), (1, 2, 3, 6), (1, 2, 4, 5), (1, 2, 4, 6), (1, 2, 5, 6), (1, 3, 4, 5), (1, 3, 4, 6), (1, 3, 5, 6), (1, 4, 5, 6). Total 10. Five at a time : (1, 2, 3. 4, 5), (1, 2, 3, 4, 6), (1, 2, 3, 5, 6), (1, 2, 4, 5, 6), (1, 3, 4, 5, 6). Total 5. Six at a time : (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6). Total 1. The whole number of groups is 31, or if we allow the fundamental -tone (l) to count by itself as a sound of separate quality, 32. Let us next examine how many clangs of different quality can be obtained from a single combination of three fixed partial-tones by variations of inteyisity only, supposing that each IV. § 47.] THEORY OF QUALITY. 97 tone is capable of but two degrees of loudness. Re- presenting one of these by / and the other by p, we indicate, e.g., by (/, p, p) a clang in which the funda- mental tone is sounded forte, and the two overtones piano. The different cases which present themselves are the following : (///), {f,p,n ipj,/). ip.pj), if,f,p), if'P'P)' {p>f'P)> {p,p,p) or seven in all, since {p, p, p) has the same quaHty as if,/,/). The number of cases increases very rapidly as we take more partial-tones together. Thus a clang of four tones will produce 15 sounds of different quality ; one of five tones 3 1 ; one of six tones 63 ; by variations of intensity only. Alto- gether we could form, with six partial-tones, each susceptible of only two different degrees of intensity, upwards ot four hundred clangs of distinct quality, all having the same fundamental tone. The suppo- sition above made utterly understates, however, the possible variety of quality dependent only on changes of relative intensity. A very slight increase or dimi- nution of loudness, on the part of a single constituent tone, is enough to produce a sensible change of qua- lity in the clang. We should be still far below the mark if we allowed each partial-tone four different degrees of intensity, though even this supposition would bring us more than eight thousand separate T. 7 98 THEORY OF QUALITY. [IV. § 47. cases. Since many more variations of intensity are practically efficacious, and also since the disposable partial-tones need by no means be limited to the first six, the above calculation will probably suffice to convince the reader that the varieties of quality which the theory we are engaged upon is capable of accounting for are almost indefinitely numerous. This is, in fact, no more than we have a right to demand of the theory, when we reflect on the fine shades of quality which the ear is able to distinguish. No two instruments of the same class are exactly alike in this respect. For instance, grand pianofortes by Broadwood and by Erard exhibit unmistakable differences, which we describe as ' Broadwood tone ' and ' Erard tone.' Less marked, but stiU perfectly recognisable, diiferences exist between individual in- struments of the same class and maker, and even between consecutive notes of the same instrument. To these we have to add the variations in quahty due to the manner in which the performer handles his instrument. On the pianoforte the kinds of tone elicited by a dull slamming touch, and by a lively elastic one, are clearly distinguishable. With other instruments such distinctions are much more marked. On the violin we perceive endless gradations of quality, from the rasping scrape of the beginner up to the smooth and superb tone of a Joachim. A IV. § 47.] THEORY OF QUALITY. 99 precisely similar remark applies to wind instruments ; the differences, for example, between first-rate and inferior playing on the hautbois, bassoon, horn, or trumpet, being perfectly obvious to every musical ear. In the next chapter we will discuss the quality and essential mechanism of the principal musical in- struments, among which the pianoforte will receive an amount of attention proportionate to its popu- larity and general use. We begin with the simple tones of which all composite sounds are made up. 7—2 CHAPTER V. ON THE ESSENTIAL MECHANISM OF THE PEINCIPAL MUSICAL INSTEUMENTS CONSIDEEED IN EEFEEENCE TO QUALITY. 1. Sounds of tuning-forks. 48. Wlien a vibrating tuning-fork is held to the ear, we at once perceive, beside the proper note of the fork, a shrill, ringing and usually rather dis- cordant, sound due to overtones which do not follow the series of such tones set out in § 43. If, how- ever, the fork is mounted on its resonance-box, as in Fig. 24, p. 85, its proper note is so much strength- ened that the extraneous sound is by comparison insignificant. Provided the fork be but moderately excited, e.g. by gentle stroking with a resined bow, the sound heard is practically' a simple tone. It is characterised by extreme mildness, without a trace ' Except with very gentle bowing, however, or unless the fork's note has been allowed to grow faint, examination with resonators shows traces of overtones belonging to the ordinary series. V. § 49.] SIMPLE TONES. 101 of anything which could be called harsh or piercing. As compared with a pianoforte note of the same pitch the fork-tone is wanting in richness and viva- city, and produces an impression of greater depth, so that one is at first inclined to think the fork em- ployed must be an Octave too low. It is a direct inference from the general theory of the nature of quality that simple tones can differ only in pitch and intensity. Accordingly we find that tuning-forks of the same pitch, mounted on resonance-boxes and set gently vibrating by a resined bow, exhibit, whatever be their forms and sizes, differences of loudness only. When made to sound with equal intensity by suitable bowing, their tones are absolutely undistinguishable from each other. 2. Sounds of vibrating strings. 49. Sounding strings vibrate so rapidly that their movements cannot be followed directly by the eye. It will be well, therefore, to examine how the slower and more easily controllable vibrations of non-sounding strings are performed, before treating the proper subject of this section. Take a flexible caoutchouc tube ten or fifteen feet long and fasten its ends to two fixed objects separated from each other by that distance. The tube can be conveniently set in periodic vibration by impressing a swaying 102 FORMS OF VIBRATION. [V. § 49. movement upon it with the hand near either ex- tremity, in suitable time. According to the rapidity of the motion thus communicated, the tube wUl take up different forms of vibration. The simplest of these is shown in Fig. 26. A and B being its fixed ex- tremities, the tube vibrates as a whole between the two extreme positions AaB and AbB. Fig. 36 The tube may also vibrate in the form shown in Fig. 27, where AahB and AcdB are its extreme positions. Fig.2.1 In this instance the middle point of the tube, C, remains at rest, the loops on either side of it moving independently, as though the tube were fastened at C as well as at A and B. For this reason the point C is called a node, from the Latin nodus, a knot. The loops AC and CB are termed ventral segments. Fig. 28 shows a form of vibration with two nodes, at C and D, dividing the distance AB into three V. § 50.] DIRECT AND REFLECTED PULSES. 103 equal ventral segments. We may also obtain forms with three, four, five, &c., nodes, dividing the tube into four, five, six, &c., equal ventral segments, re- FigS.8. spectively. The stifihess of very short portions of the tube alone imposes a limit on the subdividing process. Let us examine the mechanical causes to which these effects are due. 50. If we unfasten one end of the tube, and, holding it in the hand as in Fig. 29, raise a hump upon it, by suddenly jerking the hand transversely F1^. ZO ■ i«a to it through a small distance, the hump will run along the tube until it reaches its fixed extremity B ; it will then be reflected and run back to A, where it will undergo a second reflexion, and so on. At each reflexion the hump will have its convexity 7-eversed. Thus, if while travelling from A towards B its form was that of a, Fig. 30, on its way back it will have 104 DIRECT AND REFLECTED PULSES. [V. § 50. the form h. After reflexion at A, it wUl resume its first form a, and so on. Now, instead of a single jerk, let the hand holding the free end execute a series of equal continuous trans vei'se vibrations. Each complete vibration will cause a wave, ah Fig. 31, to Fig. 31. pass along the tube from A to B, where reflexion wUl reverse the protuberances, so that the wave wiU re- turn from B to A stern foremost. Next let the tube be again fastened at both ends, as before, and the vibrations of the hand impressed at some intermedi- ate point, as C, Fig. 32. □ i < - — c ^jT-iT ^^m Two sets of waves will now start from C in the directions of the arrows. They will be reflected at A and B, and then their effects wiU intermingle. Let us suppose that the tube has been set in steady motion and, on the removal of the hand, continues its vibra- tions without any external force acting on it. Two sets of equal waves are now moving with equal velocities from A towards B and from B towards A, and we have to determine the motion of the tube under their joint action. V. § 51.] FORMATION OF NODES. 105 Suppose that a crest' a, Fig. 33, moving from A towards B, meets an equal trough' h, moving from B towards A, at the point c. An undisturbed particle of the tube situated at this point is solicited by equal forces in opposite directions, and therefore remains at rest. The two equal and opposite pulses then proceed to cross each other, but, as a moves to the right and 6 to the left with equal speed, there is nothing to give either of them at the point c an influence superior to that exerted in the contrary direction by the other. The particle at c therefore remains at rest under their joint influence, i.e. a node is formed at that point. If a trough had been moving from A to- wards B, and an equal crest from B towards A, the result would clearly have been the same : hence A node is formed at every point where two equal and opposite pulses, a crest and a trough, meet each other. 51. The annexed figure represents two series of equal waves advancing in opposite directions with equal velocities. The moment chosen is that at ' Provided that confusion with water-waves be explicitly guarded against, there is no objection to retaining this convenient phraseology for distinguishing between opposite protuberances. 106 FORMATION OF NODES. [V. § 51. which crest coincides with crest and trough with trough. The joint effect thus produced does not appear in the figure, our object at present being merely to determine the number and positions of the resulting nodes. For the sake of clearness, one set of waves is represented slightly below the other, though in fact the two are strictly coincident. Let the waves ahdf...z be moving from left to right, the waves z'ts'q'... a' frora right to left. The crest klm meets the trough pn'm at m. After these have crossed each other, the trough ghk and the crest rq'p will also meet at m, since km and pm are equal distances. Similarly the crest efg and the trough ts'r will meet at m. Accordingly the point in is a node, and, by exactly the same reasoning, so are a, c, e, g, h, p, r, t, &c. The distances between pairs of consecutive nodes are all equal, each being a single pulse-length, i.e. half a wave-length, of either series. Two pulse-lengths, as gk and km, give three nodes g, k, and m ; three pulse-lengths four nodes, and so on. There is thus always one node more than the number of pulses. On the other hand, the fixed ends of the tube, which are the origins of the systems of V. §52.] NATURE OF SEGMENTAL VIBRATION. 107 reflected waves, occupy two of these nodes. Deduct- ing them we arrive at this result : — The number of nodes formed is one less than the number of the pulse-lengths {or half wave-lengths), ivhich together make up the length of the vibrating tube. 52. We will now ascertain how the portions of the tube between consecutive nodes move while the Tig. 35. (O (0 (0 « CO two systems of waves are simultaneously passing along it. Let A and B, Fig. 35, be the fixed ends, as before, and let us take five nodes at the points 1,2, 3, 4, 5. In (1), the systems of waves coincide, ac- cordingly each point of the tube is displaced through 108 RATE OF SEGMENTAL VIBRATION. [V. § 52. twice as great a distance as if it had been acted on by only one system. The tube thus takes the form indicated by the strong line in the figure. In (2), one set of waves has moved half a pulse-length to the right, and the other the same distance to the left. The two systems are now in complete antagonism, the displacements being equal in amount and opposite in direction at every point. The tube is therefore momentarily in its undisturbed position. In (3), each system has moved through a pulse-length, and the maximum displacements are again produced on the tube, but in opposite directions to those of (1). In (4), where the systems have moved through a pulse- length-and-a-half, the tube passes again through its undisturbed position, and, in (5), regains that which it occupied in (l), the systems of waves, meanwhile, having each traversed two pulse-lengths, or one wave- length \ Thus the tube executes one complete vibra- tion in the time occupied by a pulse in passing along a length of the tube equal to tivice one of its own ventral segments. In other words, the tube's rate of vibration varies as the number of segments into ivhich it is divided. It moves most slowly in the form shown in Fig. 26 with but a single segment ; twice ' The reader will find that Fig. 35 is rendered more readily intelligible by drawing the two systems of waves in difierent odours, and the successive positions of the tube in black. V. § 53.] RATE OF SEGMENTAL VIBRATION. 109 as fast in that of Fig. 27, where it is divided into two segments ; three times as fast with three segments, and so on. It is easy to confirm this by direct ex- periment, the swaying movement of the hand on the tube needing to be twice as rapid for a form of vibration with two segments as for a form with one, and so on. 53. Instead of comparing the difierent rates at which the same tube vibrates when divided into different numbers of ventral segments, we may com- pare the rates of vibration of tubes of different lengths divided into the same number of segments. Let us take as an example the two tubes AB, CD, Fig. 36, each divided by three nodes into four Ai ! 1 1— .3 ''' 1 1 i 'o ventral segments. By what has been already shown, the time of vibration of either tube will be that which a pulse occupies in traversing two of its ven- tral segments. Therefore the time of vibration of AB will be to that of CD as ^2 is to C2, i.e. as one half of AB is to one half of CD, or as AB is to CD. This reasoning is equally applicable to any other case. Accordingly we have the general result that, when tubes of different lengths are divided into the 110 MOTION OF A SOUNDING STRING. [V. § 54. same number of ventral segments, their times of vibration are proportional to the lengths of the tubes, or, which comes to the same thing, their rates of vibration are inversely proportional to their lengths. The reader should observe that it has been through- out this discussion assumed that the material, thick- ness and tension of the tube, or tubes, in question were subject to no variation whatever. Any changes in these would correspondingly affect the rates of vibration produced, but according to less simple laws than variation in length only. 54. We are now prepared to examine the motion of a sounding string. Its ends are fastened to fixed points of attachment and the string is excited at some intermediate point, by plucking it with the finger, as in the harp and guitar, by striking it with a soft hammer, as in the pianoforte, or by stroking it with a resined bow, as in the violin and other instru- ments of the same class. The impulses thus set up are reflected at the extremities of the string (in the violin at the bridge and at the finger of the per- former) and behave towards each other exactly as in the case of the vibrating tube considered above. The results there obtained are, accordingly, at once applicable to the case before us. The string may vibrate in a single segment as in Fig. 26. This is the form of slowest vibration with a string of given V. §54.J POINT OF PERCUSSION. ill length, material and tension. Accordingly, when thus vibrating, the string produces the deepest note of which, all other conditions remaining the same, it is capable. The string may also vibrate in the forms shown in Figs. 27, 28, 35, 36, or in forms with larger numbers of segments. The rapidity of vibration in any one of these forms is, as we have seen [§ 52], proportional to the number of segments formed, so that, with two segments, the string vibrates twice, with three, thrice, with four, four times, as fast as in the form with one segment. It follows hence [§ 43] that the notes obtained by causing a string to vibrate successively in forms with 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, &c., segments are all partial- tones of one compound sound, the lowest being of course its fundamental-tone. The modes of eliciting the sounds of stringed instruments described on the preceding page are not capable of setting up any one of the above forms of vibration by itself, but give rise to a movement which is the resultant of several such vibration- forms compounded together. Each separate vibra- tion-form thus called into existence sings, as it were, its own note, without heeding what is being done by its fellows. Accordingly, a certain number of tones belonging to one family of partial-tones are simultaneously heard. 112 MODE OF PERCUSSION. [V. § 54. How many, and which, members of the general series of partial-tones are present, and with what relative intensities, in the sound of a string set vibrating by a blow, depends on the position of the point at which the blow is delivered, on the nature of the striking object, and on the material of the string. It is clear that a node can never be formed at the point of percussion. Therefore no partial-tone requiring for its production a node in that place can exist in the resulting sound. If, for instance, we excite the string exactly at its middle point, the forms of vibration with an even number of ventral segments, all of which have a node at the centre of the string, are excluded, and only the odd partial-tones, i.e. the 1st, 3rd, 5th, and so on, are heard. In this manner we can always prevent the formation of any assigned partial-tone, by choosing a suitable point of percussion. On the other hand, a vibration-form is in the most favourable position for development when the middle 'point of one of its ventral segments coincides with the point of per- cussion. The more nearly it occupies this position the louder will be the corresponding partial-tone ; while the further it recedes from this position towards that in which one of its nodes falls on the point of percussion, the weaker will the partial-tone become. The form and material of the hammer, or other V. §55.] PITCH OF STRING-SOUNDS. 