The original of tliis book is in tlie Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924029037468 le Measurement of Attention BY L. R. GEISSLER The Measurement of Attention BY L. R. GEISSLER THBSIS PRESENTED TO THE UNIVERSITY FAC- ULTY OS" CORNELI, UNIVERSITY IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENT FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Reprinted from the American Journal of Psychology October, 1909, Vol. XX, pp. 473-529. THE MEASUREMENT OF ATTENTION^ By I/. R. GbiSSLER CONTENTS I. Previous Views and Methods 473 i. Systematic 474 ii. Experimental 485 a. Peripheral Vision 493 b. Muscular Strength 494 c. Liminal and Differential Sensitivity 495 d. Reaction Time 496 e. Degree of Precision 497 f. Graded Distraction 501 II. Measurement of Attention in Terms of Clearness Values 502 i. Experiments on Motor Inhibition 503 ii. Experiments on Degree of Clearness: Continuous Addition 509 iii. Experiments on Degree of Clearness: Marking Circles 515 III. Summary of Results 529 PART I A Criticai, Study of Previous Vibws and Methods As soon as the experimental method was carried into psy- chology and applied to mental phenomena, the fundamental importance of attention, which until then had not been gen- erally recognized,^ became convincingly manifest. There is, indeed, no field of psychological experimentation in which attention does not play a determining part, or, in Ribot's words, "attention is, in fact, the fundamental psychical condi- 1 Prom the Psychological Laboratory of Cornell University. 2W. James: Princ. of Psychol. I, 1890, 402; Ebbinghaus: Grundz. d. Psychol., 2d ed., 1905, 6io f.; Titchener: I. cit., 79. 21/add: El. of Physiol. Psych., 1889, 541. <^0p. cit., 541. 4 Op. cit., 543- "Op. cit., 544- « Psych. Descr. and Explan., 1894, 74-75. ' Op. cit., 42. 484 GEISSLER tention as essentially characterized by clearness, and thus we find I/add's discussion of attention wavering between two difierent standpoints. According to Baldwin, the whole area of consciousness may be subdivided into five levels or degrees. The lowest is be- yond consciousness; it is the physiological region of the Un- conscious. Then follows the Subconscious, next Passive or Diffused Consciousness, then Active Consciousness or Atten- tion, and finally the level of Apperception.^ In another pas- sage he likens attention to "the line of mental vision," or to "the visual field in which objects are scattered, those being most clearly seen which are in the line of direct vision or cen- tre of the field. Between these limits," he immediately adds, "there are all degrees of distinctness."^ This last sentence would seem to imply that each of the four or five levels must itself include a large number of lesser differences, although perhaps no hard and fast distinction could be drawn between them. As to the nature of these degrees we are left to infer that they are differences of clearness. Such an impli- cation is for instance made in his discussion of the relation of attention to the intellect, where he says: "in general, it may be said that attention increases the vividness of representative states,"' and lower down on the same page: "the capacity to retain mental pictures depends upon the intensity of the origi- nal presentation, and the clearness of its relations; and this intensity and clearness are enhanced by the attention." How- ever, in Baldwin's later writings the clearness factor is en- tirely disregarded, a fact due probably to the dynamogenetic point of view. In this respect he is not followed by J. R. Angell, who otherwise seems to accept Baldwin's view as ex- pressed in his earlier discussion of attention, even reprint- ing the diagram of the five concentric circles which represent the different levels of consciousness.* The latest systematic treatise on attention, that of Pillsbury, again emphasizes the clearness factor. He admits various de- grees of attention, and identifies them with degrees of clear- ness: "increase in the degree to which an impression is con- scious and increase in attention to that impression are synony- mous."* In his sixth chapter he discusses in some detail "the Methods of Measuring Attention" and makes special reference to the experimental work on this problem, which we, too, shall have to consider later on. For the sake of com- 1 Baldwin: Senses and Intellect, 1889, 68. ^Op.cit.,(>^. 'Baldwin: op. cit., 75. * Angell: Psychology, 1904, 65-67. * Pillsbury: Attention, 1908, 2. THB MBASURBMENT OF ATTENTION 485 pleteness we should mention in this place Titchener's latest discussion of our topic, but since it also is intimately con- nected with the experimental work upon attention we reserve it likewise for later treatment. To sum up our review of the theoretical discussions con- cerning the nature and number of degrees of attention, we find that most of the writers, as far as they are explicit on this point, agree that there are an indefinitely large number of such degrees, and that the degrees consist in differences of clear- ness of the conscious processes attended to or distracted from. We are now ready to pass to the experimental work upon different degrees of attention. Here we have, first of all, to distinguish between the general means of inducing variations of attention on the one hand, and the special methods proposed or used for the sake of standardising or accurately measuring these variations on the other hand. Of course, all the inves- tigations here in question had to employ some means of varying the attention; but some of them did not go beyond this, simply because their main problem lay elsewhere. Nevertheless, in our present discussion of the various means of inducing differ- ent degrees of attention, we shall not confine ourselves to this class, but shall include also those of the other class, because this procedure will enable us to reduce unavoidable repetition of references to a minimum. I^ater on, when we come to dis- cuss the special methods of standardising degrees of attention, the investigations of the first kind will naturally have to be omitted. The main question for any one who attempts to vary atten- tion is this : How can an observer be induced (or forced against his will) to give less than maximal attention to a prescribed task? The choice actually made among the numerous pos- sibilities that offer themselves has depended partly upon the nature of the given task, and partly upon