113 object with which the string is struck, have also a great influence in modifying the quahty of the sound produced. Sharpness of edge and hardness of sub- stance tend to develope high and powerful over- tones, a rounded form and soft elastic substance to strengthen the fundamental-tone. The material of the string itself produces its effect chiefly by limiting the number of partial-tones. Its stiffness resists division into very short segments, and this implies, for every string, a fixed limit beyond which further subdivision ceases and where, therefore, the series of overtones is cut off. Hence very thin mobile strings are favourable, thick weighty strings un- favourable, to the production of a large number of partial-tones. 55. Having examined what determines the quality of the sound of a vibrating string, we have next to enquire on what its pitch depends. This term is indeed, strictly speaking, inappropriate to a compo- site sound containing a series of different tones each having its own vibration-number and independent position in the musical scale. If, however, we use the phrase ' pitch of a sound' as equivalent to ' pitch of the fundamental-tone of the sound,' we shall avoid any confusion arising from this circumstance. The pitch of a string-sound depends, of course, on the rate at which the string is vibrating. We have seen T. 8 114 LENGTHS AND INTERVALS. [V. § 55. that when the material, thickness and tension of a string remain the same, its rate of vibration varies inversely as the length of the string. Accordingly, the vibration-number of a string-sound varies inversely as the length of the string. It follows hence that the relations which connect the vibration-numbers of sounds forming given intervals with each other, hold equally for the lengths of the strings by which those sounds are produced. To verify this by experiment we have only to stretch a wire between two fixed points A and B, Fig. 36 bis, and divide it into two segments by applying a bridge to it at some inter- mediate point C. If AC bears to CB any one of the simple numerical ratios exhibited in the table on Fig. 36 {bis.) I 1 B A C p. 71, we obtain the corresponding interval there given by alternately exciting the vibrations of the two segments at any pair of points in -4 C and CB respectively. Thus, if CB is twice as long as AC, the sound produced by the former wUl be one Octave lower than that produced by the latter. If A C is to CB in the proportion of 2 to 3, AC's sound will be a Fifth above CB's ; and similarly in other cases. It was by experiments of this kind that the ancient Greek philosopher Pythagoras is said to have made V. §55.] LENGTHS AND INTERVALS. 115 out the existence of a connexion between certain musical intervals and the ratios of certain small integers. Thus an Octave- vfas produced by a string divided into two parts in the proportion of 2 to 1 ; a Fifth was obtained by division in the proportion of 3 to 2, and so forth. The relations existing between these lengths and the vibration-numbers of the notes produced by them were, however, entirely unknown to Pythagoras and his contemporaries ; indeed it was not until the seventeenth century that they were discovered, by Galileo. In instruments of the violin class, the pitch of the notes varies according to the position of the finger on the vibrating string. The length of string intercepted between the fixed bridge and the finger admits of being altered at pleasure, and thus every shade of pitch can be produced from such instruments. The resined bow maintains the vibration of the string by alternately dragging it out of its position of rest, letting it fly back again, catching it once more, and so on. The hollow cavity of the instrument rein- forces the string-sound by resonance. The quality of instruments of the violin class is vivacious and piercing. The first eight partial-tones are well represented in their sounds. 8—2 116 THE PIANOFORTE. ["V. § 56. The Pianoforte. 56. In this instrument each wire is stretched between two pegs, which are fixed into a flat plate of wood called the sound-board. The string is fast- ened to one peg, and coiled round the other, which admits of being turned about its own axis by means of a key of suitable construction. This allows the string to be accurately tuned, since by tightening or loosening the wire we raise or lower its pitch at pleasure. In small instruments two, in larger ones three, wires in unison with each other usually correspond to each note of the key-board. While the pianoforte is not in action a series of small pieces of wood covered with list, called ' dampers,' rest upon the wires. These are connected with the key-board in such a manner that, when a note is pressed down, the corresponding damper rises from its place, and the wires it previously covered remain free until the note is allowed to spring up again, when the damper immediately sinks back into its original position. Each note is connected with an elastic hammer, which deals a blow to its own set of wires and then springs back from them. The wires thus set in motion continue to vibrate until either the sound gradually dies away, or is abruptly ex- tinguished by the descent of the damper. The V. § 56.] ACTION OF PEDALS. 117 action of the two pedals is as follows : the soft pedal shifts the hammers in such a way that each hammer only acts on one, or on two, of the wires cor- responding to it, instead of on its complete set of two, or of three, wires. The sound produced by striking a note is therefore proportionately weakened. The loud pedal lifts all the dampers off the wires at once. It thus not only allows notes to continue sounding after the fingers of the player have quitted them, but places wires other than those actually struck in a position to sound by resonance. The number of wires thus brought into play by striking a single note of the instrument will be easily seen to be con- siderable. Suppose, first, that a simple tone, e.g. that of a tuning-fork, is sounding near the wires of a pianoforte with the loud pedal down, its pitch being that of middle C, ^ ^ ; the wires of the corre- sponding note will of course resonate with it, vibrat- ing in the simplest form with only one ventral seg- ment. The wires of the note ^zzscrz , one Octave below it, are also capable of producing middle C when they vibrate in the form with two segments. So are those of ^ , a Twelfth below it, when m vibrating with three segments ; those of ;::;3;:^:^ , two 118 QUALITY OF PIANOFORTE SOUNDS. [V. § 56. Octaves below it, vibrating with four segments, and so on. Proceeding in this way, we determine a series of notes on the key-board of the pianoforte the wires of which are able to produce a simple tone of the pitch of middle C They obviously follow the same law as the harmonic overtones of a compound sound with middle C for its fundamental-tone, except that the successive intervals are reckoned downwards instead of upwards. The wires of all these notes will reinforce the tone of the tuning-fork by reso- nance. If now we remove the fork and strike middle C on the pianoforte itself we obtain of course a sound consisting of a number of simple tones. To each of these latter there corresponds a descending series of notes on the key-board determined in the manner just explained. A full chord struck in the middle region of the instrument will, therefore, command the more or less active services of many more wires than have been set vibrating by direct percussion. The increase of loudness thus secured is not very considerable, the effect being rather a heightened richness, like that of a mass of voices singing pianissimo. The sustaining power of the loud pedal renders care in its employment essential. It should, as a general rule, be held down only so long as notes belonging to one and the same chord are struck. Whenever a change of harmony occurs, the V. § 56.] ACTION OF SOUND-BOARD. 119 pedal should be allowed to rise, in order that the descent of the dampers may at once extinguish the preceding chord. If this precaution is neglected, perfectly irreconcilable chords become promiscuously jumbled together, and a series of jarring discords ensue which are nearly as distressing to the ear as the actual striking of wrong notes. The quality of pianoforte notes varies greatly in different parts of the scale. In the lower and middle region it is full and rich, the first six partial-tones being audibly present, though, 4, 5, 6 are much weaker than 1, 2, 3. Towards the upper part of the instrument the higher partial-tones disappear, until in the uppermost Octave the notes are approximately simple-tones, which accounts for their tame and uninteresting character. The pianoforte shares with all instruments of fixed sounds certain serious defects which will be discussed in detail in a subsequent chapter. When a vibrating wire is passing through its undisturbed position, its tension is necessarily some- what less than at any other moment, since, in order to assume the curved segmental form, it must be a little elongated, which involves a corresponding in- crease of tension. Hence the two pegs by which the ends of a wire are attached to the sound-board are submitted to an additional strain twice during each complete segmental vibration. The sound-board. 120 SOUNDS OF OBQAN-PIPES. [V. § 57. being purposely constructed of the most elastic wood, yields to the rhythmic impulses acting upon it, and is thrown into segmental vibrations coincident in period with those of the wire. These vibrations are communicated to the air in contact with the sound-board, and then transmitted further in the ordinary way. The amount of surface which a wire presents to the air is so small that, but for the aid of the sound- board, its vibrations would hardly excite an audible sound. The reader will not fail to notice that the sound-board of the pianoforte plays the same part as the hollow cavity of the viohn, and is in fact a solid resonator. In the harp, the framework of the instrument serves the same purpose. We have, in this combination of a vibration-exciting apparatus with a resonator, the type of construction adopted in nearly all musical instruments. 3. Sounds of organ-pipes. 57. It has been shown [§51] that, when two series of equal waves due to transverse vibrations travel along a stretched wire in opposite directions, nodes are formed at equal distances along it, sepa- rated by ventral segments of equal lengths. Let us now suppose that two series of equal waves due to longitudinal vibrations are traversing, in opposite directions, a column of air contained in a tube of V. §58.] 'STOPPED' AND 'OPEN' ORGAN-PIPES. 121 uniform bore. Each set of such waves has its own associated wave-form [§ 18, p. 39]. We have only, therefore, to consider the curves drawn in Fig. 35 as associated waves corresponding to longitudinal air- vibrations, in order to make the conclusions of § 52 at once applicable to the case in hand. The result is a series of equidistant nodes, or points of per- manent rest, distributed along the air-column. The intervening portions of air vibrate longitudinally at the same rate as the corresponding ventral segments of Fig. 35. We have here, as in the case of the sounding wire, all the conditions for the production of a musical note of pitch correspond- ing to the rapidity of vibration obtained. It only remains to show that, in the case of every organ- pipe, two sets of equal waves traverse in opposite directions the air-column which it contains. Organ-pipes are of two kinds, called respectively * stopped ' and ' open,' — terms which, however, apply only to one end of the pipe ; the other is in both kinds open. To begin with first variety : c ^37 58. Let AB, Fig. 37; be the closed end of a 122 REFLEXION AT A CLOSED ORIFICE. [V. § 58. stopped pipe, and let a series of pulses of condensa- tion and rarefaction be passing along the air within it in the direction shown by the arrow. First, let a pulse of condensation, CABD, have just reached AB. By supposition, the air in CABD is denser, and there- fore at a higher pressure, than that behind it. It win therefore expand. Forward motion being barred by AB, the expansion must take place entirely in the opposite direction. Hence the pulse of conden- sation is reflected at the end of the pipe, and pro- ceeds to describe its previous course in the reverse direction. Next, suppose CABD to be a pulse of rarefaction. The air in it is at a less pressure than that of the air behind it. As there is a fixed obstacle in front, this rarefaction can only be filled up from behind. The condensed air behind will expand more during the process, and become itself more rarefied, than if there had been no obstacle. A rarefied pulse will therefore return along the pipe. Thus a pulse of rarefaction, equally with one of condensation, is reflected at the closed end of the pipe. Neither pulse suffers any change other than reversal of its direction of motion. Since every pulse is thus regularly reflected at AB, and made to travel back unchanged along the pipe, it follows that a system of equal waves advancing in the direction of the arrow are necessarily met by an V. § 58.] REFLEXION AT AN OPEN ORIFIGE. 123 exactly equal system proceeding in the opposite direction. For stopped pipes, therefore, the point required to be proved is made out. Let AB, Fig. 38, be one end of an open pipe along which condensed and rarefied pulses are being alternately transmitted in the direction of the arrow. First, let CABD be a pulse of condensation which has just reached AB. If the pipe had continued to the left beyond AB, the particles of air near AB C FigSZ Af B' D would have moved in the direction of the arrow tUl they were stopped by the increasing pressure in their front. This would correspond to the instant of greatest density at AB. They would then begin to move in the direction opposite to that of the arrow, the pressure in front, i.e. on the left in the figure, being greater than that behind. As, how- ever, the pipe is open and the air free to move in all directions, the condensation just outside its mouth is never great : the particles will move further in the direction of the arrow before being stopped than they would do in the case of a continuous tube, and will, therefore, leave behind them a region in which the air is more rarefied than it would have 124 REFLEXION AT AN OPEN ORIFICE. [V. § 58. been at the same time in the corresponding part of the continuous tube. This region of rarefied air will originate a pulse of rarefaction, which will be transmitted in the direction opposite to that of the arrow. Conversely, when a pulse of rarefaction reaches the open end of the pipe, the rarefaction just within its mouth cannot become great, since the air is free to enter from all sides. The particles behind, which in the case of a continuous tube would move in the direction of the arrow and diminish the rarefaction, wdl therefore be sooner stopped : the pressure and density will be greater than at the corresponding place and time in the continuous tube : and thus a pulse of condensation will be produced and transmitted back along the pipe. To look at the matter from a somewhat different point of view, the motion of the air in the open pipe wUl be practically the same as it would have been if the pipe had formed part of a continuous tube, but air had been removed at the point corresponding to the opening whenever a pulse of condensation arrived there, and air had been injected at the same point whenever a pulse of rarefaction reached it. By the above reasoning^ which the student should ' I am indebted for this popular explanation of reflexion at an open orifice to the late Mr Coutts Trotter, formerly Senior Fellow of Trinity College, Cambridge. V. § 59.] YIBEATION-FORMS FOR A STOPPED PIPE. 125 carefully compare with that of § 21, it is clear that reflexion takes place at an open as well as at a closed end of a pipe : with this important difference, how- ever, that, in the case of the open orifice, condensation is turned into rarefaction and rarefaction into con- densation, so that the wave returns hind part fore- most. 59. We will next examine what forms of seg- mental vibration the air in a stopped pipe can adopt. Every such form must necessarily have a node coin- cident with the closed end of the pipe, since no longi- tudinal vibrations are possible there. The impulses constituting the series of direct waves are not, as we shall see presently, originated, like those of a piano- forte string, at some intermediate point, but enter the pipe at its open end. This must therefore be a point of maximum vibration. Now a glance at Fig. 35 shows that the maxima of vibration are at the middle points of the ventral segments. Hence the centre of a segment must coincide with the open end of the pipe. The above considerations suffice to solve the problem before us. If the closed end of the pipe is placed at A, Fig. 35(1), the open end must be midway between^ and 1, or between 1 and 2, 2 and 3, 3 and 4, and so on. No other form of vibration is possible. Fig. 39 shows the air in a stopped pipe of 126 VIBRATION-FORMS FOR A STOPPED PIPE. [V. § 59. given length vibrating in four such ways. The vertical lines indicate the positions of the nodes. For the sake of greater clearness, the loops of the associated vibration-forms are in each case drawn in dotted lines. (a) (b) (0 (°) In [A) we have half a segment ; in {B) a segment and a half; in (C) two segments and a half; in {D) three segments and a half The numbers of seg- ments into which the air-column is divided in these four cases are, therefore, proportional to i, li, 2^, 3i, i.e. to I, f, I, f, or to the whole numbers 1, 3, 5, 7. Now by § 53 it appears that the rate of vibration V. § 60.] VIBRA T ION-FORMS FOR AN OPEN PIPE. 1 27 in any form varies as the number of segments which it contains. The vibration-numbers of the sounds produced in the present instance are, therefore, proportional to 1, 3, 5, 7, respectively, i.e. we get the first four odd partial-tones of a sound of which {A) gives us the fundamental-tone [§ 43]. The reasoning here adopted evidently applies equally well to cases in which the air-column is subdivided to any assigned extent. It follows, therefore, that the notes obtain- able from a stopped pipe are all odd partial-tones belonging to one and the same clang. 60. The case of the open pipe shall next be investigated. Here, as in the previous case, the centre of a ventral segment must coincide with the end of the pipe at which the direct pulses enter. The considerations alleged in § 58 indicate that the same thing must also hold good at the opposite orifice'. Referring once more to Fig. 35 (1), we obtain all the possible modes of vibration which satisfy both the above conditions by placing one end of the pipe mid- way between A and 1, and the other successively half way between 1 and 2, 2 and 3, 3 and 4, and so on. ' I here assume, for the sake of simplicity, what is not rigorously true, viz. that reflexion at an open end of a pipe is as complete as at a stopped end. It is however approximately true for the pipes employed for musical purposes, whose transverse dimension is small compared to their length. ] 28 VIBRATION-FORMS FOR AN OPEN PIPE. [V. § 60. The first four of the cases thus obtained, for a tube of constant length, are shown in the next figure, which is drawn on precisely the same plan as Fig. 39. In each case, the two half-segments at the ends of the pipe make up one whole segment. The num- bers of segments into which the air-column is divided are, therefore, in {A), 1; in [B), 2; in (C), 3; in (Z)), 4. The same law would obviously hold for higher subdivisions. Hence, in the case of an open pipe, the rates of all the possible modes of segmental (.A) Fig.40. (P) (O (^) V vibration are as the numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, &c. The notes obtainable from such a pipe are, therefore, the complete series of partial-tones belonging to one and the same clang. V. §§ 61, 62.] PITCH AND LENGTH OF PIPE. 129 61. If the slowest forms of vibration, shown at {A) in Figs. 39 and 40, are compared with each other, it will be at once seen that the ventral segment in Fig. 39 is exactly twice as long as that in Fig. 40. Hence, the gravest tone obtainable from a stopped pipe is always one Octave lower than the gravest tone producible from an open pipe of the same length. It has been shown in § 39 that this result of theory is borne out by experiment. 62. In order to complete this investigation, it is necessary to determine the pitch of the lowest note which a pipe of given length is capable of uttering. By § 52 we know that a complete segmental vibra- tion is performed during the time occupied by a pulse in traversing twice the length of a single seg- ment. In [A) Fig. 39, this is equal to four times the length of the tube. The velocity of the pulse is here the velocity of Sound in air, which, under ordinary conditions of temperature, &c., we may put at 1125 feet per second'. The vibration-number of a stopped pipe's lowest tone is therefore found by dividing 1125 by four times the length of the pipe expressed in feet. Conversely, the length of a stopped pipe which is to have as its deepest tone a note of given pitch, is found by dividing 1125 by four times the ' Tyndall's Sovmd, p. 23. 130 PITCH AND LENGTH OF PIPE. [V. §62. vibration-number of the note to be produced. The quotient gives the required length in , feet. For example, middle C of the pianoforte makes 264 vibrations per second. The required length in this case would be expressed by , which is rather more than 1 ft. -^in., i.e. roughly speaking, one foot. An open pipe, to produce the same note, would there- fore have to be two feet in length. It has been just shown that the vibration-number of the lowest tone producible, either from a stopped or an open pipe, varies inversely as the length of the pipe. Hence the relations established in § 55 for strings hold also for columns of air contained in pipes. The case of the pipe-sounds is, however, somewhat simpler than that of the string-sounds, since the pitch of the latter depends on the tension of the strings as well as on their lengths, whereas, in the former, pitch depends, under given atmospheric con- ditions, on length alone. Hence we may define a note of assigned pitch by merely stating the length of the stopped, or open, pipe whose fundamental tone it is. The open pipe is commonly preferred for this purpose, and accordingly organ builders call middle C ' 2-foot tone ;' the Octave below it ' 4-foot tone,' and so on. The lowest C on modern piano- fortes is ' 1 6 -foot tone ;' that one Octave lower, which V. § 63.] PITCH AND LENGTH OP PIPE. 131 is found only on the very largest organs, * 32-foot tone.' The highest note of the pianoforte, usually A, would be about ' 2 -inch tone.' 63. The reader should observe that, in the course of this discussion, we have incidentally obtained a more complete theory of resonance than could be given in chapter iii. When a tuning-fork is held at the orifice of a tube, the strongest resonance will be produced if the note of the fork coincides with the fundamental tone of the tube. A decided, though less powerful, resonance ought also to ensue if the fork-note coincides with one of the higher tones of the tube, which, as we know, are all overtones of its fundamental. A resonance-box is only a stopped' pipe under another name. We may therefore eraploy it to test the truth of our result, that the only tones obtainable from a stopped pipe are the odd partial- tones of a clang of which the first is the fundamental tone. I possess a series of forks giving the first seven partial -tones of a clang. When I strike 1, 3, 5 or 7, and hold them before the open end of the stopped resonance-box corresponding to 1, a decided rein- forcement of their tones is heard. If I do the same with 2, 4, or 6, hardly any resonance is pro- ' For forks of high pitch it is, however, found best to use resonance-boxes open, at both ends, and therefore corresponding to open pipes. 9—2 132 CONSTRUCTION OF FLUE-PIPES. [V. §§ 64, 65. duced. Thus our theoretical result is experimentally- verified. 64. Organ-pipes are divided into two classes according as their sounds are originated : (1) by blowing against a sharp edge, (2) by blowing against an elastic tongue. Those of the first class are called _/?Me-pipes ; those of the second class reecZ-pipes. We will consider each class by itself. 65. Flue-pipes. Here the wind is driven through a narrow slit against a sharp edge placed exactly opposite to it, in the manner shown in Fig. 41, which represents a vertical section of a portion of the pipe near the end at which its sound originates. FyAl. The air is forced by the bellows through the tube V. § 65.] QUALITY OF SOUNDS OF FLUE-PIPES. 133 ab into the chamber c, and escapes through the slit d, thus impinging against the edge e, and exciting in the air-column above segmental vibrations whose period is controlled by the length of the pipe. The regular musical note thus produced is ac- companied by a hissing sound, which may be imitated by blowing with the mouth against a knife-edge held in front of it. This sound however contributes nothing to the general effect, being, where a pipe is properly constructed, inaudible except in its im- mediate neighbourhood. Stopped wooden flue-pipes of large aperture, blown by only a light pressure of wind, produce sounds which are nearly simple tones ; only a trace of partial tone No. 3 being perceptible. Such tones, like the fork-tones with which they are in fact almost identical, sound sweet and mild, but also tame and spiritless. A greater pressure of wind developes 3 distinctly in addition to 1, and, if it becomes excessive, may spoil the quality by giving the overtone too great an intensity compared to that of the fundamental, or may even extinguish the latter altogether, and so cause the whole sound to jump up an Octave and a Fifth. This result may easily be obtained by blowing with the mouth into a small 6-inch stopped pipe, which can readily be pro- cured at any organ factory. 134 'REED'-PIPES. [V. §66. Stopped pipes of narrow aperture develope the third and fifth partial-tones with distinctness. In the case of an open pipe the fundamental- tone is never produced by itself According to the dimen- sions of the pipe, and the pressure of wind, it is accompanied by from two to five overtones. Open flue-pipes present, therefore, various degrees of quality which are exhibited in different 'stops' of a large organ. 66. Reed-pipes. The apparatus by which the sounds of pipes of this class are originated is the following. One end of a thin narrow strip of elastic metal, called a 'tongue,' is fastened to a brass plate, while the other end is free. A rect- angular aperture, very slightly larger than the tongue, is cut through the plate, so as to allow the tongue to swing into and out of the aperture, like a door with double hinges, without touching the edges of the aperture as it passes them. The accom- panying figure shows this piece of mechanism, which is called a ' reed,' in its position of rest. FisiS It is set in motion by a current of air being driven against the free end of the tongue, which is thus V. § 66.] MECHANISM OF A REED. 135 made to swing between limiting positions as shown in the annexed sections. Tig.4,3 . When the tongue occupies any position inter- mediate between that of {A) and its position of equi- librium, air is blown through the aperture in the direction indicated by the arrows in {A). From the moment that the tongue passes through its equili- brium-position towards that shown in {B), the current of air is barred by the accuracy with which the tongue fits into the aperture beneath it. Only when the tongue again emerges can the air resume its passage. A series of equal impulses of air are thus produced at equal intervals of time, and the instrument, there- fore, gives rise to a regular musical sound. Its clang is highly composite, containing distinctly recognisable partial-tones up to the 16th or 20th of the series. Thus a reed does not need to be associated with a resonating column in order to produce a musical sound ; in fact the instrument called the harmonium consists of reeds without such adjuncts. The quality of an independent reed is, however, characterised by too great intensity on the part of the higher partial-tones. It is desirable to correct 136 QUALITY OF REED-SOUNDS. [V. § 66. this defect by strengthening the fundamental-tone of the clang. This is done by placing the reed in the mouth of an open pipe whose deepest tone coincides with the fundamental-tone of the reed- clang. This tone will then be most powerfully reinforced by resonance. The other partial-tones of the clang wUl also be strengthened by resonance, but to a smaller and smaller extent as their order rises. The force required to throw a column of air into rapid vibration is greater than that which suffices to set up a slow vibration. Since the force necessary to produce seg- mental vibration increases very rapidly as the sub- divisions of the air-column become more numerous, the very high partial-tones of the reed-clang are practi- cally unsupported by the resonance of the associated pipe. We shall see in the sequel how it happens that the quality of the resulting sound is improved by this circumstance. It is clear that sounds varying widely in quality may be obtained by associating a reed with pipes of varying lengths and forms. The resulting quality will be quite different according as the lowest tone which the pipe is capable of strengthening by reso- nance coincides with the fundamental-tone, or with one of the overtones, of the reed-sound. The form of the pipe may also be modified so as to be conical or of any other shape, which will bring in other V. §§ 67, 68.] ORCHESTRAL WIND-INSTRUMENTS. 137 changes in its resonating properties. In these ways we have provision for the great variety of quaUty among reed-pipes which we find represented in organ stops of that class. 4. Sounds of orchestral wind-instruments and of the human voice. 67. The fiute is in principle identical with an open flue-pipe. The lips, and a hole near the end of the tube, play the parts of the narrow slit and opposing edge. The quality of the instrument is sweet, but too nearly simple to be heard during a long solo without becoming wearisome. Its best effects are produced by contrast with the more brilliant quality of its orchestral colleagues. The clarionet, hauthois and bassoon have wooden reeds. In the horn and trumpet the lips of the per- former supply the place of a reed. 68. The apparatus of the human voice is essen- tially a reed (the vocal chords) associated with a resonance-cavity (the hollow of the mouth). The vocal chords are elastic bands situated at the top of the windpipe, and separated by a narrow slit, which opens and closes again with great exact- ness as air is forced through it from the lungs. The 138 MECHANISM OF THE HUMAN VOICE. [V. § 68. form and width of the sht allow of being quickly and extensively modified by the changing tension of the vocal chords, and thus sounds widely differing in pitch may be successively produced with sur- prising rapidity. In this respect, the human ' reed ' far exceeds any that we can artificially construct. The size and shape of the cavity of the mouth may be altered by opening or closing the jaws, raising or dropping the tongue and tightening or loosening the lips. We should expect that these movements would not be without effect on the reso- nance of the contained air, and this proves, on experiment, to be the fact. If we hold a vibrating tuning-fork close to the lips, and then modify the resonating cavity in the ways above described, we shall find that it resounds most powerfully to the fork selected when the parts of the mouth are in one definite position. If we try a fork of different pitch, the attitude of the mouth, for the strongest resonance, is no longer the same. Hence, when the vocal chords have originated a reed-sound containing numerous well-developed partial-tones, the mouth-cavity, by successively throwing itself into different postures, can favour by its resonance first one partial-tone, then another ; at one moment this group of partial-tones, at another V. § 69.] OVERTONES OF THE HUMAN VOICE. 139 that. In this manner endless varieties of quality are rendered possible. Good vocalisation, therefore, requires the resonating cavity to be so placed as to modify in the way most attractive to the ear the quality of the sounds produced by the vocal chords. The complete analysis of the sounds of the hu- man voice into their separate partial-tones presents peculiar difficulties to the unassisted ear, and can hardly be effected without the help of resonators such as those described in § 42. By their aid we can detect in the lower notes of a bass voice, when vigorously produced, shrill overtones reaching as far as No. 16, which is four Octaves above the funda- mental-tone. Under certain conditions these high overtones can be readily heard without recourse to resonators. When a body of voices are singing fortissimo without any instrumental accompaniment, a peculiar shrill tremulous sound is heard which is obviously far above the pitch of any note professedly being sung. This sound is, to my ears, so intensely shrill and piercing as to be often quite painful. I have also observed it when listening to the lower notes of a powerful contralto voice. The reason why these acute sounds are tremulous will be given later. 69. We close this discussion by describing a 140 COALESCENCE OF PARTIAL-TONES. [V. § 69. mode of submitting Helmholtz's general theory of musical quality to a further and very severe test. The sounds of tuning-forks mounted on their appropriate resonance-boxes are, as we know, very approximately simple tones [§ 48]. If therefore we allow a number of such sounds, coincident in pitch with the fundamental-tone and with the other partial- tones of any given clang, to be simultaneously pro- duced, the effect on the ear ought, if Helmholtz's theory is true, to be that of a single musical sound, not that of a group of separate notes. To try the experiment in its simplest shape, take two mounted forks forming the interval of an Octave, and cause them to utter their respective tones to- gether. For a short time we are able to distinguish the two notes as coming from separate instruments, but soon they blend into one sound, to which we assign the 'pitch of the loioer fork, and a quality more brilliant than that of either. So strong is the illusion, that we can hardly believe the higher fork to be really still contributing its note, until we ascertain that placing a finger on its prongs at once changes the timbre, by reducing it to the dull, un- interesting quality of a simple tone. The character of a clang consisting of only one overtone and the fundamental may be shown to admit of many dif- ferent shades of quality by suitably varying the V. § 69.] SYNTHETIC APPARATUS. 141 relative intensities of the two fork-tones in this experiment. If we add a fork a Fifth above the higher of the first two, and therefore yielding the third partial-tone of the clang of which they form the first and second, the three tones blend as per- fectly as the two did before, the only difference perceptible being an increase of brilliancy. The experiment admits of being carried further with the same result. If we were able to produce by means of tuning- forks as many simple tones of the series on p. 89 as we pleased, and also to control at will their relative intensities, it would be possible to imitate in this manner the characteristic quality of every musical in- strument. The unmanageable character of very high forks has as yet prevented this being done for sounds containing a large number of powerful overtones, but an apparatus on this principle was devised by Professor Helmholtz which imitates successfully sounds not involving more than the first six or eight partial-tones. His theory of quality is thus experimentally demonstrated both analytically and synthetically. We will examine in the next chapter certain theoretical considerations which have an important bearing on that theory. CHAPTER VI. ON THE CONNEXION BETWEEN QUALITY AND MODE OF VIBRATION. 70. It was stated in § 37 that, when a pendulum performs oscillations whose extent is small compared to the length of the pendulum, the period of a vibration is the same for any extent of swing loithin this limit. We will apply this fact to prove that the prongs of a tuning-fork vibrate in the same mode [§ 1 1] as a pendulum. When a sustained simple tone is being trans- mitted by the air, we may regard it as originated by a tuning-fork of appropriate pitch and size. But we know experimentally that, by suitable bowing, we may elicit from such a fork tones of various de- grees of intensity, but having all the same pitch. Here, therefore, though the extent of vibration varies, the period remains constant, which is the pendulum- law. Accordingly, the vibrations of a tuning-fork are identical in mode with those of a pendulum. The same thing will hold good of the aerial vibra- VI. §70.] COMPOSITION OF VIBRATIONS. 