the main purpose of the particular investigation. Some of the authors have found it necessary to use various means. Nevertheless, in general, all procedures may be divided, as has been said in the intro- duction, into two great classes, which we may briefly charac- terise as the "single-task" method and the "double- task" method, sometimes called the method of distraction and the method of simultaneous activities. These two methods are intimately connected with the basal fact of consciousness that at any given moment only a limited number of mental processes may be attended to, that is, may rise to focal clearness, while other simultaneously present processes occupy the relatively obscure background. In the single-task method, as the name implies, either the processes constituting the prescribed task 486 GEISSLER are to be attended to, while other processes, constituting what is usually called the distraction, are artificially introduced in the background in order to reduce indirectly by their strength and number the attention given to the focal processes of the task ; or, more frequently (and especially in the later experi- ments), the mental processes of the distraction are made focal while the original task is relegated to the background. In the double-task method, on the other hand, two sets of mental processes are introduced which are intended to occupy simul- taneously the focus of consciousness, while the background processes are not taken into account. Kiilpe was the first to suggest this distinction, when he said : ' ' experience shows that there are but two ways, in the normal waking state, by which this end may be accomplished. We may either distract the attention, or we may divide it".^ In both cases, the variation of attention is inferred from the change in quality or quantity (or both) of the task performed.'' The single-task method is, so far as our knowledge goes, chronologically the older of the two. It seems to have been first employed by Wundt' and Obersteiner*, as early as 1874. Obersteiner, who attempted to measure attention by means of the reaction time, used auditory, cutaneous, and visual dis- tractors. He says: "I placed a musical box which played softly in his vicinity", or "I applied a tolerably strong induc- tion current to the left arm' ' , while in other series the reactor "looked into a kaleidoscope with changing figures".* Ober- steiner assures us that his observer "always endeavored to abstract from the disturbing ifluences and to concentrate his attention on the reaction".' A few years later. Boas, in con- nection with his work on the difference limen, reported experi- ments on the determination of the difference limen of bright- nesses in which ' 'the attentive listening to a piece of music' ' was used as a distraction. Here, then, we notice for the first time iKiilpe: The Problem of Attention; The Monist, XIII, 1903, 44 f. 2 Sometimes the term distraction has been applied to one of the two tasks which had to be attended to simultaneously. Or, the double- task method has been called the method of simultaneous activities. This has resulted in a good deal of confusion, since the single-task method likewise involves simultaneous activities, the difference con- sisting mainly in the direction of the attention. Therefore we have thought it better to employ the new terms "single-task" and "double- task" method as indicating the essential difference between the two, instead of following KUlpe and calling them the methods of distracted and distributed attention. ' Wundt: Grundz. d. physiol. Psychol., ist ed., 1874, 745-749. * Obersteiner : Virchow's Arch. f. pathol. Auat. and Physiol., LIX (N. S. IX), 1874, 427-458. 5 Obersteiner : Brain I, 1879, 447 f . 8 Op cit., 449. THE MEASURBMBNT OF ATTENTION 487 that the observer did not attend to the original task of brightness discrimination, but to the distraction, so that the former was relegated to the periphery or background of consciousness/ Boas' results were known to Stumpf, to whom they perhaps suggested a new method of standardizing the different degrees of attention, which we shall later discuss in detail under the head- ing : method of graded distractors. The first criticism of sen- sory distractors, as unable to reduce indirectly the attention given to a prescribed task, was made by Cattell in 1886, in his investigation of the influence of degrees of attention upon reaction -time. He "let three metronomes beat and ring rapidly"," and he found that "the attention can be more thoroughly distracted if the brain is busied with some other operation while the reactions are being made. A good way to accomplish this is to let the subject beginning with any num- ber add as rapidly as possible 17 after 17 to it".' It remains doubtful even here whether in the few moments preceding and including the reactions the subject's attention was not quickly withdrawn from the addition-task. Miinsterberg has used both the single-task* and the double-task^ method, employing as a rule addition as a distractor. In some of Bliss' experi- ments it was desirable to note the influence of attention upon the task of tapping, and therefore "an effort was made to dis- tract the attention of the person tapping" by weak sounds, but without effect, while on the other hand ' 'the blowing of a loud whistle was followed by a great irregularity", as hap- pened also with "the mental addition of 214 and 23" and the "mental multiplication of 14 by 5".° Swift reports that mus- cular reaction-times were lengthened "while a metronome was ticking one hundred and twenty times each minute".' He also made some experiments "to find how the simple 'muscular' and the 'choice' reactions would vary while the reactor's attention was directed to certain kinds of work. Three tasks were given : i , repeating a poem already committed to memory; 2, reading an English book; and 3, reading Kant's Kritik der reinen Vemunft. . . . The instructions were to fix the atten- tion as closely as possible on the work assigned".^ In the years from 1895 to 1900 there appeared a relatively large number of experimental articles bearing upon our prob- lem. The single-task method was mainly used in the in- 1 Boas : PMger's Archiv, XXVI, 1881, 496. » Cattell : Mind, XI, 1886, 237. » Op. cit., 238. * Miinsterberg: Zeits. f. Psychol., I, l8go, 104. 5 Miinsterberg: Beitrage z. exp. Psychol., IV, 1892, 200. « Bliss: Stud. fr. the Yale Psychol., I