143 tions to which those of a fork give rise. Hence a simple tone is due to vibrations executed according to the pendulum-law. Such vibrations will, therefore, give rise to waves of condensation and rarefaction whose associated wave-form is that drawn in Fig. 17 his. It will be convenient to call the vibrations to which a simple tone is due simple vibrations ; and the associ- ated waves simple waves. We proceed to examine the modes of vibration corresponding to composite sounds. Let us first take the case of a clang consisting of but two simple tones, the fundamental and its first overtone. A particle of air engaged in transmitting this sound is simultaneously acted upon by two sets of forces which, if they acted separately, would cause it to perform two simple vibrations having periods bearing to each other the ratio of 1 to 2. We have to investigate what its motion will be under the joint action of these forces. The problem before us is the composition of two vibrations executed in the same straight line. Suppose that a series of particles originally at rest in the dotted line through O, Fig. 44, and capable of vibrating in paths perpendicular to it, are under simultaneous forces which, if acting separately, would give rise to the transmission of the parts of waves drawn in the figure. Let a and b be the 144 COMPOSITION OF VIBRATIONS. [VI. § 71. respective positions which a particle originally at O would at a given moment occupy if the forces acted separately. 71. In determining the corresponding position of this particle under the forces acting simultaneously we employ the following principle : — Tlie resultant displacement is equal to the sum or difference of the component displacements, according as these are pro- duced in the same or in opposite directions. (1) Fi^.44. (.z) In the application of this principle four different cases present themselves. In (l), crest falls on crest ; in (2), trough on trough ; and the displace- ment, Oc, of the particle from its position of rest, 0, is in both these instances equal to the sum of the displacements Oa and Oh. In (3) and (4), where the crest of one wave meets the trough of another, Oc is equal to the difference between Oa and Oh ; c being above or below the undisturbed line, accordmg VI. §72.] DETERMINATION OF JOINT WAVE. 145 as Oa is greater than Oh, as in (3), or less than Oh, as in (4). Thus, each tributary set of forces produces its full effect in its own direction, or, in other words, the displacement due to one set is superposed on that due to the other. In order to determine the form of the resultant wave, we have only to apply this principle to suc- cessive particles. We thus obtain an assemblage of points constituting the wave required. The problem of the motion of an air-particle engaged in transmitting simultaneously two partial- tones of a clang is, therefore, to be solved as follows. Let each simple vibration be represented by its associated wave. Ascertain by the process just de- scribed to what joint form the superposition of these two waves leads. The result will be the associated wave-form corresponding to the mode of particle- vibration to which the compound sound is due. 72. Before, however, we can lay down the two tributary waves, an important point remains to be settled. We wUl, for a moment, suppose that the two simple tones on which we are engaged are sustained by two tuning-forks situated as in the annexed figure, and that we are examining the transmission of their resulting clang along the dotted line with respect to which they are sym- metrically situated. T. 10 146 DIFFERENCE OF PHASE. [VI. § 72. Let the forks have been set sounding by pre- cisely simultaneous blows. They will then commence swinging out of their positions of rest in the same FigAS. ft V direction at the same instant. The points in an asso- ciated wave-form which correspond to the initial position of a vibrating particle are those in which the wave-form cuts the initial line. Hence, in laying down the tributary waves we must, in the present instance, begin them at one and the same point in the initial line, and take care that their convexities at that point are turned the same way, as at 0, Fig. 46. In this case the two vibrations are said to start in the same phase. If one of the two forks have been struck before the other, the former will not necessarily be passing through its equilibrium position at the moment when the latter is swinging out of that position. Hence, both sets of associated waves will no longer necessa- VI. § 72.] QUALITY AND PHASE. 147 rily cut the initial line at the same point. The result is of the kind shown in Fig. 47, where three different cases are represented: We have here vibrations starting in different -phases. It is clear from the figure that all phase- differences can he properly represented by merely causing the waves engaged to assume different po- sitions upon the initial line with reference to each other. The second and third cases are obtainable from the first by sliding the system of shorter waves bodily along the initial line, while the other system of waves retains its position. By the help of the instrument mentioned in § 69, Professor Helmholtz has demonstrated that, when a number of partial-tones are independently produced, the clang into which they coalesce has the 10—2 148 SIMPLE AND RESULTANT WAVE-FORMS. [VI. §73. same quality, whatever differences of phase may exist among the systems of sim,ple vibrations to which the constituent partial-tones are due. Accordingly, we may expect to find that not one single waveform, but many such forms, correspond to a sound of given quality and pitch. In Figs. 48, 49, 50, the associated wave-form cor- responding to our clang of two partial-tones (§71) is constructed for three degrees of phase-difference. The simple constituent waves are shown in thin, the result of their composition in full lines. In each case two complete wave-lengths of the latter are exhibited. Figs. 51 and 52 present two wave-forms drawn, in the same way, for a clang of constant pitch and quality containing the partial-tones 1, 2 and 3. The dissimilarity of form, and therefore of corre- sponding particle -vibration, is in both sets of figures very marked. 73. It has been shown that, by mere alteration of phase, a very great variety of resultant wave-forms can be obtained from two sets of simple waves of given lengths and amplitudes. Each one of these forms will give rise to a cycle of others, if we allow the relative amplitudes of the constituent systems to be changed, while keeping the difference of phase constant. If, therefore, we have at our disposal the yi. § 73.] SIMPLE AND RESULTANT WA VE-FORMS. 149 150 VARIETY OF RESULTANT WAVE-FOBMS. [VI. §73. systems of simple waves corresponding to an unlimited number of partial-tones, and can assign to them any Fig.eL degrees of intensity and phase-difference that we choose, it is manifest that we may produce by their combination an endless series of different resultant wave-forms. On the other hand, it is not evident that, even out of this rich abundance of materials, we could build up every form of wave which could possibly he assigned. The French mathematician Fourier has, however, demonstrated that there is no form of wave which (unless itself simple) cannot be compounded out of a number of simple waves YI. § 73.] FOURIER'S THEOREM. 151 whose lengths' are inversely as the numbers 1, 2, 3, 4, &c. He has further shown that each individual wave-form admits of being thus compounded in only one way, and has provided the means of calculating, in any given case, how many and which members of the series will appear, their relative amplitudes and their differences of phase. When translated from the language of Mechanics into that of Acoustics, the theorem of Fourier asserts that every regular musical sound is resolvable into a definite number of simple tones whose relative pitch follows the law of the partial-tone series. It thus supplies a theoretical basis for the analysis and synthesis of composite sounds which have been ex- perimentally effected in chapters iv. and v. When we are listening to a sustained clang, the air at any one point within the orifice of the ear can have only one definite mode of particle- vibration at any one moment. How does the ear behave towards any such given vibration? It proceeds as foUows. If the vibration is simple, it leaves it alone. If com- posite, it analyses it into a series of simple vibrations whose rates are once, twice, three times &c. that of the given vibration, in accordance with Fourier's theorem. In the former event the ear perceives only a simple tone. In the latter, it is able to recognise, ' Arranged in diminishing order. 152 ANALYSING POWER OF THE EAR. [VI. § 73. by suitably directed and assisted efforts, partial- tones corresponding to the rate of each constituent into which it has analysed the composite vibration origi- nally presented to it. The ear being deaf to differ- ences of phase in partial-tones (§ 72), perceives no difference between sounds due to modes of vibration such as those which give rise to the three resultant associated wave-forms shown in Figs. 48, 49, 50, but merely resolves them into the same single parr of partial-tones. Since, however, only one such resolu- tion of a given vibration-mode is possible, the ear can never vary in the group of partial-tones into which it resolves an assigned clang. The power possessed by the ear of thus singling out the constituent tones of a clang and assigning to them their relative intensities is unlike any cor- responding capacity of the eye. Take for instance the two curves shown in Figs. 51 and 52, and try to determine, by the eye alone, what simple waves, present with what amplitudes, must be superposed in order to reproduce those forms. The eye wUl be found absolutely to break down in the attempt. We have seen that the loudness of a composite sound depends on extent of vibration, and its pitch on rate of vibration. There remains only one variable element, viz. mode of vibration, to account for the quality of the sound. From this consideration it VI. § 73.] QUALITY AND MODE OF VIBRATION. 153 follows that some connexion must exist between the quality of a sound and the mode of aerial vibration to which the sound is due. Up to the time of Helm- holtz no advance had been made in clearing up the nature of this connexion. It was reserved for him to show that, while no two sounds of different quality can correspond to the same mode of vibration, an indefinitely large number of modes of vibration may give rise to a sound of only one degree of quality. In other words, mode of vibration determines quality, but quality does not determine mode of vibration. CHAPTER VII. ON THE INTERFERENCE OF SOUND, AND ON ' BEATS.' 74. In § 71 we examined the principle on which the general problem of the composition of vibrations is solved. We now approach certain very import- ant particular cases of that problem, which it wiU be worth while to solve both independently and as instances of the method repeatedly applied in § 72. Suppose that a particle of air is vibrating be- tween the extreme positions A and B while convey- Fig.SS. ing a sustained simple tone pi-oduced by a tuning-fork, or stopped flue-pipe. Now let a second instrument of the same kind be caused to emit a tone exactly in unison with the first. We will assume that, when the waves of the second tone reach the pai-ticle, it is just on the point of starting from A towards B. Two extreme cases are now possible, depending on the movement which the particle would have exe- cuted in virtue of the later-impressed vibration alone. VII. § 74.J INTERFERENCE OF SOUND. 155 First, suppose that movement to be from A along the line AB, either through a greater or less distance than AB, back again to A, and so on. Here the separate effects of the two sets of vibrations will be added to- gether, the particle will, therefore, perform vibrations of larger extent than it would under either component separately. Next, suppose that, under the second set of vibrations alone, the particle would move from A in the opposite direction to its former course, i.e. along BA produced, shown by a dotted line in the figure. In this case the separate effects are absolutely antagonistic ; accordingly the joint result is that due to the difference of its components. The particle wUl, therefore, execute less extensive vibrations than it would have done under the more powerful of the two components acting alone. The most striking result presents itself when the two systems of vibrations, besides being in opposition to each other, are also exactly equal in extent. In this case the air-particle, being solicited by equal forces in opposite directions, remains at rest, the two systems of vibrations completely neutralising each other's effects. In general, however, these systems, even when equal in extent of vibration, are neither in complete opposition nor in complete accordance, but in an intermediate attitude, so as only partially to counteract, or support, each other. These conclusions 156 INTERFERENCE OF SOUND. [VII. § 74. admit of being exhibited in a more complete manner by means of associated waves. We have only to lay down the simple wave-forms corresponding to the constituent vibrations, and superpose them as in § 72. The reader will have noticed that the diffe- rences of relative motion described on the preceding page are merely phase-differences. Fiy. 54 . In Fig. 54, (1), (2), (3), we have two waves of unequal amplitudes in complete accordance, complete antagonism and an intermediate condition, respec- VII. § 75.] INTERFERENCE OF SOUND. 157 tively. In Fig. 55, a case of equal and opposite waves is shown. In (l) Fig. 54, the resultant wave is the sum, and in (2) the difference of the compo- nent waves. In (3), we get a wave of intermediate amplitude. These three resultant waves are neces- sarily simple, as otherwise two simple tones in unison would give rise to a composite sound, which would be absurd. In Fig. 55 the wave-form degenerates into the initial line, i.e. no efi'ect whatever is pro- duced. Fi0 .SS. 75. Thus, when one simple tone is being heard, we by no means necessarily obtain an increase of loudness by exciting a second simple tone of the same pitch. On the contrary, we m,ay thus weaken the original sound, or even extinguish it entirely. When this occurs we have an instance of a phe- nomenon which goes by the name of Interference. That two sounds should produce absolute silence seems, at first sight, as absurd as that two loaves should be equivalent to no bread. This is, however, only because we are accustomed to think of Sound as 158 INTERFERENCE OF SOUND. [VII. § 76. something possessing an external substantial exist- ence ; not as consisting merely in a state of motion of certain air-particles, and therefore liable, on the application of suitable forces, to be absolutely anni- hilated. A single tuning-fork presents an example of this very important phenomenon. Each prong sets up vibrations corresponding to a simple tone, and the two tones so produced are of the same pitch and intensity. If the fork, after being struck, is held between the finger and thumb, and made to re- volve slowly about its own axis, four positions of the fork with reference to the ear wUl be found where the sound goes completely out. These posi- tions are mid-way between the four in which the plane sides of the prongs are held straight before the ear. As the fork revolves from one of these positions of loud sound to that at right-angles to it, the sound gradually wanes, is extinguished in passing the Interference-position, reappears very feebly im- mediately afterwards, and then continues to gain strength until the quarter of a revolution has been completed. 76. The case of coexistent unisons has now been adequately examined : we proceed to enquire what happens when two simple tones diffeHng slightly in fitch are simultaneously produced. The problem is, YII. § 76.] BEATS OF SIMPLE TONES. 159 in fact, to compound two sets of pendulum-vibrations whose periods are not exactly equal. Let us fix on a moment of time at which the two component vibra- tions are in complete accordance, and suppose that a resultant vibration is just commencing from left to right. It will be convenient to call this an outward, and its opposite an inward swing. Since the periods of the two component vibrations are unequal, one of them will at once begin to gain on the other, and therefore, directly after the start, they will cease to be in complete accordance. It is easy to ascertain what their subsequent bearing towards each other will be, by considering two ordinary pendulums of unequal periods, both beginning an outward swing at the same instant. Let A be the slower, B the quicker pendulum. When A has just finished its outward swing, B will have already turned back and per- formed a portion of its next inward swing. Thus, during each successive swing oi A, B will gain a certain distance upon it. When B has, in this manner, gained one whole swing, i.e. half a complete oscillation, upon A, it will begin an inward swing at the moment when A is commencing an outward swing. The two vibrations are here, for the moment, in C07n- plete opposition. After another interval of equal length, B, having gained another whole swing, will be one complete oscillation ahead of A, and they will 160 BEATS OF SIMPLE TONES. [VII. § 76. therefore start on the next outward swing together, i.e. the vibrations will be momentarily in complete accordance. Thus, during the time requisite to enable B to perform one entire oscillation more than A, there occur the following changes. Complete accordance of vibrations, lasting only for a single swing of the more rapid pendulum, followed by partial accordance, in its turn gradually giving way to discordance, which culminates in complete opposi- tion at the middle of the period, and then, during its latter half, gradually yields to returning accordance, which regains completeness just as the period closes. It follows from this that, in the case of two simple tones differing slightly in pitch, we must hear a sound going through regularly recurring alternations of loudness in equal successive intervals of time, its greatest intensity exceeding, and its least intensity falling short of, that of the louder of the two tones. Each recurrence of the maximum in- tensity is called a heat, and it is clear that exactly one such beat will be heard in each interval of time during which the acuter of the two simple tones performs one more vibration than the graver tone. Accordingly, the number of beats heard in any assigned time will be equal to the number of com- plete vibrations which the one tone gains on the VII. § 77.] NUMBER OF BEATS PER SECOND. 161 otlier in that time. We may express this result more briefly as follows : — The number of heats per second due to two simple tones is equal to the difference of their respective vihration-numhers. 77. By means of associated wave-forms we can obtain a graphic representation of beats, which will probably be more directly intelligible than any verbal description. In Fig. 56, the constituent simple waves are laid down, and their resultant is constructed, for the interval of a semi-Tone. The vibration-fraction for that interval is — -; i.e. 16 15 vibrations of the higher tone are performed in the same time as 15 of the lower. The figure repre- sents completely the state of things from a maxi- inum of intensity to the adjacent minimutn. The time during which this change occurs is one-half of that above-mentioned : accordingly the figure shows 8 and 7-J wave-lengths of the respective systems. Thus half a heat is here pictorially represented, the amplitude of the resultant waves steadUy diminish- ing during this period. "We have only to turn the figure upside down, to get a picture of what occurs in the next following equal period. The amplitudes here again increase until they regain their former dimensions. One whole heat is thus accounted for. T. 11 162 GRArmC REPRESENTATION OF BEATS. [VII. §77. VII. §78.] EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF BEATS. 163 In addition to the alternations of intensity whicli characterise beats, they also contain variations of pitch. The existence of such variations is both theoretically demonstrable and experimentally re- cognisable, but they are too minute to require ex- amination here'. 78. The most direct way of studying the beats of simple tones experimentally is to take two unison tuning-forks and attach a small peUet of wax to the extremity of a prong of one of them. The fork so operated on becomes slightly heavier than before ; its vibrations are therefore slightly retarded, and its pitch lowered. When both forks are struck and held to the ear, beats are heard. These will be most distinct when the forks' tones are exactly equally loud, for in this case the minima of intensity will be equal to zero, and the beats will therefore be separated by intervals of absolute, though but mo- mentary, silence. The increase in rapidity of the beats, as the interval between the beating tones widens, may be shown by gradually loading one of the forks more and more heavily with wax pellets, or by a small coin pressed upon them. If it is desired ■ The reader may, if he wishes to pursue this subject, refer to a Paper, by the Author, ou 'Variations of Pitch in Beats,' in the Philosophical Magazine for July, 1872, from which Fig. 56 has been copied. 11—2 164 EXPERIMENTAL STUDY OF BEATS. [VII. § 78. to exhibit these phenomena to several persons at once, the forks should first be mounted on their resonant-boxes, and, after the pellets have been attached, stroked with a resined bow, care being taken to produce tones as nearly as possible equal in intensity. Slow beats may also be obtained from any instrument capable of producing tones whose vibration-numbers differ by a sufficiently small amount. Thus, if the strings corresponding to a single note of the pianoforte are not strictly in unison, such beats are heard on striking the note. If the tuning is perfect, a wax pellet attached to one of the wires will lower its pitch sufficiently to produce the desired effect. Beats not too fast to be readily counted arise between adjacent low notes on the harmonium, or, still more conspicuously, on large organs. They are also frequently to be heard in the sounds of church bells, or in those emitted by the telegraph. wires when vibrating in a strong wind. In order to observe them in the last instance, it is best to press one ear against a telegraph-post and close the other : the beats then come out with re- markable distinctness. It should be noticed that, when we are dealing with two composite sounds, several sets of beats may be heard at the same time, if pairs of partial-tones are in relative positions suited to produce them. Thus, suppose that two VII. § 78.] BEATS OF COMPOUND SOUNDS. 165 clangs coexist, each of which contains the first six partial-tones of the series audibly developed. Since the second, third, &c. partial-tones of each clang make twice, three times, &c. as many vibrations per second as their respective fundamental-tones, [§ 43] it follows that the differences between the vibration-numbers of successive pairs of partial-tones belonging to the two clangs will be twice, three times, &c., the differ- ence between the vibration-numbers of the two fundamental-tones. Accordingly, if the fundamental- tones give rise to beats, we may hear, in addition to the series so accounted for, five other sets of beats, respectively twice, three, four, five and six times as rapid as they. In order to determine the number of beats per second for any such set, we need only multiply the number of the fundamental beats by the order of the partial-tones concerned. The beats of two simple tones necessarily become more rapid if the higher tone be sharpened, or the lower flattened ; i.e. if the interval they form with each other be widened. The beats may, however, also be accelerated without altering the interval, by merely placing it in a higher part of the scale. Greater vibration-numbers are thus obtained with a pro- portionately larger difference between them, though their ratio to each other remains what it was before. Thus the rapidity of the beats due to 166 BEATS OF A GIVEN INTERVAL. [VII. § 78. an assigned interval depends jointly on two ele- ments, the width of the interval and its position in the musical scale ; in other words, on both the relative and absolute pitch of the tones which form it. CHAPTER VIII. ON CONCOKD AND DISCORD. 79. A question of fundamental importance now presents itself, viz. What becomes of beats when they are so rapid that they can no longer be separately perceived by the ear? In order to answer it, the best plan is to take two unison-forks of medium pitch, mounted on their resonance-boxes, attach a small pellet of wax to a prong of one of them, and then gradually increase the quantity of wax. At first very slow beats are heard, and as long as their number does not exceed four or five in a second, the ear can follow and count them without difficulty. As they become more rapid the difficulty of counting them augments, until at last they cease to be separ- ately recognisable. Even then, however, the ear retains the conviction that the sound it hears is a series of rapid alternations, and not a continuous tone. Its intermittent character is not lost, although the intermittances themselves pass by too rapidly 168 CAUSE OF DliSSONANCE. [VIII. §79. for individual recognition. Exactly the same thing may be observed in the roll of a side drum, which no one is in danger of mistaking for a continuous sound. Rapid beats produce a decidedly harsh and grating effect on the ear ; and this is quite what the analogy of our other senses would lead us to expect. The disagreeable impressions excited in the organs of sight by a flickering unsteady light, and in those of touch by tickling or scratching, are familiar to every one. The effect of rapid beats is, in fact, identical with the sensation to which we commonly attach the name of dissonance. Let us examine, in somewhat greater detail, the conditions necessary for its production between two simple tones. If we take a pair of middle- C forks, and gradually throw them more and more out of unison with each other in the way already described, the roughness due to their beats reaches its maximum when the interval between them is about a half-Tone : for a whole Tone it is decidedly less marked, and when the interval amounts to a Minor Third, scarcely a trace of it remains. Hence, in order that dissonance may arise between two simple tones, they must form with each other a narrower interval than a Minor Third. If we call this interval the heating -distance for two such tones, we may express the above condition thus. Dissonance can arise directly between two simple VIII. §79. THE 'BEATING DISTANCE.' 169 tones only when they are within heating-distance of each otherK It follows from what was shown at the end of Chapter VII. that the same beating interval will give rise to very different numbers of beats per second according as the tones which form it occupy a low or high position in the scale. Such an interval, e.g. a whole Tone, becomes palpably less dissonant as it is successively raised in pitch. Accordingly the beating-distance, which for tones of medium pitch we have roughly fixed at a Minor Third, must be understood to expand somewhat in low, and cor- respondingly contract in high, regions of the scale. The general partial-tone series consists of simple tones which, up to the seventh, are mutually out of beating-distance : above the seventh they close in rapidly upon each other. In the neighbourhood of 10, the interval between adjacent partial-tones is about a whole Tone; near 16, a semi-Tone; higher in the series they come to still closer quarters. These high partial-tones are, therefore, so situated as to produce harsh dissonances with each other. Where they are strongly developed in a clang, there ' It will be shown in the sequel [§§ 89 — 92] that dissonance may, in the case of simple tones forming intervals wider than a Minor Third, arise from beats other than those here under con- sideration. 170 DISSONANT OVERTONES. [VIII. § 80. will therefore be a certain inevitable roughness in its quality. This is the cause of the harsh character of trumpet or trombone notes, and also of the shrill tremulous sounds sometimes observed in the human voice (§68). In fact we may regard the partial- tone series above the seventh tone, when fully repre- sented, as contributing mere noise to the clang. This explains why it is advantageous that the lower partial-tones of organ reeds should be more strength- ened by resonance than the higher ones [see § 66]. 80. It has been shown that, when two simple tones are simultaneously sustained, beats can arise directly between them only under one condition, viz. that the tones shall differ in pitch by less than a Minor Third, or thereabouts. When, however, the two co-existing sounds are no longer simple tones, but composite clangs each consisting of a series of well-developed partial-tones, the case becomes alto- gether different. Let us examine the state of things which then presents itself The sounds of most musical instruments practi- cally contain only the first six partial-tones ; we will, therefore, assume this to be the case with the clangs before us. No beats can then arise between partial-tones of the same clang, since no two of them are written a Minor Third of each other. Dissonance due to beats will, however, be produced VIII. § 81.] DISSONANCE OF TWO CLANGS. 171 if a partial-tone belonging to one clang is within that distance of a partial-tone belonging to the other clang. Several pairs of tones may be thus situated, and, if so, each will contribute its share of roughness to the general effect. The intensity of the roughness due to any such pair will depend chiefly on the respective orders to which the beating partial-tones belong, and on the interval between them. The lowest partial-tones, being the loudest, produce the most powerful beats, and half-Tone beats are, in general, harsher than those of a whole Tone. In determining the general effect of a combination of two clangs, we have to ascertain what pairs of partial-tones come within beating-distance, and to estimate the amount of roughness due to each pair. The joint effect of all these roughnesses, if there are several such pairs, or the roughness of a single pair if there is but one, constitutes the dissonance of the combination. If there be no dissonance, the combi- nation is described as a perfect concord. When dissonance is present, its amount will decide whether the combination shall be called an imperfect concord or a discord. The line separating the two must therefore, of necessity, be somewhat arbitrarily drawn. 81. Let us examine the principal consonant intervals, in the manner above described, beginning with the Octave. 172 CONSONANCE AND DISSONANCE. [VIIT. § 8L '.k -P-' St The minims here represent the fundamental- tones ; the crotchets above them corresponding over- tones. Those belonging to the higher clang are only written down as far as the third, since the fourth, fifth and sixth meet no corresponding tones of the lower clang with which to form beating pairs. As long as the tuning is perfect, each partial-tone of the higher clang coincides exactly with one belonging to the lower. No dissonance, therefore, can occur, and the combination is a perfect concord. But suppose the higher C to be slightly out of tune : each of its partial-tones will be correspondingly too sharp or too flat, and three sets of beats will be heard between the partial-tones 2 — 1, 4 — 2 and 6 — 3. When the higher C is as much as a semi-Tone wrong, the result is i^k iT^- -¥-' zipi The pair 2 — 1 is of the most importance, on account of the prominence of the partial-tones which VIII. §81.] OCTAVE. 173 form it. The higher pairs contribute a weaker dis- sonance. As beats of a semi-Tone correspond to about maximum roughness in the middle region of the scale, we have before us an exceedingly harsh discord. As the pitch of the higher note is gradually corrected, the rapidity of the beats diminishes, but the tuning must be rigorously accurate to make them entirely vanish. If the note makes but one vibration per second too many or too few, which corresponds to a difference in pitch of only about a thirtieth part of a whole Tone, the defect of tuning makes itself felt by three sets of beats, of 1, 2 and 3 per second respectively. The tuner must keep slightly altering the pitch until he at length hits on that which completely extinguishes the beats. We saw in an earlier part of this inquiry (§ 34) that, when two sounds form with each other the interval of an Octave, their vibration-numbers must be in the ratio of 2 : 1. Long after it had been experi- mentally ascertained that rigorous compliance with this arithmetical condition was essential for securing a perfectly smooth Octave, the reason for this ne- cessity remained entirely unknown, and nothing but the vaguest and most fanciful suggestions were offered to account for it — such as, for instance, that " the human mind delights in simple numerical rela- tions." This attempt at explanation overlooked the 174 COINCIDENCE OF PARTIAL TONES. [VIII. § 82. obvious fact that many people who knew nothing either about vibrations or the dehghts of simple numerical relations, could tell a perfect Octave from an imperfect one a great deal better than most men of science. The true explanation, which was left for Helmholtz to discover, lies in the fact that only by exactly satisfying the assigned numerical relation can the partial-tones of the higher clang he brought into exact coincidence with partial-tones of the lower, and thus all beats and consequent dissonance prevented. 82. No interval naiTower than an Octave can be found which gives an absolutely perfect concord. The nearest approach to such a concord is the Fifth :— ^: Here we get two pairs of coincidences, 3 — 2 and 6 — 4, but a certain roughness is caused by 3 of the higher clang being within beating-distance of both 4 and 5 of the lower clang. It is true that, since 4 and 5 are generally weak, and the beating intervals whole Tones, this roughness will be but slight : still, the dissonance thus caused prevents our classing the Fifth as an interval quite equally smooth with the VIII. § 83.] FIFTH AND FOURTH. 175 Octave. The tuning must be perfectly accurate, the Fifth being closely bounded by harsh discords. The result of an error of a semi-Tone is as follows : — W^^ 31^1^ --l?i- %J For every single vibration per second by which the higher clang is out of tune, there will be two beats per second from the pair 3 — 2, with others of greater rapidity, but less intensity, from the higher pairs. 83. For the Fourth we have tl ^ mz The amount of dissonance is greater than in the case of the Fifth, since 3 and 2 are usually hotli powerful tones, and therefore produce louder beats than those of 4 — 3 and 5 — 3. There are, further, the beating pairs 5 — 4, 6 — 4 and 6 — 5. Moreover the first pair of coincident partial-tones, 4—3, are in general weaker than the beating pair below them, 3 — 2. The Fourth is bounded only on one side by a harsh discord. If its upper clang is half a note 176 MAJOR THIRD AND MAJOR SIXTH. [VIII. § 84. too sharp, we have the interval C — i^8, which is treated in the last figure but one. Slight flattening of the F will set the pair 4 — 3 beating ; the disap- pearance of their beats thus secures the accurate tuning of the interval. On the other hand, lowering F weakens the beats of 3 — 2 by widening the dis- tance between those tones, and therefore tends to lessen the whole amount of roughness. These con- siderations go far to explain the fact that a long dispute runs through the history of Music, as to whether the Fourth ought to be treated as a concord or as a discord. The decision ultimately arrived at in favour of the former of these alternatives was per- haps, as Helraholtz suggests, due more to the fact that the Fourth is the inversion' of the Fifth than to the inherent smoothness of the former interval. 84. Next come the intervals of the Major Third and Major Sixth, which shall be taken together, as they are very nearly equally consonant. ii^i^ at U2Z ±?ai m The dissonance due to the pair 3 — 2, separated by a Tone, in the Sixth, is perhaps about equal to ' See § 97 post. VIII. § 85.] MINOR THIRD AND MINOR SIXTH. 177 that of the weaker pair 4 — 3, which are only a semi- Tone apart in the Third. The ' definition ' of these intervals, i.e. the accuracy of tuning requisite for a good effect, will, as it depends in each on the fifth partial-tone of the lower clang, be in general not great. 85. The remaining intervals narrower than an Octave which rank as concords are the Minor Third and Minor Sixth. Each contains strong elements of dissonance ; in fact we are here near the boundary between concords and discords. As regards sharpness of definition, the 2 OE ibf2-J tones 6 and 5, on which it depends in the first of the two intervals, are, in the sounds of many instruments, weak or even entirely absent, while for the second interval the series of partial-tones must be extended as far as the 8th of the lower clang in order to reach the first coincident pair. Accordingly the Minor Sixth can hardly be said to be defined at all, for clangs of ordinary quality, by coincidence of partial- tones. Its powerful beating pair 3 — 2, separated by the interval of greatest dissonance, a semi-Tone, makes it the roughest of all the concords. On the T. 12 178 DISSONANT INTERVALS. [VIII. §§ 86, 87. pianoforte, and other instruments of fixed sounds, the same notes (CE?) which represent the Minor Third have also to do duty as one of the harshest discords, the Augmented Second (CDS), and, in like manner, CA\> stands for the Minor Third and also for the Augmented Fifth (CGt). The extremely defective consonance of the Minor Third and Minor Sixth could hardly be more conclusively shown than by the fact just mentioned. 86. As regards the dissonant Tonic intervals of the scale, we have, beside those incidentally examined above, the semi-Tone, Tone and Minor Seventh. The first two need not be examined, since obviously each pair of corresponding overtones are brought within the same beating intervals as the two fundamentals. The dissonance resulting is, of course, harsher for the half than for the whole Tone. The Minor Seventh is constituted thus : — HI ^ It is the mildest of the discords, in fact in mere smoothness it decidedly surpasses the Minor Sixth. 87. In order that the reader may see at a glance the whole result of this somewhat laborious discussion, VIII. § 87.] PICTURE OF INTERVALS. 179 we subjoin a graphical representation of the amount of dissonance contained in the several intervals of the scale. The figure is taken, with some slight altera- tions, from one given in Helmholtz's work. The in- tervals, reckoned from middle G, are denoted by dis- tances measured along the horizontal straight line. The dissonance for each interval is represented by the vertical distance of the curved line from the corresponding point on the horizontal line. The cal- culations on which the curve is based were made by Helmholtz for two constituent clangs of the quality of the violin. THg.Sr. G At) A Bb B C The figure indicates the sharpness of definition of an interval by the steepness with which the curve ascends on both sides of it. If we regarded the out- line as that of a mountain chain, the discords would be represented by peaks, and the concords hj passes. The lowness and narrowness of a particular pass would measure the smoothness and definition of the corresponding musical interval. 12—2 180 CONSONANCE AND QUALITY. [VIII. § 88. 88. The theory of musical consonance and dis- sonance, our examination of which is now concluded, shows that the degree of smoothness or roughness possessed by an assigned interval is not fixed and invariable, but dependent on the quality of the sounds hy which the interval is held. The results we have arrived at are generally true for sounds containing the first six partial-tones, but they will not apply, without modification, to clangs differently constituted. Let us take a case or two in illustration of this point. Suppose, for instance, we are dealing with sounds such as those of stopped organ-pipes which contain only odd partial-tones (§ 59). It is at once clear from § 82 that the interval of the Augmented Fourth, C — F$, which owes its dissonant character to the beating pairs 3 — 2, 4 — 3 and 6 — 4, wiU become something quite different when the dissonance due to all these pairs disappears, as it must do, since each of them contains at least one partial-tone of an even order. The Minor Sixth would also gain in smoothness with notes of such quality, by the cessation of dissonance due to the pair 3 — 2. Further, if the two notes forming any interval are held by instruments of different quality, the smoothness of the combination may greatly depend on which instrument takes the lower and which the upper note. Thus e.g. for the interval of a Fifth VIII, § 89.] CASE OF SIMPLE TONES. 181 reference to § 82 will show that the instrument which has the weaker third partial-tone should for the smoother effect take the upper note, since that arrangement reduces to a minimum the dissonance due to the pairs 4 — 3 and 5 — 3. For the interval of a Fourth the opposite arrangement gives the smoother effect by minimising the dissonance 3 — 2. Experiments confirming theoretical conclusions of this kind may be made with great ease on the stops of an organ possessing more than a single manual. The above considerations laid down by Helmholtz open a whole field of results which I believe to be entirely in advance of any hitherto obtained by Musical theorists'. 89. It is possible to draw from the general theory of consonance and dissonance an inference which seems, at first sight, fatal to the truth of the theory itself "If," it may be said, "the difference between a consonant and a dissonant interval depends entirely on the behaviour towards each other of particular overtones ; then, in the case of sounds like those of large stopped flue-pipes, where there are no overtones at all, the distinction between concords and discords ought entirely to disappear, and the interval of a Seventh, for instance, ' " Musical theory " is as misleading a term as could possibly be invented to describe conclusions wholly empirical. 182 COMBINATION-TONES. [VIII. § 90. to sound just as smooth as that of an Octave. As this is not the fact, the theory cannot be true." In order to meet this objection, it will be necessary- first to acquaint the reader with certain known expe- rimental facts which Helmholtz has dragged out of the obscurity in which they had lain for fully a century, and forced to bear testimony in completion of his theory. 90. Let two tuning-forks of different pitch mounted on their respective resonance-boxes be thrown into powerful vibration by a resined bow. With adequate attention it is possible to recognise, in addition to the tones of the forks themselves, cer- tain new sounds which usually differ in pitch from both the former ones. These sounds, called from the man- ner of their production combination-tones, fall into two categories, with only one of which, discovered in 1740 by a German organist named Sorge, we need now concern ourselves. It consists of a series classed as combination-tones of the Jirst, second, third, &c., orders, of which the first is of the most importance, as it can be heard without difficulty. Its pitch is determined by the following law. The combination-tone of the first order of two simple primary tones has for its vibration-number the differ- ence between the respective vibration-numbers of the primaries. Thus, e.g., if the two primaries make 200 VIII. § 90.] CO MB IN A TION- TONES. 183 and 300 vibrations per second, and therefore form a Fifth with each other, the combination-tone will make 100 vibrations per second, and therefore lie exactly one Octave below the graver of the two primary tones. By applying this rule we can deter- mine the pitch of the first-order combination-tone for a pair of simple primaries forming any given in- terval with each other. The following table, copied from Helmholtz's work, shows the results for all the consonant intervals not exceeding one Octave. Interval. Vibration-ratio. Difference. Depth of the CombiDation-tone below the graver primary. Octave 1 2 Unison Fifth 2 3 Octave Fourth 3 4 Twelfth Major Third 4 5 Two Octaves Minor Third 5 6 1 Two Octaves & a Major Third Major Sixth 3 5 2 Fifth Minor Sixth 5 8 3 Major Sixth In Musical notation the same thing stands thus, the primaries being denoted by minims and the combination-tones by crotchets. lE ^- ^ e^ ti 184 INTENSITY OF COMBINATION-TONES. [VIII. §90. Combination-tones are produced with unusual distinctness by the harmonium. If the primaries shown in the treble stave are played on that instru- ment while the pressure of air in the bellows is vigorously sustained, the corresponding combina- tion-tones of the first order, written in the bass, come out with unmistakable clearness. With pianoforte- sounds, on the contrary, combination-tones can be recognised only when the primaries are struck very forcibly, and they are always extremely faint and rapidly evanescent. If, however, the key corre- sponding to the combination-tone sought be first silently pressed down, its note is sometimes sustained by resonance when the keys of the two primaries have been smartly struck "staccato'^. Combination-tones of the second order may be treated as if they were first-order tones produced betM^een one or other of the primaries and the com- bination-tone of the first order. Similarly we may regard each combination-tone of the third order as due to a second-order tone paired either with one of the primaries, with the first-order tone or with its own fellow of the second order. Successive subtraction will therefore enable us to determine the vibration- number of a combination-tone of any order from the vibration-numbers of the two primaries. ' i.e. allowed to rise again as quickly as possible after the blow. VIII. § 91.] BEATS OF COMBINATION-TONES. 185 Combination-tones grow rapidly feebler as their order becomes higher. Those of the first order are usually distinct enough, and those of the second can be heard with a little trouble. The third order is only recognisable when entire stillness is secured, and the greatest attention paid. It is a moot point whether fourth -order tones can be heard at all. 91. We can now show that the existence of combination-tones prevents intervals formed by two simple tones from altogether lacking the characteristic differences of consonance and dissonance, though those differences are far less marked than in the case of composite sounds. To begin with the Octave. Let us suppose that we have two simple tones form- ing nearly this interval, but that the higher of them is a little sharp, so that the Octave is not strictly in tune, is what is called slightly impure. Let the lower tone make 100, the higher 201, vibrations per second. They will give rise to a combination-tone making 101 vibrations per second (§ 89), and this with the lower primary will produce one heat per second. If the higher primary had been flat instead of sharp, making say 199 vibrations per second, we should have had 99 as combination-tone, giving rise, with 100, to beats of the same rapidity as before. These beats cannot be got rid of except by making the vibration-ratio exactly 1 : 2, i.e. the Octave 186 INFLUENCE OF COMBINATION-TONES. [VIII. §91. perfectly pure. The roughness must increase both when the interval widens and when it contracts, so that the Octave, in simple tones, is a well-defined concord bounded on either side by decided discords. This result may be easily verified experimentally by taking two tuning-forks forming an Octave with each other, and throwing the interval slightly out of tune by causing a pellet of wax to adhere to a prong of one of them. On vigorously exciting the forks the beats will be distinctly heard'. The Octave is the only interval which is defined by the beats of a combination-tone of the first order with one of the primary tones. For the next smoothest concord, that of the Fifth, we are obliged to have recourse to the second order. Thus, pro- ceeding by successive subtraction, we have : — Prunaries 200 301 C. T. of 1st order 101 C. Ts. of 2nd order 99 200 No. of beats per sec. 2 ' As, however, this mode of treatment produces very percept- ible overtones [§ 48, note\, the experimenter must be ou his guard against attribviting to combination-tone beats an effect which is really due to beats of overtones. There is special risk of being thus led astray in the case of the Octave, where the first-order combination-tone coincides in pitch with the lower of the two primaries. The result of my own observation is that with mounted forks excited in the gentlest possible way there is but slight dissonance even in an impure Octave. VIII. §92.] INFLUENCE OF COMBINATION-TONES. 187 The Fifth is, thus, a fairly well-defined concord, though decidedly less sharply bounded than the Octave, owing to the feebleness of the C. T. of the second order. For the Fourth we have : — Primaries 300 401 C. T. of 1st order 101 C. Ts. of 2nd order 199 300 C. Ts. of 3rd order 101 202 98 No. of beats per sec. 3 The third-order tones being excessively weak, the interval of a Fourth can scarcely be said to be de- fined at all. Still less can the remaining consonant intervals of the scale be regarded as defined by beats due to combination-tones of yet higher orders. 92. With two moderately loud simple tones, then, the case stands thus. The interval of a Second is rendered palpably dissonant by direct beats of the primaries ; that of a Major Seventh slightly so by beats of a first-order combination- tone with one of the primaries. There is a certain amount of dissonance in intervals slightly narrower, or slightly wider, than a Fifth, but of a feebler kind than in the case of the Octave, inasmuch as one of the two Combination-Tones producing it is of the second order. Whatever dissonance may exist near the Fourth is practically imperceptible. All other intervals are free from dissonance. Accordingly 188 IMPORTANCE OF OVERTONES. [VIII. § 92. all intervals from the Minor Third nearly up to the Fifth, and from a little above the Fifth up to the Major Seventh, ought to sound equally smooth. This conclusion, however inconsistent with the views of Musical theorists, who are apt to regard concord and discord as entirely independent of quality, is strictly borne out by experiment. The intervals lying be- tween the Minor and Major Thirds, and between the Minor and Major Sixths, though sounding somewhat strange, are entirely free from roughness, and there- fore cannot be described as dissonant. Helmholtz advises such of his readers as have access to an organ to try the effect of playing alter- nately the smoothest concords, and the most extreme discords, which the Musical scale contains, on stops yielding only approximately simple tones, such e.g. as the flute or stopped diapason. The vivid contrasts which such a proceeding calls out on instruments of bright quality, like the pianoforte and harmonium, or the more brilliant stops of the organ, such as principal, hautbois, trumpet &c., are here blurred and effaced, and everything sounds dull and inanimate in consequence. Nothing can show more decisively than such an experiment that the presence of over- tones confers on Music its most characteristic charms. Thus the remark put into the mouth of a sup- posed objector in § 89 turns out to be no objection VIII. §93.] COMBINATION-TONES OF OVERTONES. 189 whatever to Helmholtz's theory of consonance and dissonance, but, so far as it represents actual facts, to be valid against a view commonly acted on by Musical theorists. 93. A point connected with combination-tones, which might otherwise occur as a difficulty to the reader's mind, shall here be briefly noticed. When two clangs coexist, combination-tones are produced between every pair which can be formed of a partial- tone from one clang with a partial-tone from the other. These intrusive sounds will usually be very numerous, and, for aught that appears, might be thought likely to interfere with those originally present to such an extent as to render useless a theory based on the presence of partial-tones only. Helmholtz has removed any such apprehension by showing generally that dissonance due to combina- tion-tones produced between overtones never exists except where it is already present by virtue of direct action among the overtones themselves. Thus the only effect attributable to this source is a somewhat in- creased roughness in all intervals except absolutely perfect concords. No modifications, therefore, have to be introduced on this score into the conclusions of §§ 81-86. CHAPTER IX. ON CONSONANT TRIADS. 94. In the ensuing portion of this inquiry we shall have to make more frequent use than hitherto of vibration-fractions. It may, therefore, be well to explain at this point the rules for their employment, in order that the student may acquire the requisite facility in handling them. The vibration-fraction of an assigned interval expresses the ratio of the numbers of vibrations performed in the same time by the two notes which form the interval. The particular length of time chosen is a matter of absolute indifference. The upper note of an Octave, for instance, vibrates twice as often as the lower does in any time we choose to select, be it an hour, a minute, a second or a part of a second. In like manner the vibration- fraction ^ indicates that while the lower of two notes forming a Major Third makes four vibrations the higher of them makes five. Therefore while the lower makes one vibration the higher makes |-ths of a vibration. The same reasoning being equally IX. § 95.] USE OF VIBRATION-FRACTIONS. 191 applicable to all other cases, it follows that the vibration-fraction of any interval denotes the number of vibrations and parts of a vibration made by the higher of the two notes which form that interval while the lower of them is making a single vibration. We will next investigate rules for determining the vibration-fractions of the sum and difference of any two intervals whose vibration-fractions are known. 95. Suppose that, starting from a given note, we sound a second note, a Fifth above it, and then a third note, a Major Third above the second. What will be the vibration-fraction of the interval formed by the first and third notes, i.e. of the sum of a Fifth and a Major Third ? We wiU, for shortness, call the three notes (l), (2), (3), in order of ascending pitch. The vibration-fractions being, for (l) — (2), f, and, for (2) — (3), I", we proceed thus : — While (2) makes 4 vibrations, (3) makes 5 vibrations. Therefore, while (2) makes 1 vibration, (3) makes J vibrations. Therefore, while (2) makes 3 vibrations, (3) makes 3x4 vibrations. But while (2) makes 3 vibrations, (1) makes 2 vibrations. Therefore, while (1) makes 2 vibrations, (3) makes 3 x f vibrations. Therefore, while (1) makes 1 vibration, (3) makes |^ x f vibrations. Our result, then, is the two vibration-numbers multiplied together. The reasoning is perfectly general, and gives us the following rule. 192 USE OF VIBRATION-FRACTIONS. [IX § 96. To find the vibration-fraction of the sum of two intervals, multiply their separate vibration-fractions together. 96. Next, take the opposite case. Let (2) be a Major Third above (l), and (3) a Fifth above (l), and let the vibration-fraction for the interval (2) — (3) be required. While (1) makes 4 vibrations, (2) makes 5 vibrations. Therefore, while (1) makes 1 vibration, (2) makes ^ vibrations. But, whUe (1) makes 2 vibrations, (3) makes 3 vibrations. Therefore, while (1) makes 1 vibration, (3) makes | vibrations. Hence, while (2) makes { vibrations, (3) makes | vibrations. Therefore, whUe (2) makes J of a vibration, (3) makes J x f of a vibration. Therefore, while (2) makes 1 vibration, (3) makes | x f vibrations. The result here is the quotient resulting from, the division of the larger vibration-fraction by the smaller : hence we have this general rule : — To find the vibration-fraction of the difi^erence of two intervals, divide the vibration-fraction of the wider by that of the narrower interval. Thus multiplication and division of vibration- fractions correspond to addition and subtraction of intervals^. ' By simply reducing the numerical results, obtained in ^ 95, 96, the student will establish the following propositions : ' A Major Third added to a Fifth gives a Major Seventh.' 'A Major Third subtracted from a Fifth leaves a Minor Third.' IX. §§ 97, 98.] INVERSION OF INTERVALS. 193 97. One of the simplest cases of our second rule occurs when an interval has to be 'inverted.' By the ' inversion ' of any assigned interval narrower than an Octave is meant the difference between it and an Octave, i.e. the interval which remains after it has been subtracted from an Octave. Thus to find the vibration-fraction for the inversion of the Minor Third we merely have to divide 2 by |-, or in other words invert the vibration-fraction of the interval and multi- ply by 2. This applies to all cases. In the particular example selected the result is |- ; the inversion of the Minor Third is therefore the Major Sixth. The relation between an interval and its inversion is obviously mutual, so that each may be described as the inversion of the other. Accordingly the inversion of the Major Sixth is the Minor Third. The following table ^ shows the three pairs of consonant intervals narrower than an Octave which stand to each other in the mutual relation of inver- sions. Minor Third (f)— Major Sixth (|) Major Third (|)— Minor Sixth (f ) Fourth (f)— Fifth (f ) 98. A combination of musical sounds of different ' The last two results of this table will be easily verified by the student. T. 13 194 CONSONANT TRIADS. [IX. § 99. pitch is called a 'chord.' Hitherto we have con- sidered only chords of two notes, or ' binary ' chords. We now go on to chords of three notes, or, as they are usually called, 'triads.' A triad contains three intervals, one between its extreme notes, and one between the middle note and each of the other two. In order that the chord may be free from dissonance, those intervals must all three be concords. 99. We may, then, search for consonant triads in the following manner. Having selected the lowest of the three notes at pleasure, choose two others each of which forms with the bottom note a consonant interval. Next, examine whether the interval formed by these two notes tvith each other is also a concord. If so, the triad itself is consonant. In order to determine all the consonant triads within an Octave above the fixed bottom note, Ave must assign to the middle and top notes every possible consonant position with respect to the bottom note, and reject all such positions as give rise to dissonant intervals between those notes themselves. The remaining positions will constitute all the consonant triads which have for then- lowest note that originally selected. The intervals at our disposal are, for the middle note, from the Minor Third to the Minor Sixth, and, for the upper note, fi:om the Major Third to the Major Sixth. TX. § 99.J CONSONANT TRIADS. 195 In the annexed table ^ the possible positions of the middle note with respect to the bottom note are shown in the left-hand vertical column, the name of each interval being accompanied by its vibration- Major Thii-d Fourth Fifth Minor Sixth Major Sixth 6 4 ■3^ 1 8 6 s Minor Third e T 2S 10 5 T Major Third 4 s Fourth 26 Major Third 5 T 16 e T Minor Third 32 IT 4 Fourth Fourth 9 6 "5 5 4 ^ Minor Third Major Third Fifth 3 16 15- 10 Minor Sixth 8 T 25 24 fraction. The possible positions of the top note are similarly shown in the highest horizontal column. Each space common to a horizontal and a vertical column contains the vibration-fraction of the interval formed between the simultaneous positions of the middle and upper notes named at their extremities. ' Copied with slight modifications from Helmholtz's work. The reader should verify by calculation each statement made in the table. 13—2 196 CONSONANT TRIADS. [IX. §§ 100, 101. When this interval is dissonant its vibration- fraction is enclosed in a square bracket. When it is a concord the name of the interval is, in each case, appended. 100. The following, then, are all the cases ; Middle Note. Upper Note. Minor Third Fifth or Minor Sixth Major Third Fifth or Major Sixth Fourth Minor Sixth or Major Sixth or, in Musical notation. i ^1=5 s ;:;3i They give us two groups of three triads each, which may be arranged thus : — , > jFifth. „. [Minor Sixth. , . [Major Sixth. ^^' JMajor Third. ^ ' \MSnox Third. ^''^ iFourth. , , [Fifth. , -. [Major Sixth. , , [Minor Sixth. ^"^ \Minor Third.^^ iMajor Third. ^'^' iFourth. 101. Instead of defining our six consonant triads, as is here done, by the intervals formed by their middle and top notes with the bottom note, we may define them by the intervals separating the middle from the bottom note, and the top firom the IX. § 101.] CONSONANT TRIADS. 197 middle note. In order to make this change we have, in each case, a process of subtraction of intervals to perform. Thus e.g. the difference between a Fifth and a Major Third is f x f , i.e. f, or a Minor Third. Proceeding in this way, we find that the top and middle notes are separated by the following intervals : (a) (6) («) (a) (« (y) Minor Third Fourth Major Third Major Third Fourth Minor Third Hence we may write our two groups as follows : — , ,.JMinorThird.., /Fourth. ,,. [Major Third. ^" ' iMajor Third. ^ ' IMinor Third. ^^ '^ iFourth. , „ [Major Third. - , J Fourth. ... [Minor Third. ^" ' iMinor Third.^ 'iMajor Third.^^' iFourth. It will now be easy to show that the triads of each group are very closely connected together. Take {a'), and let us form another triad from it by causing its bottom note to ascend one Octave, the other two remaining where they were. The middle will then become the bottom note, the top the middle note, and the Octave of the former bottom note the top note. Hence the lower interval of the new triad will be the upper interval of the old one, i.e. a Minor Third. The upper interval of the new triad 198 INVERSION OF TRIADS. [IX. § 102. will necessarily be the inversion of the interval which separated the extreme notes of the old triad. This interval is a Fifth [see (a), § 100], and its inversion, by the table in § 97, is a Fourth. Hence the new triad is J ' \ which is identical with (&'). \Minor Third./ If we modify (6') in the same way, the new interval is the inversion of the Minor Sixth, i.e. the Major Third, and the resulting triad, viz. ^ ■ I is identical with (c'). This triad, when Fourth. / ^ ' similarly treated, brings us back to [a'), and the cycle of changes is complete. By an extension of the word ' inversion,' it is usual to call the triads (6') and (c') the^rs^ and second inversions of the triad (a'). Exactly similar relations hold between the mem- bers of the second group of triads : (/8') and (y') are accordingly called the first and second inver- sions of the triad (a'). The proof is exactly like that just given, and wUl be easily supplied by the reader. 102. If we choose C as the bottom note of {a') and (a'), these two groups, which are called respec- tively Major and Minor triads, will be expressed in Musical notation by ^ S and ^BtbB («') (»') («') (0-) tf) (7') IX. §§ 103, 104.] INVERSION OF TRIADS. 199 They may also be defined in the language of Thorough Bass, which refers every chord to its lowest note in accordance with the mode adopted in (a), (6), (c) ; (a), (;8), (y). Thus the triads {a'\ {V), (c') would be indicated by the figures |, |, | respectively, and so would the triads (a') (/8') (y') ; the differences between Minor and Major Thirds and Sixths being left to be indicated by the key-signature. The positions [a') and (a') are regarded as the fundamental ones of each group ; [h'), (c') and (/S'), (y') being treated as derived from them by successive inversion. 103. The fundamental triads bear the name of their lowest notes, thus {a!) and (a') are called re- spectively the Major and Minor common chords of C. The remaining members of each group are not named after their own lowest note, but after that which was lowest in the fundamental position of these chords ; thus (&'), (c') and (j8'), (y') are respec- tively Major and Minor common chords of C in their first and second inversions. Common chords of more than three constituent sounds can only be formed by adding to the conso- nant triads notes which are exact Octaves' above or below those of the triads. 104, The marked distinction existing, for every musical ear, between the bright open character of 200 MAJOR AND MINOR EFFECTS. [IX. § 104. Major, and the gloomy veiled effect of Minor chords, is connected by Helmholtz with the different ways in which combination-tones enter in the two cases. The positions of the first-order combination-tones for each of the six consonant triads are shown in crotchets in the appended stave, the primaries being indicated by minims. Each interval gives rise to its i ^^ ^ f (i — ''— St SI- ^5E — I =1 — — I — - j— 1-4- own combination-tone, but, in the cases of the funda- mental position and second inversion of the C Major triad, two combination-tones happen to coincide. The reader will at once notice that in the Major group no note extraneous to the harmony is brought in by the combination-tones. In the Minor group this is no longer the case. The fundamental position and the first inversion of the triad both bi'ing in an A\> which is foreign to the harmony, and the second inversion involves an additional extraneous note, Bj. The position of these adventitious sounds is not such as to produce dissonance, for which they are too far apart from each other and from the notes of the triad ; but they cloud the transparency of the har- mony, and so give rise to effects chai-acteristic of the Minor mode. IX. § 104.] MA JOB AND MINOR EFFECTS. 201 The imperfect nature of Minor compared with Major triads comes out with pecuhar distinctness on the harmonium ; as indeed, from the powerful combi- nation-tones of that instrument (§ 90) was to be anticipated. CHAPTER X. ON PURE INTONATION AND TEMPERAMENT. 105. The vibration- fractions of the intervals formed by the notes of the Major scale with the tonic are, including the Octave of the tonic, these : — 8' 4' 3' 2' 3' 8' ^' The intervals between the successive notes of the scale are determined by dividing each of these fractions by that which precedes it (§96). Thus the consecutive intervals of the Major scale come out as follows : — CDEFGABC a 1 1^ i 1 £. Ig. 8 7 15 B 7 8 T5 Only three different intervals are obtained, -f is slightly wider than ^^ ; \^ decidedly narrower than the other two. f and ^-^ are called whole Tones, \^ a half-Tone or semi-Tone, though, strictly speak- ing, two intervals of this width added together somewhat exceed the greater of the two whole Tones ; since If x ^f or Mf ^^ to f in the ratio of 2048 to 2025. Suppose we had a keyed instrument containing X. § 105.] REQUISITES FOB PURE INTONATION. 203 a number of Octaves, eacli divided into seven notes forming the ordinary scale as above, so that any Music could be played on it not involving notes foreign to the key of C Ma.] or. But now, suppose we wanted to be able to play in another Major key as well as in that of C, for instance in G. It would be necessary for this purpose to introduce two new notes in every Octave of the key-board. If G is the new tonic, A wUl not serve as the Second of its scale, because the step between tonic and Second is not ^ but |-. Hence we must have a fresh note lying between A and B. Further, F wiU not do for the seventh of the scale of G, as it is separated from G by f instead of ^. This necessitates a second additional note lying between i^and G. If we take as our original Octave that from middle C upwards, we have the following vibration-numbers : — CDEFGABC 264 297 330 352 396 440 495 528 The new notes, being respectively -f above and -^ helow G, have for their vibration-numbers -f x 396 and ^x 396, i.e. 445^ and 371^. The other notes of the scale of G Major can be supplied from that of C Major. Hence these two scales are closely con- nected with each other. Another key nearly related to the key of C is that of F. Its Fourth is | x 352, or 469^, which falls between A and B. Its Major 204 REQUISITES FOR PURE INTONATION. [X. § 106. Sixth is f x352, or 586f, which is clearly not the exact Octave of any note between C and C. The corresponding note in that Octave, found by division by 2, is 293^, which comes between C and D. Thus, two more new notes in the Octave must be introduced to make the key of F major attainable. 106. In order that the reader may see at a glance the variety of sounds which are requisite to supply complete Major scales for the seven keys of C, D, E, F, G, A and B, the vibration-numbers for aU the notes of these scales are calculated out and exhibited in the following table. Reducing within the compass of an Octave those notes which lie beyond it, by dividing their vibration-numbers by 2, and arranging the results Tonic Second Major Third Fourth Fifth Major Sixth Major Seventh 0, 264 297 330 352 396 440 495 D, 297 3341 371J 396 445^ 495 5561 E, 330 371^ 412^ 440 496 550 618| F, 352 396 440 469i 528 586| 660 G, 396 4451 495 528 594 660 742| A, 440 495 550 586| 660 733J 825 B, 495 556^ 618f 660 7421 825 928^ X. § 107.] REQUISITES FOB PURE INTONATION. 205 in order of magnitude, we have eleven notes foreign to the scale of G Major, the positions of which, with reference to the notes of that scale, are as follows : — C, 275, 27^^, 293^, D, 309|, E, 334^, F, 366f, 371^, G, 4121 A, 4451 464tL, 469i, B. 107. If it is desired to be able to play in the several Minor modes of each of the seven keys, as well as in the Major, additional notes will be called for. Each scale must contain four notes making with the Tonic the intervals Minor Second, Minor Third, Minor Sixth, and Minor Seventh respectively. The following subsidiary table exhibits the vibration- numbers of the sounds forming these intervals with the successive key-notes. Tonie Minor Second Minor Third Minor Sixth Minor Seventh C, 264 281f 3164 422| 469| D, 297 316| 356| m\ 528 E, 330 352 396 528 586f F, 352 375,1, 422| 563^ 625| G, 396 422f 4751 633| 704 A, 440 m\ 528 704 782f B, 495 528 594 792 880 206 REQ UISITES FOR PURE INTONATION. [X. §§ 108, 109. Reducing these to one Octave, as before, we find eight notes not included in the previous list, occupy- ing the following positions : — C, 281f, D, 312f, 316f, E,F, 356f, 375,^, 391^, G, 422f, A, 475i, B. 108. Hence, to play perfectly in tune in both Major and Minor modes of the seven keys C, D, E, F, G, A, B, it is necessary to have a key-board with twenty-six notes in every Octave. This number, large as it is, by no means includes all necessary notes. Modem music is written in sharp and fiat keys, i. e. in such whose tonics are not coincident with any one of the notes CDE...B. Moreover, the sharp and flat key-notes are dijBferent from each other. Thus Gt, being a Major Third above E, is, as the first table shows, 412-|-; while ^1? is seen, by the second table, to be 422|-, which is a somewhat sharper note. As the seven keys which have been already examined require 26 notes in the Octave, we may anticipate that the ten additional sharp and flat keys will bring in a still larger number. 109. It is, however, needless to institute a de- tailed inquiry into these scales, as the facts already established amply suffice to show how serious are the imperfections of tune which inevitably beset instru- ments of fixed sounds, such as the pianoforte, har- X. § no.] DEFECTS OF KEYED INSTRUMENTS. 207 monium and organ, containing only twelve notes in each Octave. Pure intonation in the 'natural' keys alone, i.e. those whose tonics are white notes on the board, demands, as has been seen, more than twice this number of available sounds ; and many more stUl, if the keys with tonics on black notes are to be included. Perfect tuning in all the keys being entirely out of the question, a compromise of some kind is the only possible course. Thus we may tune a single key, say C, perfectly ; in which case most of the other keys will be so out of tune as to be unbearable. Or again, we may distribute the errors over certain often-used keys, and accu- mulate them in others which are of less frequent occurrence. Expedients of this kind are described as modes of 'tempering,' and the system adopted in tuning any particular instrument is called its 'temperament.' A large number of different methods of tempering have been proposed and tried from very early times in the history of music. 110. That which has at last been almost univer- sally adopted is the system of equal temperament. It consists in dividing each Octave into twelve pre- cisely equal intervals. Each of these intervals is called a semi-Tone, and any two of them together a Tone or whole Tone. 208 'TEMPERING' AND 'TEMPERAMENT: [X. § 111. The Octave of which C, 264, is the lowest note, will contain, on the equal temperament system, the sounds set out below. The vibration-nimibers are given true to the nearest integer. When a note is slightly sharper than that so indicated, this is shown by the sign + attached to the vibration-number in question ; when slightly flatter, by the sign — . For the sake of comparison, the perfect intervals of the same scale are written below the tempered ones. C, Cjt, D, D(, E, P, FJt, G, GJt, A, AJf, B 264, 280- 296+, 314-, 333-, 352+, 373+, 395+, 419+, 444-, 470+, 498+ C, Db, D, Eb, E, F, G, Ab, A, Bb, B 264, 282-. 297, 317-, 330, 352, 396, 422+, 440, 469+, 495 It is clear that the regions of the tempered scale where the tuning is most imperfect are in the neigh- bourhood of the Thirds and Sixths. E and A are nearly three vibrations per second too sharp. The Fourth and Fifth are wrong by only a fraction of a vibration per second. 111. The intervals of the tempered scale are so nearly equal to those of the perfect scale that, when the notes of the former are sounded successively, it requires a delicate ear to recognise the defective character of the tuning. When, however, more notes than one are heard at a time, the case becomes quite different. We saw in Chapter viii. how rigorously ac- curate the tuning of a consonant interval must be to X. §lll.j EQUAL TEMPERAMENT. 209 secure the greatest smoothness of which it is capable. Such intervals were also shown to be generally very closely bounded by harsh discords. Now since, in the system of equal temperament, no interval except that of the Octave is accurately in tune, it follows that every representative of a concord must be less smooth than it would be were the tuning perfect. One of the greatest charms of Music, and especially of modern Music, lies in the vivid contrast presented by consonant and dissonant chords in close juxta- position. Temperament, by impairing, even though but slightly, the perfection of the concords, necessarily somewhat weakens this contrast, and takes the edge off the musical pleasure which, in the hands of a great composer, it is capable of giving us. A fact already once adverted to (§ 85) may be again adduced here, as illustrating the effect of temperament in blurring distinctions of consonance and dissonance, viz. that on the key-board of the pianoforte the same two notes which represent CE'7, which is a concord, also appear in CDt which is a decided discord, and the same thing holds of CA]> and CGt. One of the readiest ways of recognising the de- fective character of equal-temperament tuning is, first to allow a few accurate voices to sing a series of sustained chords in three or four parts, without accompaniment, and then, after noticing the effect, T. 14 210 EFFECT OF TEMPERAMENT. [X. § 112. to let them repeat the phrase while the parts are at the same time played on the pianoforte. The sour character of the concords of the accompanying instrument will be at once decisively manifested. Voices are able to sing perfect intervals, and their clear transparent concords contrast with the duller substitutes provided by the pianoforte in a way obvious to every moderately sensitive ear. 112. Since the voice is endowed with the power of producing all possible shades of pitch within its compass, and thus of singing absolutely pure inter- vals, it is clear that we ought to make the most of this great gift, and, especially in the case of those persons who are to be public singers, allow during the season of preparation contact with the purest examples of intonation only. Unfortunately the practice of many singing-masters is the very reverse of this. The pupil is systematically accompanied, during vocal practice, on the pianoforte, and thus accustomed to habitual familiarity with intervals which are never strictly in tune. No one can doubt the tendency of such constant association to impair the sensitiveness to minute differences of pitch on which delicacy of musical perception depends. Evil communications are not less corrupting to good ears than to good manners. I am convinced that we have here the reason why so comparatively few of our X. §112.] VOCAL INTONATION. 211 trained vocalists, whether amateurs or professionals, are able to sing perfectly in tune. The untutored singing of a child who has never undergone the ear- spoiling process often gives more pleasure by the natural purity of its intonation than the vocalisation of an opera-singer who cannot keep in tune. The remedy is to practise without accompaniment, or with that of an instrument like the violin^, which is not tied down to a few fixed sounds. Even with the pianoforte something might be done by having it, when intended to be used only in assisting vocal practice, put into perfect tune in one single key, and using that key only. The services of such an instrument would, no doubt, be comparatively very restricted, but this might not be without a corresponding advantage, if the vocalist were thereby compelled to rely a little more on his own unaided ear, lay aside his corks and swim out boldly into the ocean of Sound. ' That a violinist can play 'pure intervals was established by Professor Helmholtz by the following decisive experiment, performed with the aid of Herr Joachim. A. harmonium was employed which had been tuned so as to give pure intervals with certain stops and keys ; and tempered intervals with others. A string having been tuned in unison with a common tonic of both systems, it was found that the intervals played by the eminent violinist agreed with those of the mxtural, not with those of the tempered scale. 14—2 212 ESTABLISHED MUSICAL NOTATION. [X. § 113. 113. The Musical notation in ordinary use evi- dently takes for granted a scale consisting of a limited number of fixed sounds. Moreover, it indi- cates directly absolute pitch, and only indirectly relative pitch. In order to ascertain the interval between any two notes on the stave, we must go through a Uttle calculation involving the clef, the key-signature, and perhaps additional ' accidental' sharps or flats. Now these are complications which, if necessary for pianoforte Music S are perfectly gra- tuitous in the case of vocal Music. The vocalist wants only to be told on what note to begin, and what intervals to sing afterwards, i.e. is essentially concerned with absolute pitch only at the start, and need be troubled with it no further. The established notation thus encumbers the vo- calist with information which he does not want, and yet fails to communicate the one special piece of in- formation which he does want, viz. the relation which the note to be sung bears to the tonic or key-note. There is nothing in the established notation to mark clearly and directly what this relation is, in each case, intended to be. Unless the vocaHst, besides his own ' part' is provided with that of the accompaniment, ' In a Paper published in Vol. i. of the Proceedings of the Musical Association (London, Chappell and Co.), I have en- deavoured to prove that there is no such necessity. X. § 113.] ESTABLISHED MUSICAL NOTATION. 213 and possesses some knowledge of Harmony, he can- not ascertain how the notes set down for him are related to the key-note and to each other. The extreme inconvenience of this must have become painfully evident to any one who has frequently sung in concerted Music from a single part. A Bass, we will suppose, after leaving off on Ft, is directed to rest thirteen bars and then come in fortissimo on his high E\>. It is impossible for him to keep the absolute pitch of Fi, in his head during this long interval, which is perhaps occupied by other voices, or instruments, in modulating into some remote key ; and his part vouchsafes no indication in what relation the Ely stands to the notes, or chords, immediately preceding it. There remains, then, no- thing for him to do but to sing, at a venture, some note at the top of his voice, in the hope that it may prove to be Fh, though with considerable dread, in the opposite event, of the conspicuous ignominy entailed by a fortissimo blunder. The essential requisite for a system of vocal notation therefore is that, whenever it specifies any sound, it shall indicate, in a direct and simple manner, the relation in which that sound stands to its tonic for the time being. A method by which this condition is very completely satisfied shaU now be briefly described. 214 TONIC SOL-FA NOTATION. [X. § 114. 114. The old Italian singing-masters denoted the seven notes of the Major scale, reckoned from the key-note upwards, by the syllables do, re, mi, fa, sol, la, si, pronounced, of course, in the continental fashion. So long as a melody moves only in the Major mode, without ' chromatic ' notes or modulation, it clearly admits of being written down, as far as relations of pitch only are concerned, by the use of these syllables. The opening phrase of ' Rule Britannia,' for instance, would stand thus : — sol*, do, do, do, re, mi, fa, sol, do, re, re, mi, fa, mi. In order to abridge the notation, we may indi- cate each syllable by its initial letter. The ambiguity which would thus arise between sol and si is got rid of by altering the latter syllable into ti. In order to distinguish a note from those of the same name in the adjacent Octaves above and below it, an accent is added either above or below the corresponding initial. Thus d' is an Octave above d ; d^ an Octave below d. Where a modulation, i.e. a change of tonic, occurs, it is shown in the following manner. A note necessarily stands in a two-fold relation to the out- going and to the in-coming tonic. The intei-val it * The asterisked sol is to be sung an Octave below that not thus marked. X. § 114.] TO NIG SOL-FA NOTATION. 215 forms with the new tonic is different from that which it formed with the old one. Each of these intervals can be denoted by a suitable syllable-initial, and the displacement of one of these initials by the other represents in the aptest manner the super- session of the old by the new tonic. The old initial is written above and to the left of the new one. Thus ^indicates that the note re is to be sung, but its name changed to fa. As this is a somewhat difficult point, a few instances are appended, ex- pressed both in the established notation and in that now under consideration. The modulations selected are, from C to G; from Cto i''; from E to C; from GtoFt. ^ ^=8= 3= P \> i ^te^ji^ a ^^^s zst Immediately after a modulation the ordinary syllable-initials come into use again, and are employed until a fresh modulation occurs. It will be seen at once that the difficulty of ' remote' keys, which is so formidable in the established notation, here altogether 216 TONIC SOL-FA NOTATION. [X. § 115. disappears. For instance, a vocal phrase from Spohr's ' Last Judgment ' which in the established notation stands as follows : — 4.J?/ l^ ^ iici rft ?^e tpr s q— »g^k=t<- ^^ takes, in the notation before us, the perfectly simple form, s I t \d' mfs\sf I I I s \f m As another example, take the following from the same work : — g^g^^ ^ d^f::^: A. f r f 115. The system of notation of which a cursory sketch has just been given originated with the late Miss S. A. Glover of Norwich, but received its present form at the hands of the late Mr J. Curwen, to whom it also owes the name of ' Tonic- Sol-fa,' by which it is now so widely known. As it is no part of the plan of the present work to go into technical details, only so much has been said about Mr Curwen's system as was necessary to enable the reader to grasp its essential principle. No mention has been made of the notation for Minor and Chro- matic intervals, nor of that for denoting the relations X. § 115.] TONIG SOL-FA NOTATION. 217 of time by measures appealing directly to the eye, instead of by mere symbols. On these and other points connected with his system, Mr Curwen's pub- lished works on Tonic Sol-fa give full and thoroughly lucid explanations, Mr Curwen also created an ex- tensive literature of vocal Music, printed in his own notation, which since his death' has been, and is still being, continually enlarged; gave a most remarkable impulse to choral singing ; and established a sys- tem of graded certificate-examinations guaranteeing the attainment of corresponding stages of Musical cultivation. I have enjoyed some opportunities of watching the progress of beginners taught on the old system, and on that of the Tonic Sol-fa, and assert without the slightest hesitation that, as an instrument for imparting the power of sight-singing, the new system is enormously, overwhelmingly, superior to the old. In fact, I am prepared to maintain that the compli- cated repulsiveness of the pitch-notation in the old system must be held mainly responsible for the humiliating fact that, of the large number of musically well-endowed persons of the opulent classes who have " Mr Curwen died on May 26, 1880. His son, Mr J. Spencer Curwen, is now at the head of the Tonic Sol-fa Association, the publications of which are on sale at 8 and 9 Warwick Lane, London, E.C. 218 TONIC SOL-FA NOTATION. [X. § 115. undergone an elaborate vocal training, comparatively few are able to sing even the very simplest Music at sight. Set a young lady thus instructed to sing a psalm-tune she has never seen before, and we all know what the result is likely to be. Now, there is no greater difficulty in teaching a child with a fairly good ear to sing simple music at sight, than there is in teaching him to read ordinary print at sight. A vocalist who can sing only a few elaborately pre- pared songs ought to be regarded as on a level with a school-boy who should be unable to read except out of his own book. If evidence be wanted to make good this assertion, it is at once to hand in the fact that the youngest children, when well trained on the Tonic Sol-fa system, soon obtain a power of steady and accurate sight-suigiag. The reader is requested to observe that the above remarks are strictly limited to the achievements of the Tonic Sol-fa system in vocal Music. I express no opinion as to the apphcability of its notation to instrumental Music, nor do I wish to maintain that even in the vocal branch it has arrived at perfection. On the contrary, I am doubtful whether its time- notation, when applied to very complicated rhythmic divisions, does not become more difficult than the system in ordinary use, and I consider the notation adopted for the Minor mode to be seriously de- X. § 116.] PHYSICS AND ESTHETICS. 219 fective\ On the main point, however, viz. the decisive superiority of its pitch-notation over that of the established system, and the vitally important consequences as to purity of intonation which neces- sarily follow from this superiority, I desire to express the most confident and uncompromising opinion^ 116. In closing the enquiry which occupies the preceding chapters, it will be advisable to examine very concisely the bearing of our principal result, the theory of consonance and dissonance, on the aesthetics of music. Dissonance was shown to arise from rapid beats, and the concords were classed in order according to their more or less complete free- dom from dissonance ; the Octave coming first, fol- lowed by the Fifth, Fourth, Major Third and Sixth, and Minor Third and Sixth. This classification was strictly physical, depending exclusively on smooth- ness of eflfect. On its own ground, therefore, this classification is absolutely unassailable, and whoever says, for instance, that a Major Third is a smoother concord than a Fifth or Octave, asserts what is as ^ See a pamphlet by me entitled " The Minor Notation of the Tonic Sol-fa system." London, J. Curwen and Sons, 8 and 9 Warwick Lane, E.C. ^ These topics will be found handled somewhat more at large in a Lecture delivered by me at Manchester on Jan. 11, 1883, entitled "The Tonic Sol-fa Movement.'' London, J. Ourwen and Sons, 8 and 9 Warwick Lane, E.G. 220 PHYSICS AND ESTHETICS. [X. § 117. demonstrably false as that the moon goes round the earth in an exact circle. Nevertheless, it by no means necessarily follows that the smoothest con- cords must be the most gratifying to the ear. There may be some other property of an interval which . gives us greater satisfaction than mere consonance. Assuming, for the moment, that such a property does in fact exist, the ear, if called on to arrange the consonant intervals in the order of their pleasantness, might very well bring out a different arrangement from that adopted by physical science on grounds of smoothness alone. Esthetic considerations come in here with the same right to be heard that mechanical considerations possess within their own domain. 117. Now unquestionably the ear's order of merit is not the same as the mechanical order. It places Thirds and Sixths first, then the Fourth and Fifth, and the Octave last of all. The constant recurrence of Thirds and Sixths in two-part Music, compared with the infrequent employment of the remaining concords, leaves no doubt on this point. In fact these intervals have a peculiar charm about them, not possessed by the Fourth or Fifth to any- thing like the same extent, and by the Octave not at all. The thin effect of the Octave undoubtedly de- pends on the fact that every partial-tone of the X. § 117.] PHTSICS AND ESTHETICS. 221 higher of two clangs forming that interval coin- cides exactly with a partial-tone of the lower clang. Thus no new sound is introduced by the higher clang ; the quality of that previously heard is merely modified by alteration of relative intensity among its constituent partial-tones. Major and Minor Thirds bring in a greater variety of pitch in the resulting mass of sound than does the Fifth ; but this can hardly be said of the Major and Minor Sixths compared with the Fourth. On the whole, I am inclined to attribute the predilection of the ear for Thirds and Sixths over the other concords to circumstances connected with its perception of key- relations, though I am not able to give a satisfactory- account of them. The ear enjoys, in alternation with consonant chords, dissonances of so harsh a description as to be barely endurable when sustained by themselves. As passages containing such extreme discords take the following. The chord marked* should in each case be played ./irsf hy itself, and then in the place assigned to it by the composer. The effect of the isolated discord is so intensely harsh, that it is at first difi&cult to understand how any preceding and succeeding concords can make it at aU acceptable ; yet the sequence, in both the phrases cited, is of the rarest beauty. Considerations such as those just alleged tend to 222 CONCLUSION. [X. § 117. show that, while physical science is absolutely au- thoritative in all that relates to the constitution of ^ w: > * J=Jz Last Chorus, BctcWa "Passion" {St. Matthew). m m bpz --^^■ > N \ ^ *- gfcS f^HE =4^^ > ^ ai ^^ =t5=q^ ^:±^i ^^=^^r'Tr=fTf ais^ A j^ , ^ * i tfctrH ^ musical sounds and the smoothness of their combi- nations, the composer's direct perception of what is musically beautiful must mainly direct him in the em- ployment of his materials. It would be a serious error to force upon him a number of rules constructed, on scientific principles, to obtain the maximum smooth- ness of effect ; since mere smoothness is often a matter of extremely secondary importance compared with grandeur of harmony and masterly movement of parts. The nature of the subject may sometimes call for treatment planned to secure exceptional smoothness. In such a case these rules may become of considerable importance. It is well, therefore. X. § 117.] CONCLUSION. 223 that a composer should know and be able to handle them, but he should never allow them to fetter his freedom in wielding the higher and more spiritual weapons of his warfare. CAMBRIDGE: PKTNTED BY J. & C. F. CLAY, AT THE UNIVEKSITY PRESS.